About the Authors:
Samaneh Delshad
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Queensland University of Technology (QUT), Centre for Vision and Eye Research, School of Optometry and Vision Science, Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Kelvin Grove, Queensland, Australia
ORCID logo https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1465-0624
Michael John Collins
Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Validation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Queensland University of Technology (QUT), Centre for Vision and Eye Research, School of Optometry and Vision Science, Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Kelvin Grove, Queensland, Australia
Scott Andrew Read
Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Queensland University of Technology (QUT), Centre for Vision and Eye Research, School of Optometry and Vision Science, Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Kelvin Grove, Queensland, Australia
Stephen James Vincent
Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Queensland University of Technology (QUT), Centre for Vision and Eye Research, School of Optometry and Vision Science, Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Kelvin Grove, Queensland, Australia
ORCID logo https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5998-1320
Introduction
Optical defocus can lead to predictable changes in choroidal thickness and eye growth in various animal species [1–5]. Rapid choroidal thickening in response to imposed myopic defocus [6, 7] precedes a slowing of longer-term eye growth [8–11], while rapid choroidal thinning in response to imposed hyperopic defocus [6, 7] precedes accelerated eye growth [8–11]. These compensatory changes result in the retinal photoreceptors moving closer to the defocused image plane. In humans, the quality of visual experience can also influence ocular growth. In eyes experiencing chronic deprivation of form vision through ocular conditions such as ptosis [12, 13], congenital cataract [14], corneal opacity [15], and vitreous haemorrhage [16], abnormal ocular growth is often observed. Similarly, manipulation of retinal image focus through optical treatment strategies such as bifocal or multifocal spectacles [17, 18], soft multifocal contact lenses [19, 20], or orthokeratology [21–24] have been linked with reduced progression of axial myopia in children. Investigations in human eyes have also shown a bi-directional response to short-term imposed continuous myopic and hyperopic defocus in children [25] and adults [26–31], with a small magnitude axial length reduction associated with rapid choroidal thickening in response to myopic defocus and axial elongation associated with rapid choroidal thinning in response to hyperopic defocus.
In the absence of ocular pathology leading to a disruption in form vision, the visual control of eye growth could be associated with the dynamics of visual experience and the type and magnitude of defocus to which the eyes are exposed on a daily basis. Natural visual scenes typically comprise objects at varying distances that produce myopic and hyperopic defocus, and well-focused retinal images, depending on where the eyes are fixating and focused within the environment [32–34]. The temporal integration of these defocus signals may provide input to the regulation of eye growth [6, 32, 33, 35]. Studies using various animal models have shown that over a period of days or weeks, the eyes use a complex method for integrating defocus signals over time rather than a simple linear summation of the blur that it experiences [6, 35–44]. These studies suggest that the temporal integration of defocus signals is dependent upon the sign and power of the defocus experienced [6, 36–43], and the frequency and duration of individual episodes of blur [37, 44].
In humans, while studies have shown that the eye is responsive to short-term continuous myopic and hyperopic defocus by changing its axial length and choroidal thickness [25–31], it is not known how the eye responds to more complex temporally varying patterns of defocus, as might be encountered in real-world visual scenes. In this study, we tested the hypothesis that the short-term response of axial length and choroidal thickness to alternating episodes of myopic and hyperopic defocus in the human eye will reflect a simple temporal summation of the blur signals.
Materials and methods
Sixteen young adults (11 females, 5 males) aged between 19 and 34 years (mean ± SD, 24.8 ± 4.1 years) were recruited for the study. This sample size provided 80% power to detect an intrasession axial length change of 10 μm at the 5% level, assuming a standard deviation of 10 μm [26]. All subjects were free from any systemic or ocular diseases and had no prior history of eye injury or surgery. Before the study, each subject underwent an initial screening to ensure good ocular health and normal binocular vision, and to determine their refractive status. The spherical equivalent refraction across subjects ranged between +0.50 DS and -2.75 DS with a mean ± SD of -0.57 ± 1.12 DS. All subjects had anisometropia of less than 0.50 DS, astigmatism of ≤ -0.75 DC and exhibited visual acuity of at least 0.00 logMAR (20/20 Snellen acuity) in both eyes. No contact lens wearers were included in the study. Ethics approval was obtained from the Queensland University of Technology human research ethics committee prior to commencement of the study. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects, who were treated in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
This study involved the measurement of axial length before, during and after six different defocus conditions. Subfoveal choroidal thickness was also measured before and after exposure to defocus. Each defocus condition was conducted on a separate day in order to allow any potential effects to dissipate from the prior defocus session (a minimum 24-hour washout period between the defocus conditions). The order of the defocus conditions was randomized between subjects to ensure no order-related bias in the data. To minimize the potential for a confounding interaction between the diurnal variations in axial length and subfoveal choroidal thickness [45, 46] and the eye’s response to defocus [47], all measurement sessions were conducted at a similar time of day (between 8:00 am and 2:00 pm with an average daily time difference of 62 ± 23 minutes between sessions within each subject), and at least two hours after the subjects’ reported time of waking. None of the subjects enrolled in this study were taking any medications which could influence their ocular biometry measurements. Since smoking may affect the measurement of choroidal thickness, smokers were also not included in this study [48]. Due to the influence of caffeine intake and dynamic exercise on ocular biometry, all participants were asked to abstain from consumption of caffeine or performing vigorous exercise for at least 1 hour prior to the start of each measurement session [49, 50]. Since prior visual tasks (e.g. intense near-work) could potentially affect measurements of axial length and choroidal thickness, each subject completed a 20 minute “washout period” during which they watched a movie of their choosing at a 6 m distance with their optimal distance refractive correction before each measurement session. Following completion of the “washout period”, baseline measurements of axial length and subfoveal choroidal thickness were obtained from the right eye, and then, a 60 minute monocular “defocus period” was conducted.
During the “defocus period”, each subjects’ right eye was exposed to six different defocus conditions (monocular defocus over the right eye’s optimal distance refraction), while watching movies on a TV at 6 m, with the left eye optimally corrected for distance viewing to maintain relaxed accommodation. This experimental paradigm has been used previously and allows control of the accommodation response with the non-tested (left) eye, while simultaneously producing defocus conditions in the tested (right) eye [25–30]. Over six separate sessions, the right eye was exposed to 60 minute episodes of either continuous or alternating myopic and hyperopic defocus (+3 DS and -3 DS). During the alternating defocus conditions, the eye was exposed to either 30 minutes or 15 minutes of alternating cycles of myopic and hyperopic defocus, with the defocus order reversed in separate sessions (Fig 1).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 1. Illustration of the type of defocus imposed on the right eye for each measurement day.
On six separate measurement days, the right eye was exposed to one of the following conditions (in a randomized order): (A) continuous hyperopic (-3 D) defocus (1 x 60 minutes), (B) continuous myopic (+3 D) defocus (1 x 60 minutes), (C) alternating low frequency cycles of hyperopic then myopic defocus (2 x 30 minutes), (D) alternating low frequency cycles of myopic then hyperopic defocus (2 x 30 minutes), (E) alternating high frequency cycles of hyperopic then myopic defocus (4 x 15 minutes), and (F) alternating high frequency cycles of myopic then hyperopic defocus (4 x 15 minutes).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243076.g001
Five repeated measures of axial length (measured from the anterior corneal surface to the retinal pigment epithelium [RPE]) were obtained at baseline, and then every 15 minutes during both continuous and alternating defocus conditions, using the Lenstar optical biometer (LS 900, Haag Streit AG, Koeniz, Switzerland). This is a non-contact biometry device that works on the principle of optical low-coherence reflectometry (OLCR), using a broad-band light source (range of 20–30 nm), with a central wavelength of 820 nm. The Lenstar optical biometer has a display resolution of 10 μm. To improve the accuracy of the measurements of axial length, only five consecutive measurements with a cumulative standard deviation (as displayed on the Lenstar) of ≤ 7 μm were included. If any of the five consecutive measurements yielded a cumulative SD of > 7 μm, that measurement was deleted and then immediately repeated with a new measurement. To control for accommodation and to provide continuous exposure to defocus during biometry, a binocular beam splitter periscope system was used to allow fixation of an external target (a high contrast Maltese cross) at a 6 m distance during measurement acquisition. In order to provide the periscopic view of the Maltese cross, the system was adjusted and once the centre of the Maltese cross was aligned with the internal fixation target of the biometer (the red fixation target of the Lenstar), the subject was asked to fixate the centre of the target at a 6 m distance. When using the periscope system, the subject’s sphero-cylinder distance refraction was mounted in a trial frame in front of each eye and the additional defocus lens was positioned in front of the right eye. A schematic diagram of this experimental set-up is shown in Fig 2.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 2. Schematic representation of the experimental set-up for axial length measures.
A binocular periscope system was used to allow fixation of the distant target (Maltese cross) while the Lenstar biometer measured axial length.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243076.g002
To assess the intrasession repeatability of axial length measurements, the within-session standard deviation (SD) and within-session range of five consecutive measurements of axial length across all subjects and all defocus conditions were calculated [51]. The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) was also calculated. The within-session SD was 5 μm, with a within-session range of 11 μm, and ICC of 0.999, indicating highly precise measurements of axial length.
The subfoveal choroidal thickness of the right eye (defocused eye) was also measured using spectral domain optical coherence tomography (SD-OCT) (Copernicus SOCT-HR; Optopol Technology SA, Zawiercie, Poland). This device provides high resolution, cross-sectional images of the posterior eye, using a peak wavelength of 850 nm and has axial and transverse resolutions of 3 μm, and 5 μm (in tissue), respectively. The measurements of choroidal thickness were obtained at baseline, and then following removal of the defocus lens at the end of the 60 minute defocus period (immediately following the final measurement of axial length). The OCT images were obtained with a 5 mm horizontal foveal line scan, consisting of 40 B-scans, each with 1500 A-scans, with an acquisition time of 1.52 seconds.
Following data collection, the five repeated measures of axial length at each measurement time point were averaged for each defocus condition, and across subjects. For the measurements of subfoveal choroidal thickness, the raw OCT images were extracted from the instrument and then analysed using custom written software [52]. For each OCT image, the software aligned and averaged the 40 individual B-scans, to generate a high-quality average B-scan image with reduced speckle noise and increased visibility of the posterior segment structures. The resolution of the final OCT images was 2.26 microns per pixel. Each averaged OCT image for each subject at each measurement time point was then manually segmented by an experienced observer who was masked to the time of the measurement and type of defocus condition, for all of the scans.
To provide an assessment of the repeatability and reliability of the subfoveal choroidal thickness segmentation, the masked observer manually segmented the baseline subfoveal choroidal thickness of all subjects for two randomly selected defocus conditions, twice. The coefficient of repeatability and 95% confidence interval of the coefficient of repeatability (derived from both defocus conditions) [51], were 6 μm and 3–9 μm, respectively. A paired sample t-test revealed no significant difference between the two measurements (p > 0.05). Bland-Altman analysis [53] revealed excellent agreement between the two measures of the subfoveal choroidal thickness, with a negligible mean difference of -0.5 μm and 95% limits of agreement of -9 to +7 μm. The intraobserver reliability was assessed using the ICC (two-way mixed model, absolute agreement) and was excellent at 0.999, with a 95% confidence interval of 0.998–1.00.
The Shapiro-Wilk test of normality revealed that the axial length and subfoveal choroidal thickness data did not significantly depart from a normal distribution (p > 0.05). In order to assess the effects of defocus condition for each variable, the data were analysed using a repeated measures ANOVA with two within-subjects factors (time and type of defocus). Following the repeated measures ANOVA, for any variables with significant main effects and interactions (p < 0.05), pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni corrections were conducted. To assess the effects of defocus order and defocus frequency on each variable, a multifactorial linear mixed model analysis was used.
Results
Axial length
Repeated measures ANOVA revealed a highly significant interaction between the type of defocus and time of measurement for axial length measures across the six defocus conditions (F (20,300) = 4.60, p < 0.0001).
Continuous defocus.
Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni corrections revealed that 15 minutes after the introduction of hyperopic defocus (Condition A), there was a significant axial elongation of +5 ± 6 μm from baseline (p = 0.038). Following this initial response, the axial length remained relatively stable until the end of the defocus period and was significantly different from baseline at all subsequent measurement times (all p < 0.05). The maximum axial elongation occurred at 45 minutes with the eye being +7 ± 7 μm longer than the baseline measurement (p = 0.010). The axial length at the end of 60 minutes was +7 ± 7 μm longer than the baseline measurement (p = 0.027) (Table 1) (Fig 3). The introduction of myopic defocus (Condition B) produced a significant axial length reduction of -4 ± 5 μm from baseline after 15 minutes (p = 0.040). The axial length then continued to shorten gradually over the next 45 minutes, reaching a significant maximum axial length reduction of -8 ± 10 μm at the end of the 60 minutes (p = 0.046) (Table 1) (Fig 3).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 3. The mean change in axial length from baseline (0 minute time point), during 60 minutes of continuous hyperopic defocus (Condition A) and continuous myopic defocus (Condition B) for all subjects.
Vertical error bars represent the standard error of the mean difference in axial length, and horizontal error bars represent the standard error in measurement time. Asterisks indicate a significant mean difference from baseline axial length (p < 0.05).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243076.g003
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Mean change in axial length from baseline across different measurement times for continuous and alternating defocus conditions in all subjects.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243076.t001
Alternating defocus (30 minute cycles).
When the right eye was exposed to 30 minute alternating cycles of hyperopic then myopic defocus (Condition C), after the first 30 minutes of hyperopic defocus, the axial length was significantly longer than baseline by +7 ± 9 μm (p = 0.045). The introduction of myopic defocus then gradually cancelled the previous axial elongation effects of hyperopic defocus and over the next 30 minutes the eye approached the baseline axial length level (mean difference of 0 ± 11 μm from baseline at the end of the 60 minutes, p > 0.05) (Table 1) (Fig 4). A similar cancellation effect of opposing blur was observed when the right eye was exposed to 30 minute alternating cycles of myopic then hyperopic defocus (Condition D). After the first 30 minutes of myopic defocus, the axial length was shorter than the baseline by -6 ± 9 μm (p > 0.05). This reduction in axial length then gradually cancelled during the next 30 minutes of hyperopic defocus of equal power, and by the end of 60 minutes, the eye was almost at the baseline level (mean difference of -2 ± 12 μm, p > 0.05) (Table 1) (Fig 4).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 4. The mean change in axial length from baseline (0 minute time point) during 30 minute alternating cycles of hyperopic then myopic defocus (Condition C), and myopic then hyperopic defocus (Condition D) for all subjects.
Vertical error bars represent the standard error of the mean difference in axial length, and horizontal error bars represent the standard error in measurement time. Asterisks indicate a significant mean difference from the baseline axial length (p < 0.05).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243076.g004
Alternating defocus (15 minute cycles).
When the eye was exposed to 15 minute alternating cycles of hyperopic and myopic defocus, a cancellation effect of opposing blur sessions did not occur as the axial length was observed to shorten slowly over time. During 60 minute alternating cycles of hyperopic then myopic defocus (Condition E), after the first 15 minutes of uninterrupted hyperopic defocus, axial length changed by +5 ± 8 μm from baseline (p > 0.05). This increase in axial length was then cancelled during the next 15 minutes of uninterrupted myopic defocus, as the eye returned to below baseline level at 30 minutes. From 30 to 60 minutes, axial length continued to shorten slowly and reached a -4 ± 7 μm reduction from baseline following the final defocus session (p > 0.05) (Table 1) (Fig 5). A similar pattern of response was also observed for axial length during 15 minute alternating cycles of myopic then hyperopic defocus (Condition F), as it shortened gradually over time. Following completion of the two cycles of myopic then hyperopic defocus, the axial length was -3 ± 8 μm less than the baseline measurement at 60 minutes (p > 0.05) (Table 1) (Fig 5).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 5. The mean change in axial length from baseline (0 minute time point) during 15 minute alternating cycles of hyperopic then myopic defocus (Condition E), and myopic then hyperopic defocus (Condition F) for all subjects.
Vertical error bars represent the standard error of the mean difference in axial length, and horizontal error bars represent the standard error in measurement time. Asterisks indicate a significant mean difference from the baseline axial length (p < 0.05).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243076.g005
Effects of defocus order and defocus frequency.
Assessment of the effect of defocus order revealed that regardless of the frequency of the cycles, there was no significant difference in the final axial length response to defocus observed when the eye was exposed to myopic then hyperopic defocus, or hyperopic then myopic defocus (p > 0.05). Similarly, there was no significant difference in the final axial length response between the 15 minute and 30 minute cycling frequencies for each order of defocus, indicating no significant effects of defocus frequency (p > 0.05).
Axial length changes: Continuous vs alternating defocus
After the first 30 minutes of uninterrupted hyperopic defocus, baseline axial length increased by a similar magnitude during both the continuous hyperopic defocus (Condition A) and the alternating hyperopic then myopic defocus (Condition C) conditions (+6 ± 4 μm vs +7 ± 9 μm, p > 0.05, Figs 3 and 4 respectively). From 30 minutes to 60 minutes, axial length remained stable during continuous hyperopic defocus (+1 ± 6 μm increase, p > 0.05, Fig 3), while it reduced significantly by -7 ± 10 μm during alternating hyperopic then myopic defocus (p = 0.022, Fig 4). The magnitude of change in axial length from 30 minutes to 60 minutes between the continuous hyperopic defocus and the alternating hyperopic then myopic defocus conditions was significantly different (p = 0.033).
After the first 30 minutes of uninterrupted myopic defocus, axial length decreased by -6 ± 7 μm during continuous myopic defocus (Condition B) and by -6 ± 9 μm during the alternating myopic then hyperopic defocus condition (Condition D) (p > 0.05, Figs 3 and 4). From 30 minutes to 60 minutes, axial length changed by -2 ± 6 μm during continuous myopic defocus (p > 0.05, Fig 3) and by +4 ± 12 μm during the alternating myopic then hyperopic defocus condition (p > 0.05, Fig 4). There was a significant difference in the magnitude of change in axial length from 30 minutes to 60 minutes between the continuous myopic defocus and the alternating myopic then hyperopic defocus conditions (p = 0.044).
After the first 15 minutes of uninterrupted hyperopic defocus, baseline axial length increased significantly and by a similar magnitude during both the continuous hyperopic defocus (Condition A) and the alternating hyperopic then myopic defocus (Condition E) conditions (+5 ± 6 μm vs +5 ± 8 μm, p > 0.05, Figs 3 and 5 respectively). From 15 minutes to 60 minutes, axial length remained relatively stable during continuous hyperopic defocus (mean change of +2 ± 7 μm p > 0.05, Fig 3) while it shortened significantly by -9 ± 8 μm (p < 0.0001, Fig 5) during the alternating hyperopic then myopic defocus condition.
After the first 15 minutes of uninterrupted myopic defocus, axial length changed by -4 ± 5 μm during continuous myopic defocus (Condition B) and by -3 ± 6 μm during alternating myopic then hyperopic defocus condition (Condition F) (p > 0.05, Figs 3 and 5). From 15 minutes to 60 minutes, axial length changed by -4 ± 8 μm during continuous myopic defocus (p = 0.05, Fig 3) while it remained stable during alternating myopic then hyperopic defocus condition (+1 ± 9 μm, p > 0.05, Fig 5).
Subfoveal choroidal thickness
Imposing defocus resulted in some significant changes in subfoveal choroid thickness over time. At 60 minutes, the subfoveal choroid was -4 ± 11 μm thinner than baseline after continuous hyperopic defocus (Condition A) (p > 0.05), while it was significantly thicker by +8 ± 11 μm following continuous myopic defocus (Condition B) (p = 0.030) (Table 2). When exposed to 30 minute or 15 minute alternating cycles of defocus, subfoveal choroidal thickness did not differ significantly from baseline after hyperopic then myopic defocus (Condition C & Condition E), or after myopic then hyperopic defocus (Condition D & Condition F) (p > 0.05) (Table 2). There were no significant effects of defocus order and defocus frequency for any of the analyses of the subfoveal choroidal thickness measures (all p > 0.05).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. Mean change in subfoveal choroidal thickness from baseline across different defocus conditions in all subjects.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243076.t002
Discussion
This study has shown for the first time that the human eye is capable of temporal averaging of visual cues from alternating myopic and hyperopic defocus. Whilst exposure to continuous defocus led to significant bi-directional changes in axial length, the change in axial length was minimal when the eye was exposed to alternating periods of myopic and hyperopic defocus. During a 30 minute cycling frequency, alternating episodes of defocus of opposite power largely cancelled each other, and the eye remained at near baseline levels after 60 minutes. During a 15 minute cycling frequency, the eye’s response to myopic defocus appeared to be greater than the response to hyperopic defocus, as the eye was slightly shorter than the baseline measurement at 60 minutes, however this change was not statistically significant. Similar but smaller magnitude changes were also observed for subfoveal choroidal thickness.
The findings from the continuous defocus conditions are consistent with those from previous studies of human [26, 29–31] and other animal eyes [6, 7, 36] where small but significant changes in axial length and choroidal thickness have been reported after short term exposure to continuous myopic and hyperopic defocus. A recent investigation in human eyes has shown a significant change in axial length, approximately 2 minutes after exposure to imposed defocus [26]. Similarly, significant bi-directional changes in subfoveal choroidal thickness after 10 to 35 minutes have been reported [30]. An investigation in school children found no significant change in axial length after 60 minutes exposure to defocus [25], however, as the measurements were taken following the instillation of 1% cyclopentolate (an antimuscarinic agent that is known to affect the thickness of the choroid and axial length) [54, 55], these findings may have been influenced by the effects of the drug. Further, in a recent investigation, a 30 minute exposure to myopic defocus with full field and multifocal contact lenses yielded no significant change in choroidal thickness [56]. This difference in outcomes may be due to the shorter duration of exposure to defocus or to the influence of the contact lens correction. It is evident from our findings during the continuous defocus conditions that the human eye is able to respond to the sign of blur rapidly (within 15 minutes) and make distinct bi-directional changes in its axial length. Although the underlying mechanisms of this response are not fully understood; a variety of potential mechanisms such as the role of contrast cues from contrast adaptation (changes in contrast sensitivity at different spatial frequencies) [57–60], colour cues from chromatic aberration [61–64], or optical vergence cues from image defocus [65] have all been suggested as potential ways in which the human eye decodes the sign of blur.
The maximum mean axial length change observed during continuous hyperopic defocus was +7 ± 7 μm of elongation and during continuous myopic defocus was a -8 ± 10 μm reduction. These findings correspond closely with published data in human eyes where changes of similar magnitude in axial length following continuous short term exposure to myopic and hyperopic defocus have been reported [26, 29]. However, when the pattern of defocus was alternated at a 30 minute frequency, the eye underwent a significantly different temporal pattern of change than it did during continuous defocus. The axial length response to each type of defocus after 30 minutes was almost cancelled during the succeeding 30 minute exposure to defocus of opposite power, and the axial length was near baseline levels after 60 minutes. This finding occurred irrespective of the order of defocus, indicating that the cancelling effects of myopic and hyperopic defocus for this particular duration and alternating frequency were of similar strength.
When the frequency of alternating defocus cycles was 15 minutes, the myopic and hyperopic defocus did not appear to completely cancel the preceding effects of each other; rather, a slight non-significant trend towards a more dominant response to myopic defocus was observed, and axial length was observed to reduce slightly. In animal models of blur integration, a greater potency of myopic defocus in arresting the ocular elongation effect of hyperopic defocus has been reported. For instance, when myopic and hyperopic defocus were presented sequentially or simultaneously in chicks [6, 36–39], tree shrews [40, 41], monkeys [42], and marmosets [43], the eye responded preferentially to myopic defocus and developed less myopia/more hyperopia. Even when episodes of myopic and hyperopic defocus had equal duration when imposed alternatively over the chick eye, the axial growth still reduced, suggesting that myopic defocus provides a stronger growth signal [37]. Since the slight bias towards the dominating effects of myopic defocus observed in our study was not statistically significant, we cannot conclude if such properties also exist in the human eye. Future investigations involving longer durations of exposure to defocus or utilizing different magnitudes of defocus may provide additional insights into the relative influence of myopic versus hyperopic blur.
Defocus-mediated changes in axial length are expected to occur through modulations in the thickness of the choroid posterior to the retina, and thus affects the measurements of the axial length to the overlying RPE [26–30]. We found the bi-directional changes in the thickness of the subfoveal choroid to be consistent with the direction of the observed changes in axial length at the end of each defocus condition. In general, subfoveal choroidal thickening occurred along with axial length reduction, and subfoveal choroidal thinning occurred along with axial elongation. However, for the continuous defocus conditions, subfoveal choroidal thickening accounted for 87% of the observed mean axial length reduction during myopic defocus, and only accounted for 57% of the observed mean axial elongation during hyperopic defocus. A similar pattern of response was also observed for axial length and subfoveal choroidal thickness during alternating defocus. The discrepancies in the magnitude of axial length and choroidal thickness changes could have arisen in two possible ways. First, the final measurement of the subfoveal choroidal thickness was obtained approximately one minute after the removal of the defocus lens, therefore, during this brief period of clear viewing during the OCT imaging process, some decay in the effects of defocus could have occurred. Alternately, other factors such as expansion or contraction of the sclera might have also contributed to the observed axial length elongation and reduction with defocus. Since current imaging technology does not allow visualization of the thickness of the posterior sclera in most eyes, future studies to understand the potential role of the sclera in mediating the human eye’s response to short-term defocus seem warranted.
The changes in choroidal thickness with defocus (thickening or thinning) were found to range between 4 to 8 μm across the different defocus conditions investigated in this study. Whilst this amount of change is above the axial resolution of the OCT device (3 μm in tissue), it is within the range of the coefficient of repeatability and its 95% confidence interval for the measurements of choroidal thickness in this study (6 μm and 3–9 μm, respectively). Therefore, this limitation should be noted when considering the choroidal thickness changes observed in this study.
Previous studies suggest two potential models in which defocus signals could be integrated over time; a simple linear model, and a more complex non-linear model [6, 35, 37, 66, 67]. Based on a simple linear model, the effects of equal powers of defocus of opposite sign would be added linearly so that the resulting compensation would be relative to the average exposure of defocus that the retina experienced, summed over a period of time (e.g. +3 D for 30 minutes followed by -3 D for 30 minutes = 0 change). However, based on a non-linear model, more complex outcomes of blur integration are expected where the final compensation could be multifactorial, depending on the sign and power of the defocus experienced (myopic or hyperopic blur), and the frequency and duration of individual episodes of blur. Whilst our findings from the alternating defocus conditions suggest a simple summation of the effects of defocus of opposing sign over time, the findings from our continuous defocus conditions do not reflect a simple linear model. We observed that the rate of axial elongation and axial length reduction resulting from exposure to equal amounts of hyperopic and myopic defocus was not linearly proportional to the time exposed to blur. During continuous defocus exposure, almost 70% of the final axial elongation observed in response to hyperopic defocus (+7 ± 7 μm) occurred after 15 minutes of exposure to defocus, and 50% of the final axial length reduction (-8 ± 10 μm) in response to myopic defocus occurred after 15 minutes of exposure to defocus, indicating that the time course of the eye’s response to blur may be non-linear and may vary depending on the sign of defocus. Whilst experiments with animals have shown characteristics which strongly suggest a complex, non-linear model of temporal blur integration [6, 36–44] the mixed findings from our investigations do not allow us to confidently propose the model of temporal blur integration in human eyes. Further studies in this field seem warranted.
It has been proposed that the transient exposure to hyperopic defocus associated with near activities (e.g. due to lag of accommodation) [68–71], ocular aberrations [72, 73] or peripheral defocus [74] might predispose the eye to myopia. We found that exposure to myopic defocus is able to quickly counterbalance the axial elongation and subfoveal choroidal thinning effects of hyperopic defocus. If short-term ocular changes in response to defocus are associated with longer term refractive error development in the human eye, then optical methods of introducing myopic blur for at least a similar duration to that of exposure to hyperopic blur, may counteract the myopigenic stimulus. However, it must be noted that only one level of myopic and hyperopic defocus and limited alternating defocus frequencies were tested in this study.
The inter-subject variability in the axial length and choroidal thickness response to blur observed in this study could have been due to individual differences in retinal sensitivity to blur (e.g. an equal level of defocus degrades visual acuity by varying amounts between individuals). Also, while we attempted to control many of the known factors that influence choroidal thickness and axial length (diurnal rhythms, near work, medications, smoking, caffeine intake, and dynamic exercise) there are many other systemic factors that could potentially vary from day to day and affect the highly vascularized and sympathetically innervated choroid.
In conclusion we have shown for the first time that the human eye is capable of temporal integration of myopic and hyperopic defocus signals. Over 60 minutes of blur exposure, this integration was effectively a simple summation (cancellation) of the effects of the preceding opposite sign blur condition.
Supporting information
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
S1 Dataset. Ocular biometry data for each of the defocus conditions for all subjects enrolled in the study.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243076.s001
(XLSX)
Citation: Delshad S, Collins MJ, Read SA, Vincent SJ (2020) The human axial length and choroidal thickness responses to continuous and alternating episodes of myopic and hyperopic blur. PLoS ONE 15(12): e0243076. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243076
1. Irving EL, Sivak JG, Callender MG. Refractive plasticity of the developing chick eye. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1992; 12: 448–456. pmid:1293533
2. Graham B, Judge SJ. The effects of spectacle wear in infancy on eye growth and refractive error in the marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Vision Res. 1999; 39: 189–206. pmid:10326130
3. Smith EL, Hung LF. The role of optical defocus in regulating refractive development in infant monkeys. Vision Res. 1999; 39: 1415–1435. pmid:10343811
4. Whatham AR, Judge SJ. Compensatory changes in eye growth and refraction induced by daily wear of soft contact lenses in young marmosets. Vision Res. 2001; 41: 267–273. pmid:11164443
5. Hung LF, Crawford ML, Smith EL. Spectacle lenses alter eye growth and the refractive status of young monkeys. Nat Med. 1995; 1: 761–765. pmid:7585177
6. Zhu X, Wallman J. Temporal properties of compensation for positive and negative spectacle lenses in chicks. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009; 50: 37–46. pmid:18791175
7. Zhu X, Park TW, Winawer J, Wallman J. In a matter of minutes, the eye can know which way to grow. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005; 46: 2238–2241. pmid:15980206
8. Rada JA, Nickla DL, Troilo D. Decreased proteoglycan synthesis associated with form deprivation myopia in mature primate eyes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000; 41: 2050–2058. pmid:10892842
9. Guggenheim JA, McBrien NA. Form-deprivation myopia induces activation of scleral matrix metalloproteinase-2 in tree shrew. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1996; 37: 1380–1395. pmid:8641841
10. Summers JA. The choroid as a sclera growth regulator. Exp Eye Res. 2013; 114: 120–127. pmid:23528534
11. Wildsoet C, Wallman J. Choroidal and scleral mechanisms of compensation for spectacle lenses in chicks. Vision Res. 1995; 35: 1175–1194. pmid:7610579
12. O’Leary D, Millodot M. Eyelid closure causes myopia in humans. Experientia. 1979; 35: 1478–1479. pmid:510490
13. Hoyt CS, Stone RD, Fromer C, Billson FA. Monocular axial myopia associated with neonatal eyelid closure in human infants. Am J Ophthalmol. 1981; 91: 197–200. pmid:7468734
14. Von Noorden GK, Lewis RA. Ocular axial length in unilateral congenital cataracts and blepharoptosis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1987; 28: 750–752. pmid:3557880
15. Twomey JM, Gilvarry A, Restori M, Kirkness CM, Moore AT, Holden AL. Ocular enlargement following infantile corneal opacification. Eye. 1990; 4: 497–503. pmid:2209916
16. Miller-Meeks MJ, Bennett SR, Keech RV, Blodi CF. Myopia induced by vitreous hemorrhage. Am J Ophthalmol. 1990; 109: 199–203. pmid:2301532
17. Lam CSY, Tang WC, Tse DYY, Lee RPK, Chun RMK, Hasegawa K, et al. Defocus Incorporated Multiple Segments (DIMS) spectacle lenses slow myopia progression: a 2-year randomised clinical trial. Br J Ophthalmol. 2020; 104: 363–368. pmid:31142465
18. Chamberlain P, González-Méijome JM, Logan NS, Ngo C, Jones D, Young A. A three-year randomized clinical trial of MiSight lenses for myopia control. Optom Vis Sci. 2019; 96: 556–567. pmid:31343513
19. Lam CSY, Tang WC, Tse DYY, Lee RPK, Chun RMK, Hasegawa K, et al. Defocus incorporated soft contact (DISC) lens slows myopia progression in Hong Kong Chinese schoolchildren: a 2-year randomised clinical trial. Br J Ophthalmol. 2014; 98: 40–45. pmid:24169657
20. Anstice NS, Phillips JR. Effect of dual-focus soft contact lens wear on axial myopia progression in children. Ophthalmology. 2011; 118: 1152–1161. pmid:21276616
21. Cho P, Cheung SW. Retardation of myopia in Orthokeratology study: a 2-year randomized clinical trial. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53: 7077–7085. pmid:22969068
22. Santodomingo-Rubido J, Villa-Collar C, Gilmartin B, Gutierrez-Ortega R. Myopia control with orthokeratology contact lenses in Spain: refractive and biometric changes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53: 5060–5065. pmid:22729437
23. Charm J, Cho P. High myopia-partial reduction ortho-k: a 2-year randomized study. Optom Vis Sci. 2013; 90: 530–539. pmid:23645372
24. Lau JK, Vincent SJ, Cheung SW, Cho P. Higher-order aberrations and axial elongation in myopic children treated with orthokeratology. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2020; 61: 22. pmid:32068792
25. Wang D, Chun RK, Liu M, Lee RP, Sun Y, Zhang T, et al. Optical defocus rapidly changes choroidal thickness in schoolchildren. PLoS One. 2016; 11: e0161535. pmid:27537606
26. Delshad S, Collins MJ, Read SA, Vincent SJ. The time course of the onset and recovery of axial length changes in response to imposed defocus. Scientific Reports. 2020; 10: 8322. pmid:32433541
27. Chakraborty R, Read SA, Collins MJ. Monocular myopic defocus and daily changes in axial length and choroidal thickness of human eyes. Exp Eye Res. 2012; 103: 47–54. pmid:22971342
28. Chakraborty R, Read SA, Collins MJ. Hyperopic defocus and diurnal changes in human choroid and axial length. Optom Vis Sci. 2013; 90: 1187–1198. pmid:24061153
29. Read SA, Collins MJ, Sander BP. Human optical axial length and defocus. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010; 51: 6262–6269. pmid:20592235
30. Chiang ST, Phillips JR, Backhouse S. Effect of retinal image defocus on the thickness of the human choroid. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2015; 35: 405–413. pmid:26010292
31. Chiang ST, Chen TL, Phillips JR. Effect of optical defocus on choroidal thickness in healthy adults with presbyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2018; 59: 5188–5193. pmid:30372745
32. Tse DY, Lam CS, Guggenheim JA, Lam C, Li KK, Liu Q, et al. Simultaneous defocus integration during refractive development. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007; 48: 5352–5359. pmid:18055781
33. Flitcroft DI. The complex interactions of retinal, optical and environmental factors in myopia aetiology. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2012; 31: 622–660. pmid:22772022
34. Garcia MG, Ohlendorf A, Schaeffel F, Wahl S. Dioptric defocus maps across the visual field for different indoor environments. Biomed Opt Express. 2018; 9: 347–359. pmid:29359108
35. Zhu X. Temporal integration of visual signals in lens compensation (a review). Exp Eye Res. 2013; 114: 69–76. pmid:23470505
36. Winawer J, Zhu X, Choi J, Wallman J. Ocular compensation for alternating myopic and hyperopic defocus. Vision Res. 2005; 45: 1667–1677. pmid:15792842
37. Winawer J, Wallman J. Temporal constraints on lens compensation in chicks. Vision Res. 2002; 42: 2651–2668. pmid:12445851
38. Zhu X, Winawer JA, Wallman J. Potency of myopic defocus in spectacle lens compensation. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003; 44: 2818–2827. pmid:12824218
39. Schmid KL, Wildsoet CF. Effects on the compensatory responses to positive and negative lenses of intermittent lens wear and ciliary nerve section in chicks. Vision Res. 1996; 36: 1023–1036. pmid:8736261
40. McBrien NA, Arumugam B, Metlapally S. The effect of daily transient +4 D positive lens wear on the inhibition of myopia in the tree shrew. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53: 1593–1601. pmid:22323488
41. Norton TT, Siegwart JT, Amedo AO. Effectiveness of hyperopic defocus, minimal defocus, or myopic defocus in competition with a myopiagenic stimulus in tree shrew eyes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006; 47: 4687–4699. pmid:17065475
42. Arumugam B, Hung LF, To CH, Holden B, Smith EL. The effects of simultaneous dual focus lenses on refractive development in infant monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014; 55: 7423–7432. pmid:25324283
43. Benavente-Perez A, Nour A, Troilo D. The effect of simultaneous negative and positive defocus on eye growth and development of refractive state in marmosets. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53: 6479–6487. pmid:22918633
44. Napper GA, Brennan NA, Barrington M, Squires MA, Vessey GA, Vingrys AJ. The effect of an interrupted daily period of normal visual stimulation on form deprivation myopia in chicks. Vision Res. 1997; 37: 1557–1564. pmid:9231222
45. Chakraborty R, Read SA, Collins MJ. Diurnal variations in axial length, choroidal thickness, intraocular pressure, and ocular biometrics. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 5121–5129. pmid:21571673
46. Read SA, Collins MJ, Iskander DR. Diurnal variation of axial length, intraocular pressure, and anterior eye biometrics. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008; 49: 2911–2918. pmid:18362106
47. Moderiano D, Do M, Hobbs S, Lam V, Sarin S, Alonso-Caneiro D, et al. Influence of the time of day on axial length and choroidal thickness changes to hyperopic and myopic defocus in human eyes. Exp Eye Res 2019; 182: 125–136. pmid:30926510
48. Ulas F, Celik F, Dogan U, Celebi S. Effect of smoking on choroidal thickness in healthy smokers. Curr Eye Res. 2014; 39: 504–511. pmid:24215308
49. Altinkaynak H, Ceylan E, Kartal B, Keles S, Ekinci M, Olcaysu OO. Measurement of choroidal thickness following caffeine intake in healthy subjects. Curr Eye Res. 2016; 41: 708–714. pmid:25860873
50. Sayin N, Kara N, Pekel G, Altinkaynak H. Choroidal thickness changes after dynamic exercise as measured by spectral-domain optical coherence tomography. Indian J Ophthalmol. 2015; 63: 445–450 pmid:26139808
51. Bland JM, Altman DG. Measurement error. BMJ. 1996; 313:744. pmid:8819450
52. Alonso-Caneiro D, Read SA, Collins MJ. Speckle reduction in optical coherence tomography imaging by affine-motion image registration. J Biomed Opt. 2011; 16: 116027. pmid:22112132
53. Bland JM, Altman DG. Measuring agreement in method comparison studies. Stat Methods Med Res. 1999; 8: 135–160. pmid:10501650
54. Sander BP, Collins MJ. Read SA. The interaction between homatropine and optical blur on choroidal thickness. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2018; 38: 257–265. pmid:29691923
55. Oner V, Bulut A, Oter K. The effect of topical anti-muscarinic agents on subfoveal choroidal thickness in healthy adults. Eye. 2016; 30: 925–928. pmid:27055680
56. Breher K, García García M, Ohlendorf A, Wahl S. The effect of the optical design of multifocal contact lenses on choroidal thickness. PloS one. 2018; 13: e0207637. pmid:30444900
57. Demb JB. Functional circuitry of visual adaptation in the retina. J Physiol. 2008; 586: 4377–4384. pmid:18617564
58. Heinrich TS, Bach M. Contrast adaptation in retinal and cortical evoked potentials: no adaptation to low spatial frequencies. Visual Neuroscience. 2002; 19: 645–650. pmid:12507330
59. Diether S, Gekeler F, Schaeffel F. Changes in contrast sensitivity induced by defocus and their possible relations to emmetropization in the chicken. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2001; 42: 3072–3079. pmid:11687557
60. Diether S, Wildsoet CF. Stimulus requirements for the decoding of myopic and hyperopic defocus under single and competing defocus conditions in the chicken. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005; 46: 2242–2252. pmid:15980207
61. Cholewiak SA, Love GD, Banks MS. Creating correct blur and its effect on accommodation. J Vis. 2018; 18: 1–29. pmid:30193343
62. Seidemann A, Schaeffel F. Effects of longitudinal chromatic aberration on accommodation and emmetropization. Vision Res. 2002; 42: 2409–2417. pmid:12367740
63. Rucker FJ, Osorio D. The effects of longitudinal chromatic aberration and a shift in the peak of the middle-wavelength sensitive cone fundamental on cone contrast. Vision Res. 2008; 48: 1929–1939. pmid:18639571
64. Leube A, Kostial S, Alex Ochakovski G, Ohlendorf A, Wahl S. Symmetric visual response to positive and negative induced spherical defocus under monochromatic light conditions. Vision Res. 2017; 143: 52–57. pmid:29277449
65. Del Aguila-Carrasco AJ, Marin-Franch I, Bernal-Molina P, Esteve-Taboada JJ, Kruger PB, Montes-Mico R, et al. Accommodation responds to optical vergence and not defocus blur alone. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2017; 58: 1758–1763. pmid:28324117
66. Flitcroft DI. A model of the contribution of oculomotor and optical factors to emmetropization and myopia. Vision Res. 1998; 38: 2869–2879. pmid:9797983
67. Wallman J, Winawer J. Homeostasis of eye growth and the question of myopia. Neuron. 2004; 43: 447–468. pmid:15312645
68. Quek TP, Chua CG, Chong CS, Chong JH, Hey HW, Lee J, et al. Prevalence of refractive errors in teenage high school students in Singapore. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2004; 24: 47–55. pmid:14687201
69. Gwiazda J, Thorn F, Held R. Accommodation, accommodative convergence, and response AC/A ratios before and at the onset of myopia in children. Optom Vis Sci. 2005; 82: 273–278. pmid:15829855
70. Gwiazda J, Thorn F, Bauer J, Held R. Myopic children show insufficient accommodative response to blur. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993; 34: 690–694. pmid:8449687
71. Charman WN. Near vision, lags of accommodation and myopia. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1999; 19: 126–133. pmid:10615448
72. Collins MJ, Wildsoet CF, Atchison DA. Monochromatic aberrations and myopia. Vision Res. 1995; 35: 1157–1163. pmid:7610577
73. Hughes RP, Vincent SJ, Read SA, Collins MJ. Higher order aberrations, refractive error development and myopia control: a review. Clin Exp Optom. 2020; 103: 68–85. pmid:31489693
74. Mutti DO, Hayes JR, Mitchell GL, Jones LA, Moeschberger ML, Cotter SA, et al. Refractive error, axial length, and relative peripheral refractive error before and after the onset of myopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007; 48: 2510–2519. pmid:17525178
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2020 Delshad et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Purpose
To investigate the change in axial length (AxL) and choroidal thickness (ChT) in response to continuous and alternating episodes of monocular myopic and hyperopic defocus.
Methods
The right eye of sixteen young adults was exposed to 60 minute episodes of either continuous or alternating myopic and hyperopic defocus (+3 DS & -3 DS) over six separate days, with the left eye optimally corrected for distance. During alternating defocus conditions, the eye was exposed to either 30 or 15 minute cycles of myopic and hyperopic defocus, with the order of defocus reversed in separate sessions. The AxL and ChT of the right eye were measured before, during and after each defocus condition.
Results
Significant changes in AxL were observed over time, dependent upon the defocus condition (p < 0.0001). In general, AxL exhibited a greater magnitude of change during continuous than alternating defocus conditions. The maximum AxL elongation was +7 ± 7 μm (p = 0.010) in response to continuous hyperopic defocus and the maximum AxL reduction was -8 ± 10 μm of (p = 0.046) in response to continuous myopic defocus. During both 30 and 15 minute cycles of alternating myopic and hyperopic defocus of equal duration, the effect of opposing blur sessions cancelled each other and the AxL was near baseline levels following the final defocus session (mean change from baseline across all alternating defocus conditions was +2 ± 10 μm, p > 0.05). Similar, but smaller magnitude, changes were observed for ChT.
Conclusions
The human eye appears capable of temporal averaging of visual cues from alternating myopic and hyperopic defocus. In the short term, this integration appears to be a cancellation of the effects of the preceding defocus condition of opposite sign.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer