Introduction
Nonverbal communication generally refers to any communication made other than by words (Knapp, Hall, & Horgan, 2014; Patterson, 2011). For example, a person’s physical and vocal characteristics transmit information. Nonverbaehaviors (e.g., facial expressions, gestures) and interpersonaistances also play an important role in face-to-face interactions (Burgoon, Guerrero, & Floyd, 2010; Moore, Hickson, & Stacks, 2014). The volume, the range, and the complexity of scientific research on nonverbal communication may be seen from comprehensive handbooks on the topic published in recent years (Hall & Knapp, 2013; Manusov & Patterson, 2006; Matsumoto, Hwang, & Frank, 2016).
For security and justice professionals (e.g., police officers, lawyers, judges), the thousands of peer-reviewed articles on nonverbal communication represent important sources of knowledge, including on the detection of lies or mal-intent (Granhag & Strömwall, 2004; Granhag, Vrij, & Verschuere, 2015; Vrij, 2008). Lying, however, cannot be detected at a glance as is often staten the Internet. The belief that gaze avoidance allows lie detection is a widespread misconception (The Globaeception Research Team, 2006). Like the unrealistic expectations of the public toward forensic science (Chin & Workewych, 2016), nonverbal communication has suffered from its popularity in television series (e.g., Lie to Me) anther popular media (Levine, Serota, & Shulman, 2010; Vrij, Granhag, & Porter, 2010). In fact, scholars who have scientific expertise in lie (and truth) detection agree that there are no nonverbaehaviors that are present in aliars and are absent in all people who tell the truth. There are no nonverbaehaviors that are indicative of deception, such as Pinocchio’s nose (DePaulo et al., 2003; Vrij, 2008). In addition, when facial expressions and gestures are documented as having a link tying, this link is typically weak (DePaulo et al., 2003; Vrij et al., 2017) anften moderatey situational variables (Sporer & Schwandt, 2006, 2007). In other words, although not a silver bullet, the analysis of an individual’s nonverbaehaviors can be basen knowledge published in peer-reviewed scientific journals. In fact, research conductey the international community of scholars who have scientific expertise in nonverbal communication can inform understanding of a wide range of human behaviors (Burgoon et al., 2010; Knapp et al., 2014; Moore et al., 2014; Patterson, 2011).
Despite the wealth of peer-reviewed knowledge on nonverbal communication, security and justice professionals can readily find widely disseminated programs, methods, and approaches that fail to reflect the state of science and promote pseudoscientific claims. The objective of this article is to examine (i) concepts of nonverbal communication conveyey these programs, methods, and approaches, but also (ii) the consequences of their use. To achieve this objective, we wilescribe the scope of scientific research on nonverbal communication. Subsequently, we will examine a program aimed at identifying aviation security threats through monitoring the nonverbaehaviors and appearance of passengers at U.S. airports (SPOT; Screening of Passengers by Observation Techniques), an interview method usey many police forces (the BAI; Behavior Analysis Interview) and an approach to “read gestures” taught to security and justice professionals, including in France and Quebec (synergology). Finally, we wilutline five hypotheses to explain why some organizations in the fields of security and justice are turning to pseudoscience and pseudoscientific techniques. We will conclude the article by inviting these organizations to work with the international community of scholars who have scientific expertise in nonverbal communication anie (and truth) detection to implement evidence-based practices.
The Scope of Scientific Research on Nonverbal Communication
Many of the issues facing security and justice professionals involve the concealment and falsification of information (e.g., Garrido, Masip, & Herrero, 2004; Mann, Garcia-Rada, Houser, & Ariely, 2014; Vrij, Mann, Jundi, Hillman, & Hope, 2014). When it comes teveloping better professional practices to address these issues, the analysis of nonverbal communication may, on the face of it, seem foolproof. Indeed, it is not uncommon to rear hear that facial expressions and gestures can be used tetect lies, and that they woule more valid in this regard than words. Security and justice professionals would therefore have at their fingertips, for a few thousand, or even a few hundreollars, programs, methods, and approaches to know what the other person thinks but does not say.
Unfortunately, dubious concepts regarding nonverbal communication are widely disseminated, particularly on the Internet and in books aimed at the general public, as well as at seminars and conferences (such as “the body language never lies”). The use of such concepts can have negative and perhaps even disastrous consequences (Denault, 2015; Kozinski, 2015; Lilienfeld & Landfield, 2008). For example, security and justice professionals who are not familiar with the “peer-review” process can be misled intelieving that these dubious concepts are scientific and give them a totally unjustified authority (Jupe & Denault, 2018). As we wilemonstrate, the reliance on such concepts is fundamentally misguided, because decisions of security and justice professionals coule distorted and harm the life or liberty of individuals.
Although the detection of lies or mal-intent may be of interest to these professionals, it represents only a small part of the scientific research being conducten nonverbal communication. As Plusquellec anenault (2018) pointeut, the influence of culture, age, and mental illness on nonverbal communication, as well as the recognition of facial expressions and interpersonal sensitivity, are just some of the subjects receiving attention from a community of researchers from alver the world. Nonverbal communication is not a subject of scientific research in which psychology alone is concerned. For example, psychiatry, criminology, communication, linguistics, biology, sociology, anthropology, computer science, and ethology also focus on nonverbal communication. Regardless of the discipline, the affiliation of researchers, and their fields of research, the knowledge they develop is of great value because the process of critical appraisaf knowledge (the publication process), which is a centrauilding block in establishing confidence in the results of a research project, remains the same. Let us examine briefly just how this process unfolds.
When completing a research project, the researcher generally prepares a manuscript describing the reasons and theoretical rationale for the study, as well as all the steps preceding its conclusions, including the methodology, data collection, and analyses. All these details will eventually allow the international scientific community to scrutinize the research project to provide support or criticism of the results in full knowledge of the facts (e.g., in the light of other research on the subject). All these details will also provide other researchers the opportunity to replicate the study and compare the results (Asendorpf et al., 2013; Jupe & Denault, 2018; Shipman, 2014; Ware, 2008). The manuscript is then submitted to the editor of a scientific journal who sends it to experts on the subject for a first critical evaluation. This is the “peer-review” process. Manuscripts are commonly revieweouble-blind, meaning that the names of the reviewers are not known to the researcher and the name of the researcher is not known to the reviewers. The reviewers provide feedback to the editor. Following the reviewers’ feedback and his/her own evaluation of the manuscript, the editor informs the researcher that the manuscript is accepted as it is, with either minor or major revisions, or that it is rejected (sometimes with the possibility to revise and resubmit the manuscript after significant improvements) (Denault & Dunbar, 2017; Jupe & Denault, 2018). Some scientific journals in psychology have a rejection rate of up to 90% (American Psychological Association, 2017). Since the 1960s, approximately 30,000 peer-reviewed articles on nonverbal communication have been published (Plusquellec & Denault, 2018).
Pseudoscience in the Security and Justice Community
Despite the extent of scientific research on nonverbal communication (Burgoon et al., 2010; Knapp et al., 2014; Moore et al., 2014; Patterson, 2011), security and justice professionals in some jurisdictions have turned to programs, methods, and approaches that fail to reflect the state of science. The consequences of the misuses of nonverbal communication are important enough to question the responsibility of organizations in the fields of security and justice that have used either SPOT, the BAI, or synergology.
SPOT
Standing for Screening of Passengers by Observation Techniques, SPOT is a program aimed at identifying aviation security threats through monitoring the nonverbaehaviors and appearance of passengers. This program was implemented at numerous U.S. airports by the Transportation Security Administration (TSA) of the U.S. Department of Homeland Security (DHS) following the September 11, 2001 attacks. Basen Israeli methods for attempting tetect suspicious behaviors, SPOT was implemented in 2006 and 2007 at 42 TSA-regulated airports after being tested in 2003 and 2004 “to understand the potentiaf the program, not to validate its success” (U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2010, p. 25). In 2010, nearly 3,000 Behavior Detection Officers (hereinafter referred to as BDOs) were deployed at 161 of the 457 airports regulatey the TSA. SPOT had an annual cost of approximately $212 million (U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2010).
According to the TSA, SPOT was basen several sources, namely learning tools from federal agencies, comments from law enforcement officers (e.g., Federaureau of Investigation, Drug Enforcement Agency), and the work of researchers, including Paul Ekman, who attempted tefend it before a committee of the U.S. Congress in 2011 (Committee on Science, Space, and Technology, 2011). However, at the time of the implementation of SPOT, the validity of identifying aviation security threats through monitoring the nonverbaehaviors and appearance of passengers was unknown. The effectiveness of identifying aviation security threats using SPOT versus random questioning (i.e., questioning passengers at random rather than on the basis of behavioral indicators) was also unknown. According to the TSA, SPOT was implementeecause it was low cost, was easy to set up, and provided an additional security measure for risks that were not coverey other measures (U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2010).
For the identification of aviation security threats, SPOT employed the BDOs. These individuals were recruited from TSA employees who had performed passenger anaggage screening. They received four days of classroom and three days of field training and took an exam. The BDOs were required, among other things, to memorize the list of indicators to identify potential terrorists (e.g., avoiding eye contact, looking down, wearing clothing inappropriate for the location, having a pale face due to recent beard shaving, emitting a strong body odor, covering the mouth with the hand when speaking; The Intercept, 2015; U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2010). At airports, the BDOs’ job was tbserve waiting passengers (about 30 seconds per passenger). An exchange with a passenger coule initiateuring this observation procedure. Following the exchange, if the passenger exceeded a certain score (from the list of 94 behavioral indicators), BDOs could conduct a pat-down and a search of his/her luggage. They could then invite the passenger to continue his/her journey or contact the law enforcement officers who then had the option to arrest the passenger. In addition, when the passenger was not arrested, the TSA could still refuse to allow him/her toard the aircraft. Finally, the BDOs were to enter information about their intervention into a database (U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2010, 2013, 2017).
In 2010, the U.S. Government Accountability Office (GAO), an organization with a similar role to the Office of the Auditor Generaf Canada, recommended that the TSA bring together independent experts to validate the scientific basis of SPOT (U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2010). Three years later, the GAO recommended that the U.S. Congress consider the absence of scientific evidence on the effectiveness of identifying aviation security threats through the nonverbaehaviors of passengers in its SPOT funding decisions (U.S. Government Accountability Office. 2013). Finally, in 2017, the GAO published a report according to which 175 out of the 178 (98%) sources citey the TSA were not relevant for determining the validity of SPOT. Indeed, of the 178 sources cited, only 20 were research articles reporting data and methods. Of the remaining 158 sources, 21 were literature reviews that did not report adequate information, and 137 were opinion papers or documents that provided irrelevant information for establishing the scientific basis of SPOT (e.g., newspaper articles, screenshots of medical websites). The 20 research articles were independently evaluatey two analysts: 5 articles did not meet generally accepted research standards and 12 did not validate the behavioral indicators for which they were citey the TSA. One source justified seven of these indicators and two sources justifienly one. In other words, the TSA did not have any evidentiary source to support the validity of 28 of the 36 indicators on the reviseist usey the BDOs to identify aviation security threats2. The GAO has therefore maintained its 2013 recommendations timit SPOT funding (U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2017).
Following the 2017 report, as did the TSA in 2004 (U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2010), DHS attempted tefend SPOT, in some cases by using logical fallacies (e.g., overreliance on anecdotal evidence; Gambrill, 2005; Lilienfeld & Landfield, 2008). For example, it was reported that a passenger identifiey BDOs was carrying 4.4 kilograms of cocaine and that the techniques used to hide drugs coule used to hide explosives. In addition, the TSA replied that the BDOs haeen reassigned as Transportation Security Officers required to perform behavioral analysis for a few hours a day to maintain their skills. However, given the lack of scientific evidence on the effectiveness of identifying aviation security threats through the nonverbaehaviors of passengers, the GAO expressed concern that behavior analysis was stileing used (U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2017).
It shoule noted that proponents of SPOT may claim that this program workeecause a terrorist act such as that of September 11, 2001 has not occurred again. However, this is another logical fallacy. Although SPOT can deter some terrorists, just as dummy cameras can deter some thieves, this does not mean SPOT actually works. Furthermore, security and justice organizations should not use tragedies to justify dubious programs, methods, and approaches and avoid putting in place programs, methods, and approaches that actually work.
The use of logical fallacies to justify the importance of SPOT (a program estimated to have cost $1.5 billion from 2007 to 2015; Office of Inspector General, 2016) seems at best questionable, especially since SPOT created “an unacceptable risk of racial and religious profiling” (ACLU, 2017, p. 1). Indeed, rather than detecting terrorists, behavioral indicators have leDOs to target immigrants. Moreover, “untiate 2012, training materials for behavior detection officers focused exclusively on examples of Arar Muslim terrorists” (ACLU, 2017, p. 13; see also Winter, 2015).
Did SPOT offer a false sense of security?; Possibly. Could the financial resources allocated to the TSA (e.g., taxpayers’ money) have been invested in the development of new programs basen knowledge published in peer-reviewed scientific journals, as well as in programs that have already demonstrated their effectiveness?; Certainly. For example, rather than being invested in behavior analysis programs of unknown effectiveness, taxpayers’ money coule invested in locaaw enforcement, which is generally limited in personnel and financial resources, including the promotion of proactive policing and prosecution (Bayley & Weisburd, 2009; Howard, 2004; LaFree & Freilich, 2018).
The TSA, however, does not appear to have questioned its approach following the 2017 report of the U.S. Government Accountability Office. Indeed, the Boston Globe (2018) recently revealed the existence of Quiet Skies, a TSA surveillance program where travelers (who are not under investigation and not on a terrorist watch list) are monitorey federal agents before they board an aircraft. This surveillance was partly basen behavioral indicators as dubious as those of SPOT (e.g., strong body odor, excessive perspiration, rapid eye blinking, hands that touch the face, recent beard shaving)3. Nevertheless, the effectiveness of using nonverbaehaviors for security controls at airports or tetermine whether people are hiding an object is limited (Ormerod & Dando, 2015; Sweet, Meissner, & Atkinson, 2017). Moreover, observation of behavior alone reduces accuracy in judgments (Bond & DePaulo, 2006; Bond, Howard, Hutchison, & Masip, 2013; Reinhard, Sporer, & Scharmach, 2013; Reinhard, Sporer, Scharmach, & Marksteiner, 2011). In other words, current scientific knowledge on nonverbal communication suggests that security and justice professionals should not rely on the observation of behavioral indicators (or combinations of some of them) in face-to-face interactions tetect terrorists. Dubious concepts regarding nonverbal communication, however, continue te used, not only by Transportation Security Officers, but alsy professionals from other backgrounds.
The BAI
Standing for Behavior Analysis Interview, the BAI is the first step of the Reid technique, an interrogation technique in which more than 500,000 people have reportedly been trained (John E. Reid & Associates, n.d.b, n.d.c). Essentially, in this first step, an investigator conducts a non-accusatory interview with a suspect. Special attention is paid to the suspect’s nonverbaehaviors when answering certain questions askey the investigator (Snook, Eastwood, & Barron, 2014; Vrij, 2008). According to the creators of the Reid technique, this interview method is “designed to identify whether or not a person is telling the truth or withholding relevant information concerning a specific crime or act of wrongdoing” (John E. Reid & Associates, n.d.a). For example, the BAI claims that some nonverbaehaviors are linked with deception (e.g., closed and retreated posture, frozen and static, non-frontal alignment, tean forward constantly) or truthfulness (e.g., open and relaxed posture, dynamic, frontal alignment, tean forwarccasionally) (Inbau, Reid, Buckley, & Jayne, 2013). At the enf the BAI, when the guilt of the suspect “in the opinion of the investigator, seems definite or reasonably certain” (Inbau et al., 2013, p. 185), the investigator proceeds to the second step of the Reid technique. The objective is then tbtain an incriminating statement through a psychologically coercive adversarial interrogation (Masip, Herrero, Garrido, & Barba, 2011; Snook et al., 2014; Vrij, 2008).
In this second step, the investigator must state that there is noubt in his/her mind that the suspect is guilty of the crime. The investigator then provides a moral excuse for the crime so that the suspect can “save face”. In addition, the investigator will ensure, by various means, that the suspect cannot deny involvement (e.g., by interrupting the suspect). Finally, “to elicit an initial admission of guilt” (Inbau et al., 2013, p. 294), the investigator asks the suspect a question to which both possible answers are incriminating, for example, “Was the stolen money used tuy your drugs or to help your family?;”. Following the incriminating statement, the investigator requests details and proceeds tbtain a written statement (Inbau et al., 2013; Snook et al., 2014). Although it has many followers, especially in the United States, the Reid technique can lead to miscarriages of justice (e.g., Gudjonsson, 2014; St-Yves & Meissner, 2014).
The BAI is particularly alarming in its use of verbal and nonverbaehaviors in determining the suspect’s guilt or innocence. For example, according to Inbau et al. (2013), “shifts in the chair that occur during or immediately following a significant statement, such as denial, often indicate fear of detection and shoule associated with deception” (p. 134), and “generally speaking, a suspect whoes not make direct eye contact is probably withholding information” (p. 135). However, even if these behaviors are “inconsistent with the existing research regarding the nonverbaehaviors of truthful aneceptive suspects” (Blair & Kooi, 2004, p. 82), they can add weight to the erroneous certainty of a person’s guilt or innocence.
In addition, the specific study often presented as supporting the BAI foundations (Horvath, Jayne, & Buckley, 1994) suffers from fatal methodological shortcomings (e.g., small sample, no comparison group of untrainer lay evaluators; Kassin, 2015; Masip et al., 2011; Vrij, 2008). In fact, experimental research does not support the BAI effectiveness (Vrij, Mann, & Fisher, 2006). As Harrigan (2005) pointeut, the state of science is clear: “Unlike certain facial expressions, there are few, if any, body movements that have invariant meaning within or across cultures” (p. 139). Furthermore, research has shown that the BAI guilt or innocence indicators merely reflect common popular misbeliefs about behavioral correlates of guilt or innocence (Masip, Barba, & Herrero, 2012; Masip & Herrero, 2013; Masip et al., 2011). In short, although some other aspects of BAI may offer avenues for research, believing that a suspect’s behavior following certain questions signals a suspect’s guilt or innocence has little or no scientific basis (Masip & Herrero, 2013; Vrij & Fisher, 2016; Vrij et al., 2006; Vrij et al., 2017; see also Masip, 2017, for a recent review of scientific research on deception detection).
Of course, as for pseudoscience and pseudoscientific techniques, that is, bodies of information “that possess the superficial appearance of science but lack its substance” (Lilienfeld & Landfield, 2008, p. 1216), precautions are formulated regarding behavioral indicators. For example, Inbau et al. (2013) stated that the validity of behavioral indicators may be influencey “the perceived seriousness of the offense, the mental and the physical condition of the subject, any underlying psychiatric or personality disorders, levef intelligence, degree of maturity, and the extent or absence of social responsibilities” (p. 152). This call to caution, however, is of no use. Investigators cannot know all the variables that may influence an individual’s verbal and nonverbaehaviors. In addition, Inbau et al. (2013) do not explain how, in practice, all these factors actually influence all the behaviors that they link with deception or truthfulness. The same goes for precautions regarding specific behaviors. For example, regarding the lack of eye contact, Inbau et al. (2013) stated that it may result from various factors, including the suspect’s culture, as well as an inferiority complex and an emotionaisorder. Worse still, in the case of the lack of eye contact, scientific research on nonverbal communication has even shown that it is not a valid sign of lying (DePaulo et al., 2003; Sporer & Schwandt, 2007).
In many other contexts, the dubious meanings that the BAI assigns to verbal and nonverbaehaviors might, on the face of it, appear amusing. When usey security and justice professionals, however, they can leaoth innocent and guilty persons te subjected to a psychologically coercive adversarial interrogation and increase the risk of innocent people (especially juveniles anther vulnerable persons) making false confessions (e.g., Horgan, Russano, Meissner, & Evans, 2012; Kassin, 2015; Kassin & Gudjonsson, 2004; Kassin & Sukel, 1997; Russano, Meissner, Narchet, & Kassin, 2005). The questionable meanings that the BAI assigns to verbal and nonverbaehaviors are especially worrying because the ability of investigators tetect lies basen nonverbaehaviors is generally netter than chance (Aamodt & Custer, 2006; Bogaard, Meijer, Vrij, & Merckelbach, 2016; Hauch, Sporer, Michael, & Meissner, 2016). In addition, training in the Reid technique decreases the accuracy of these judgments, while, at the same time, increases investigators’ confidence in their accuracy (Kassin & Fong, 1999; Mann, Vrij, & Bull, 2004; Meissner & Kassin, 2002). Despite all this, the creators of the Reid technique continue to “guarantee” that training in this technique allows to “increase your ability to eliminate the innocent, identify the guilty and motivate subject’s "sic" to tell the truth” (John E. Reid & Associates, n.d.c). In addition to the BAI and SPOT, other programs, methods, and approaches promote pseudoscientific claims. Synergology, an approach to “read gestures” taught to French-speaking security and justice professionals, is one of them.
Synergology
According to its “official” website, synergology is a “scientific discipline of reading gestures” which is “anchored in a multidisciplinary field at the crossroads of neurosciences and communication sciences” (Synergology, the Official Website, n.d.b, n.d.a, our translation). It purports te “in this line of sciences which seek tetter understand any body movement as an indicator of an unconscious mental process” (Monnin, 2009, p. 35, our translation). More specifically, proponents of synergology purport that it uses “several revolutionary techniques and methods derived from the most recent discoveries in the fielf behavioral sciences” (Gagnon, n.d.b., our translation), and fills “the lack of a serious reference in nonverbal communication” (Bunard, 2018, p. 47, our translation). In addition, proponents of synergology claim their approach was “founded to undeliefs in popular communication” (Institut Québécois de Synergologie, 2016, our translation). They also claim that its use is restrictey a “code of ethics” (Association Européenne de Synergologie, n.d.).
To know what the other thinks “before the other has even accessed his own thoughts” (Bunard, 2018, p. 20, our translation), proponents of synergology say they have accumulated thousands of videos in databases aninked meanings tifferent gestures (Bunard, 2018; Gagnon & Martineau, 2010; Story, 2018). According to the founder of synergology, each association “must be verified in at least 80% of the situations resulting from databases images. In the case of micro itching, it must be verified in at least 90% of cases” (Turchet, 2009, p. 299, our translation; see alsunard, 2018; Turchet, 2012).
For example, hand movements are supposedly of the utmost importance because “looking at the hands, following them in all their movements on the face anody seems te the best way tecipher the emotions and therefore the thoughts of our interlocutor” (Turchet, 2009, p. 103, our translation). Among the hand movements, micro itching (i.e. “unconscious itching relievey the tip of the finger or nail” Turchet, 2009, p. 311, our translation) supposedly occurs “always in embarrassing situations that provoke internal contradictions when we do not allow ourselves what we want, when we censor our words, our attitudes... therefore when we are uncomfortable” (Turchet, 2009, p. 112, our translation). In other words, a micro itching supposedly emerges “when there are contradictions between what is said and what is thought” (Gagnon & Martineau, 2010, p. 54, our translation; see also Monnin, 2009). Therefore, when a person scratches under the right nostril, it would mean that “I don’t believe what the other person says” (Turchet, 2009, p. 136, our translation) and when a person scratches under the left nostril, it would mean that “I don’t say everything, or not exactly, what I think” (Turchet, 2009, p. 136, our translation). When a person scratches on top of his/her nose on the right, that would indicate “something bothers me in what the other shows” (Turchet, 2009, p. 138, our translation) and when he/she scratches on top of his/her nose on the left, that would indicate “there is something that disturbs my image” (Turchet, 2009, p. 138, our translation). According to Story (2018), “there are about fifteen ways to touch your nose, with very different interpretations” (p. 32, our translation). In total, more than 50 different points on the face would have different meanings when scratched (Turchet, 2009, 2012, 2017). In addition, positions of the body on a chair and those of the head would also have different meanings (see Denault & Jupe, 2017, for an evaluation of a “synergological” analysis).
For example, a sitting person coule in a position of withdrawal (backward), analysis (backward to the right), escape (backward to the left), stress aniscourse control (right), reserve and emotional control (left), attack (forward to the right), flexibility (forward to the left), and interest (forward) (Bunard, 2018; see also Gagnon & Martineau, 2010; Story, 2018). Moreover, “if the head is strongly bent to the left, then we speak of submission or abandonment depending on the context. If the head is strongly bent to the right, we speak then of rigidity” (Bunard, 2018, p. 100, our translation). In addition, when the head is turned to the right “we can assume that the relationship is good, that our interlocutor is confident, in a climate conducive to exchanges” (Story, 2018, p. 65, our translation) and when it is turned to the left “we can assume that he is more distant, that he analyses and classifies the information with his left brain and that as a result, he may, by nature or according to the context, be on the defensive or in search of performance” (Story, 2018, p. 65, our translation).
Proponents of synergology have also associateifferent gestures with particular contexts, including that of lying. According to Gagnon & Martineau (2010), “the gap between truth anies is never completely masked and the body transmits it” (p. 44, our translation; see alsunard, 2018). For example, Story (2018) claims that a liar “tends to make two-dimensional gestures, mechanical, disembodied, rather large” (p. 182, our translation), because “only a person who has really experienced a situation can reproduce it in three dimensions, with gestures sometimes very close to the body anf low amplitude” (Story, 2018, p. 182, our translation). Gagnon and Martineau (2010) purport that when a person pinches his/her nose while telling the customs officer that he/she has nothing teclare, “the customs officer being alert to the nonverbal, should ask for assistance in order to carry out necessary checks since this gesture is closely linked to the unspoken” (p. 57, our translation). However, according to Gagnon and Martineau (2010), one should not rely on a single gesture, but “when you see an inconsistency between the gesture and the word, be sure to identify the subject of the discussion, because it is probably (not exclusively) on this point that the person lies, exaggerates or hides something” (p. 42, our translation). Therefore, synergology would “save time, open up new avenues for consideration and investigation by identifying authenticity more quickly” (Synergology, the Official Website, n.d.c, our translation).
Despite the seemingly scientific and ethical claims of proponents of synergology, their approach neglects the process of critical appraisaf scientific research on nonverbal communication (Burgoon et al., 2010; Denault & Jupe, 2017; Harrigan, Rosenthal, & Scherer, 2005). Tur knowledge, since the “creation” of synergology in 1996 (Synergology, the Official Website, n.d.e), the meanings this approach claims to have associated with different gestures (e.g., micro itching) have not been the subject of peer-reviewed articles. They “look like rabbits you would pulut of hats: you can’t see where they come from, and they magically appear” (Lardellier, 2008, p. 12, our translation; see also Axelrad, 2012; Jarry, 2016, 2018; Lardellier, 2017). Furthermore, the effectiveness of seminars and conferences on synergology as well as the effectiveness of the 200-plus hours training to “become” a synergologist (at a cost of more than $6000 CAD) is unknown, even though the founder of synergology stated that “our methods make it possible tetect 80% of lies in the test called ‘guilty/innocent’” (Turchet, 2009, p. 322, our translation; see also Turchet, 2012).
Tur knowledge, the founder of synergology has publishenly one peer-reviewed article in a scientific journal (Turchet, 2013), which did not validate the meanings associatey synergologists tifferent gestures or the effectiveness of synergology. In addition, this article was severely criticized, particularly because of methodological and analyticaefects (e.g., inappropriate comparison, data selection, circular reasoning), and “casts serious doubt on the thousands of gestures to which synergologists give a meaning” (Rochat, Delmas, Denault, Elissalde, & Demarchi, 2018, p. 262, our translation; see alsenault, Larivée, Plouffe, & Plusquellec, 2015). In other words, synergology purports te “founded to undeliefs in popular communication” (Institut Québécois de Synergologie, 2016, our translation), but it replaces them with concepts that have not been the subject of peer-reviewed articles. Moreover, a “code of ethics” cannot make up for the lack of scientific evidence, especially because, tur knowledge, necision against a synergologist has been made public. Such a “code of ethics” (as well as the attribution of license numbers to synergologists) seems to mimic that of professional governing bodies, which are providey law. As a result, some organizations coulelieve that synergology has an official cachet that it does not have.
In addition, although synergology neglects the process of critical appraisaf scientific research on nonverbal communication, the meanings this approach claims to have associated with different gestures have been widely disseminated, particularly on the Internet and in books for the general public (e. g., Gagnon & Martineau, 2010; Turchet, 2004, 2009). One of these books, for example, was written because, according tne of the authors, “these people "the armed forces" didn’t want to wait until it was scientifically valid in ten years before they could use synergology” (Collignon, 2012, our translation). These meanings have alseen disseminated at seminars and conferences, some of which were aimed at security and justice professionals. For example, the Bar of Quebec, the professional governing body of lawyers whose mission is to ensure the protection of the public, made twnline training courses available until 2015. These training courses promoted concepts specific to synergology, which was presented as a discipline that is “basen a rigorous scientific approach” (Barreau du Québec, n.d.a, our translation).
For example, in the first training course (taken by 1,929 members of the Bar of Quebec; Lagacé, 2015), lawyers were taught that if a person “squeezes his/her lips, holds his/her right hand, recounts the past by looking to the right, scratches his/her neck back to the right, and makes low animited movements” (Barreau du Québec, n.d.a, our translation), it would indicate lying. In the second training course taken by 1,083 lawyers (Lagacé, 2015), the concepts presented had no stronger scientific foundation. For example, “open hands, exposed palms that move freely as the person speaks, and soft wrists indicate open communication where nothing is hidden” (Barreau du Québec, n.d.b, our translation). However, the use of such behavioral indicators (or combinations of some of them) has no scientific foundation (Hartwig & Bond, 2011, 2014; Mann, Vrij, & Bull, 2002; see also Vrij, Hartwig, & Granhag, 2019, for a recent review of scientific research on nonverbal communication aneception detection).
In addition to the Bar of Quebec, proponents of synergology claim to have assisted, trained, or otherwise had as clients professionals in positions of trust or authority, including police officers and judges of Québec courts (e.g., Régie du logement du Québec, Commission des lésions professionnelles, Municipal Court of the City of Montreal, Superior Court, Court of Quebec) (Denault, 2017; Denault et al., 2015). Furthermore, “official” training centers offer the 200-plus hours training to “become” a synergologist, notably in Belgium, France, Quebec, Spain, Switzerland, and the Netherlands (Institut Québécois de Synergologie, n.d.; Bunard, 2018; Synergology, the Official Website, n.d.d). In all these cases, the use of concepts specific to synergology by security and justice professionals can have very harmful effects. In Quebec courts, for example, if judges use concepts specific to synergology “having no more scientific basis than those used in medievardeals” (Denault, 2015, p. 9, our translation), the outcome of trials coule distorted, particularly when the evidence is limited to contradictory testimonies (e. g., at a sexual assault trial where the victim’s and the accused’s words are in opposition). Witnesses who tell the truth coule considereishonest, and vice-versa.
In response to criticism, synergologists argue that questions shoule asked to confirm or disprove a hypothesis after considering severaehavioral indicators anther concepts specific to their approach (e.g., Bagoe, 2015; Gagnon, 2015; Gagnon & Martineau, 2010; Institut Québécois de Synergologie, 2018; Story, 2018; Turchet, 2010). This call to caution, however, is of no use if these behavioral indicators and concepts have not been subjected to peer-review and replication. Indeed, as with the Reid technique, training in synergology could increase the confidence of judges in their ability tetect lies, while in fact, the accuracy of their judgments coulecrease. Confirmation bias, in contrast, coulead judges to ask questions to verify an erroneous hypothesis, to give more weight to the answers that corroborate it aness weight to the answers that contradict it (Porter & ten Brinke, 2009). In addition, also in response to criticism, other proponents of synergology argued that the current peer review system “offers an illusion of quality controf publications that reassures the less well-informed and enhances the image of researchers in the eyes of gullible people” (Loranger & Loranger, 2019, p. 79, our translation) and suggested that their approach is criticizeecause it is innovative (Denault, 2018; Jupe & Denault, 2018). Some proponents of synergology have also usether types of responses, including attacks on the morality and competence of the critics in an attempt tiscredit their arguments without responding to the substance of those arguments (Denault, 2018; Denault et al., 2015; Walton, 1987)4.
In spite of all this, synergology is now entering a new fielf application, that of identifying potential threats through the analysis of nonverbaehaviors and preventing terrorist acts. For example, a “specialized training in practical recognition and probable identification of malicious intentions of individuals or groups of individuals through the learning of known techniques of synergology and new safety concepts” (Cellule SCAN, n.d., our translation; see also Gagnon, 2018) is now available. Given that the effectiveness of synergology is dubious at best and the effectiveness of using nonverbaehaviors for security controls at airports or tetermine whether people are hiding an object is limited (Ormerod & Dando, 2015; Sweet et al., 2017), this new fielf application of synergology is highly questionable. Its use for preventing terrorist acts is of even more concern because the effectiveness of a $1.5 billion program (the SPOT) whose goal was similar (the identification of potential threats through the analysis of nonverbaehaviors) remains doubtfuespite years of scrutiny from the U.S. Government Accountability Office (2010, 2011, 2012, 2013, 2017).
Why Do Some Organizations Turn to Pseudoscience?;
The reasons for irrationaeliefs have been the subject of extensive scientific literature. People’s critical thinking abilities, political, and religious ideologies, as well as cognitive skills and scientific knowledge are some of those reasons (Bensley & Lilienfeld, 2017; Bensley, Lilienfeld, & Powell, 2014; Boudry, Blancke, & Pigliucci, 2015; Bronstein, Pennycook, Bear, Rand, & Cannon, 2018; Gauchat, 2012; Majima, 2015; Nisbet, Cooper, & Garrett, 2015; Pennycook, Cheyne, Barr, Koehler, & Fugelsang, 2015; Pennycook & Rand, 2018; Shen & Gromet, 2015). But why do some organizations in the fields of security and justice turn to pseudoscience and pseudoscientific techniques?; To an international scientific community that has published thousands of peer-reviewed articles on nonverbal communication, it may seem surprising that these organizations embrace programs, methods, and approaches that, on the surface, seem scientific but, in reality, are not. We offer five hypotheses as to why some organizations turn to pseudoscience.
The Problems to Solve
First, organizations in the fields of security and justice may be confronted with problems that these programs, methods, and approaches could apparently solve. For example, the importance and urgency of securing airports could partly explain why SPOT has been implemented at numerous U.S. airports. The desire to implement better professional practices could partly explain why the BAI is an interview method usey many police forces and why synergology has been taught to French-speaking security and justice professionals. The issues may alse exacerbater mitigatey circumstances within or outside these organizations. For example, the attacks of September 11, 2001, likely increased the importance and urgency of securing airports.
It is important to emphasize that, unlike scientific knowledge, pseudoscientific claims offer immediate and easy solutions to complex challenges. They are thus particularly enticing. For example, the work of security and justice professionals coule facilitatey the use of highly accurate lie detectors during their daily face-to-face interactions. While science cannot offer such devices because they simply do not exist, pseudoscientific claims can be tailored to the needs of professionals and appear te nearly infallible. Practitioners with limited knowledge of science and seeking a silver bullet might find these claims quite appealing. Thus, offering a “guarantee” that training in the Reid technique allows to “increase your ability to eliminate the innocent, identify the guilty and motivate subject’s "sic" to tell the truth” (John E. Reid & Associates, n.d.c) can have a highly persuasive effect on some police officers. Also, the fact that approaches are presented as truly scientific can increase their credibility. Thus, the assertion that synergology uses “several revolutionary techniques and methods derived from the most recent discoveries in the fielf behavioral sciences” (Gagnon, n.d.b, our translation) could convince organizations of its validity.
The Lack of Scientific Knowledge
Second, the lack of specific or general scientific knowledge could partly explain why some organizations turn to pseudoscience and pseudoscientific techniques. For example, knowledge of the state of scientific research on nonverbal communication makes it easy to recognize the true nature of the indicators conveyey SPOT, the BAI, and synergology. However, awareness and understanding of the process of critical appraisaf knowledge coulvercome the lack of specific scientific knowledge. Indeed, when science is implicitly or explicitly invoked tegitimize programs, methods, and approaches, peer-reviewed articles that demonstrate their effectiveness shoule requested and reviewed (to assess their relevance), regardless of the status of the individuals promoting those programs, methods, and approaches.
The following example illustrates the importance of this advice. In 2015, the founder of synergology (who was then pursuing a PhD in language sciences and whbtained it two years later; Turchet, 2017) sent a formal notice to a columnist requiring him to retract and apologize for criticizing him in a series of texts published in a French-language daily newspaper from Montreal. In this formal notice, which was also sharen social media by proponents of synergology, a number of references were cited, arguing that the columnist should have mentioned them. However, these references did not validate the meanings associatey synergologists tifferent gestures or the effectiveness of synergology. In fact, after reviewing the references, it became apparent that “there was no interest in considering them in order tetermine whether synergology is a sham or not” (Denault et al., 2015, p. 440, our translation). This episode is reminiscent of the 175 irrelevant sources citey the TSA tegitimize SPOT (U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2017).
The Ignorance of the Importance of Science
Third, even if organizations in the fields of security and justice do not lack specific or general scientific knowledge, they might turn to pseudoscience because they ignore the importance of science to improve their professional practices. Indeed, the importance of knowledge published in peer-reviewed scientific journals comes not only from the first critical evaluation by researchers with scientific expertise in the subject, but also from the subsequent critical evaluation of members of the international scientific community. In other words, organizations essentially have two choices: (i) scientific knowledge that, though fallible, can be supporter criticizeecause everything is accessible to so, or (ii) pseudoscientific claims which these organizations must largely trust blindly. Without knowing the merits of the knowledge published in peer-reviewed scientific journals, pseudoscience can appear much more enticing and reassuring by implementing marketing campaigns and using logical fallacies (e.g., calls for authority; Shermer, 2002).
For example, to flaunt the merits of synergology, the following statement was used: “Who are the synergologists?; Who are the participants, the clients who use synergology?; Doctors, neuropsychologists, psychologists, pharmacists, investigators, economic fraud experts, high security special agents, lawyers, social workers, caregivers, business leaders, directors, interviewers. Any other users?; Judges, lawyers, public relations officers from various backgrounds and sn” (Gagnon, 2015, our translation). However, as when the creators of the Reid technique state that more than 500,000 people were trained (John E. Reid & Associates, n.d.b, n.d.c), such a statement cannot make up for the lack of scientific evidence.
The Underestimation of the Dangers of Pseudoscience
Fourth, some organizations in the fields of security and justice have probably turned to pseudoscience because they underestimate the disadvantages (anverestimate the advantages) of using programs, methods, and approaches that, on the surface, seem scientific but, in reality, are not. Beyond the fact these organizations may overlook truly effective means because they pay attention to pseudoscientific claims, the use of dubious concepts regarding nonverbal communication can result in (i) failing tetect actual threats and misidentifying guilty individuals as innocent, (ii) misidentifying innocent individuals as guilty, and (iii) a loss of valuable time and money as well as reputational, legal, and social consequences.
Unless they have unlimited resources, it seems unwise for these organizations to invest time and money in learning and using concepts that have not been the subject of peer-reviewed articles. In addition, if some security and justice organizations are turning to pseudoscience, their reputation coule damaged, especially if it becomes public, and more so if people or other organizations (with whom they dusiness) assume that best professional practices are being used. The legal and social consequences are ness significant. When they turn to pseudoscience, these organizations are exposed to risks, such as when physicians omit to consult research findings in scientific journals and treat their patients with programs, methods, and approaches that have not been scientifically supported. In other words, when they are sueecause their patients have suffered harm that could have been preventey using evidence-based practices, physicians may find themselves in an unfortunate position (Cohen & Eisenber, 2012; Cohen & Kemper, 2005; Foster, Schwartz, & DeRenzo, 2002). First developed in medicine in the 1980s (Thoma & Eaves, 2015), evidence-based practices have since reached the fielf psychosocial intervention (Eyberg, Nelson, & Boggs, 2008; Okpych & Yu, 2014) and gained growing popularity among security and justice professionals (Lum & Koper, 2015; Sherman, 2013). Because their mission is to ensure the protection of the public, professional governing bodies (including those outside the health sector) should therefore ensure that their members rely on evidence-based practices.
The Accountability of Researchers
Finally, when organizations in the fields of security and justice have unrealistic expectations stemming from television series anther popular media, and turn to pseudoscience, part of the responsibility lies with the international scientific community (Colwell, Miller, Miller, & Lyons, 2006; Denault & Jupe, 2017). Indeed, “the scientific process doesn’t stop when results are published in a peer-reviewed journal. Wider communication is also involved, and that includes ensuring not only that information (including uncertainties) is understood, but also that misinformation and errors are corrected where necessary” (Williamson, 2016, p. 171).
In other words, the international scientific community must promote more open access to knowledge published in scientific journals by disseminating it to the general public, as well as trganizations in the fields of security and justice that wish to implement evidence-based practices (Freckelton, 2016). More broadly, academics should also promote the importance of science, explain the strengths (animitations) of peer-reviewed knowledge, and seek to provide accessible and convenient (and scientifically supported) tools to meet the needs of professionals.
Conclusion
The objective of this article was to examine (i) concepts of nonverbal communication conveyey programs, methods, and approaches that fail to reflect the state of science, but also (ii) the consequences of their use by security and justice professionals. To achieve this objective, we described the scope of scientific research on nonverbal communication and examined a program, a method, and an approach that each run counter to the state of science. Finally, we outlined five hypotheses to explain why some organizations in the fields of security and justice are turning to pseudoscience and pseudoscientific techniques. These organizations (and their employees) may be acting in good faith, perhaps believing they are using the best professional practices. Good faith, however, is not sufficient for good practice. For example, SPOT created “an unacceptable risk of racial and religious profiling” (ACLU, 2017, p. 1), the BAI increases the risk of innocent people (especially juveniles anther vulnerable persons) making false confessions, and synergology coulistort the outcome of trials and important decisions made by professionals in positions of trust or authority.
It shoule noted that not all aspects of SPOT, the BAI, and synergology are incorrect. However, the use of some evidence published in scientific journals (embedded among a host of pseudoscientific claims) tegitimize programs, methods, and approaches that are not scientifically supported is a typical feature of pseudoscience. In other words, proponents of these programs, methods, and approaches can categorically reject the overwhelming weight of a vast literature that runs counter to their views, and select a few peer-reviewed articles that support them (Blancke, Boudry, & Pigliucci, 2017; Denault et al., 2015). For example, synergologists mobilize knowledge from laboratory experiments, including on issues related to emotional expressions and investigative interviews (e.g., Gagnon, n.d.a; Story, 2018; Turchet, 2009, 2012, 2013), while the founder of synergology stated that “what we absolutely do not believe in within synergology is experiment, because body language is made in such a way that when we participate in an experiment, it does not work” (Institut Européen de Synergologie, 2015, our translation; see also Jarry, 2016).
Although the dangers of pseudoscience in security and justice contexts are undeniable, security and justice organizations woule mistaken to reject anything that relates to nonverbal communication on the grounds that it is not always easy tistinguish scientific knowledge from pseudoscientific claims. In fact, the thousands of peer-reviewed articles on nonverbal communication are important sources of knowledge for security and justice professionals (Burgoon et al., 2010; Granhag & Strömwall, 2004; Granhag et al., 2015; Knapp et al., 2014; Moore et al., 2014; Patterson, 2011; Vrij, 2008). In addition, the usefulness of an individual’s nonverbaehaviors goes far beyond the detection of lies or mal-intent.
For example, in recent years, the fielf study of thin slices of expressive behavior (e.g., using videos of a few seconds without sound; Weisbuch & Ambady, 2011) has developed in an attempt to understand experiential knowledge that results from intuition and that has a considerable impact on rapid judgments. This fielf study has alseveloped in an attempt to increase the ability to accurately observe and interpret nonverbaehaviors, including judging an individual’s personality or propensity to psychopathology (Borkenau, Mauer, Riemann, Spinath, & Angleitner, 2004; Carney, Colvin, & Hall, 2007; Fowler, Lilienfeld, & Patrick, 2009; Oltmanns, Friedman, Fiedler, & Turkheimer, 2004; Stillman, Maner, & Baumeister, 2010). In addition, the usefulness of nonverbal communication in creating a trust relationship between a police officer and a suspect to gather information (Abbe & Brandon, 2013; St-Yves, 2006; Tickle-Degnen & Rosenthal, 2009) and the automated recognition of facial expressions (Mast, Gatica-Perez, Frauendorfer, Nguyen, & Choudhury, 2015) are merely some of the many issues on which peer-reviewed knowledge coulenefit security and justice professionals.
Although some security and justice organizations are still turning to pseudoscience, others have already moved past programs, methods, and approaches that fail to reflect the state of science. In a number of organizations, researchers are already working closely with security and justice professionals to implement evidence-based practices (e.g., Centre for Research and Evidence on Security Threats, United Kingdom; High-Value Detainee Interrogation Group, United States). Therefore, we hope that our article will inspire alrganizations, regardless of the importance they currently attach to scientific research, to reflect more on the dangers of pseudoscience and the importance of science in security and justice contexts. In addition, we hope it will encourage security and justice organizations to start or continue working with the international community of scholars who have scientific expertise in nonverbal communication anie (and truth) detection tevelop evidence-based practices. We also hope that researchers will see our article as an invitation to increase opportunities tisseminate their scientific work, promote the scientific method, and engage with security and justice professionals timit the use of pseudoscience.
Notes
1 This article is a translation of the French article “L’analyse de la communication non verbale: Les dangers de la pseudoscience en contextes de sécurité et de justice” published herein with the authorization of the French journal Revue Internationale de Criminologie et de Police Technique et Scientifique. The initial manuscript of the French article was draftey Vincent Denault with the help of Pierrich Plusquellec, Louise Marie Jupe, Michel St-Yves, and Norah Dunbar. The other authors read and commented the initial manuscript and subsequent versions. All authors approved the final version.
2 The U.S. Government Accountability Office (2017) reported that the TSA revised the list of 94 behavioral indicators in 2014: “According to TSA, most of the 94 behavioral indicators were combined, condensed, or updated for incorporation into a reviseist and a small subset were eliminated” (p. 3).
3 It shoule noted that following the report of the Boston Globe, the TSA curtailed the program: “Agency officials told the Globe that air marshals nonger document the minor movements anehavior of these travelers” (Winter & Abelson, 2018).
4 For example, after having himself completed the 200-plus hours training to “become” a synergologist, the first author severed his links with synergology, transitioned to science by pursuing a master’s degree in law and a doctorate in communication, and published academic texts rigorously criticizing the use of synergology in the justice system (e.g., Denault, 2015; Denault et al., 2015). He was then the target of ad hominem attacks, including insults anisapproving comments on social media (Denault, 2018).
Cite this article as: Denault, V., Plusquellec, P., Jupe, L. M., St-Yves, M., Dunbar, N. E., Hartwig, M., Sporer, S. L., Rioux-Turcotte, J., Jarry, J., Walsh, D., Otgaar, H., Viziteu, A., Talwar, V., Keatley, D. A., Blandón-Gitlin, I., Townson, C., Deslauriers-Varin, N., Lilienfeld, S. O., Patterson, M. L., ... van Koppen, P. J. (2019). The analysis of nonverbal communication: The dangers of pseudoscience in security and justice contexts. Anuarie Psicología Jurídica, 30, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.5093/apj2019a9
Aamodt, M. G., & Custer, H. (2006). Who can best catch a liar?; A meta-analysis of individuaifferences in detecting deception. The Forensic Examiner, 15(1), 6-11.
Abbe, A., & Brandon, S. E. (2013). The role of rapport in investigative interviewing: A review. Journaf Investigative Psychology anffender Profiling, 10, 237-249. https://doi.org/10.1002/jip.1386
ACLU. (2017). Bad trip: Debunking the TSA’s ‘behavior detection’ program. Retrieved from https://www.aclu.org/report/bad-trip-debunking-tsas-behavior-detection-program
American Psychological Association. (2017). Summary report of journaperations, 2016. American Psychologist, 72, 499-500. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000172
Asendorpf, J. B., Conner, M., De Fruyt, F., De Houwer, J., Denissen, J. J. A., Fiedler, K., … Wicherts, J. M. (2013). Recommendations for increasing replicability in psychology. European Journaf Personality, 27, 108-119. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1919
Association Européenne de Synergologie. (n.d.). Code de déontologie de la synergology "Code of ethics of synergology". Retrieved from https://synergologie-aes.eu/code-ethique-de-la-synergologie/
Axelrad, B. (2012). Quane corps dit tout haut ce que l’esprit pense tout bas "When the body says out loud what the mind is thinking". Retrieved from http://www.pseudo-sciences.org/spip.php?;article1911
Bagoe, F. (2015). Tous les non-dits sont des mensonges "All the unsaids are lies". Repéré à http://www.ds2c.fr/blog/tousles-non-dits-sont-des-mensonges.html
Barreau du Québec. (n.d.a). Le langage corporel I: Décoder ce qu’on ne dit pas "Body language I: Decoding what we don’t say". Retrieved from http://webpro.barreau.qc.ca/le-langage-corporel.html (retrieven July 10, 2013).
Barreau du Québec. (n.d.b). Le langage corporel II: Maîtriser l’art de l’interrogatoire "Body language II: Mastering the art of interviewing". Retrieved from http://webpro.barreau.qc.ca/le-langage-corporel-2.html (retrieven July 10, 2013).
Bayley, D., & Weisburd, D. (2009). Cops and spooks: The role of police in counterterrorism. In D. Weisburd, T. E. Feucht, I. Hakimi, L. F. Mock, & S. Perry (Eds.), To protect and to serve: Policing in an age of terrorism (p. 81-99). New York, NY: Springer.
Bensley. D. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2017). Psychological misconceptions: Recent scientific advances and unresolved issues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 377-382. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417699026
Bensley, D. A., Lilienfeld, S., & Powell, L. A. (2014). A new measure of psychological misconceptions: Relations with academic background, critical thinking, and acceptance of paranormal and pseudoscientific claims. Learning and Individuaifferences, 36, 9-18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.07.009
Blair, J. P., & Kooi, B. (2004). The gap between training and research in the detection of deception. International Journaf Police Science and Management, 6, 77-83. https://doi.org/10.1350/ijps.6.2.77.34465
Blancke, S., Boudry, M., & Pigliucci, M. (2017). Why do irrationaeliefs mimic science?; The cultural evolution of pseudoscience. Theoria, 83, 78-97. https://doi.org/10.1111/theo.12109
Bogaard, G., Meijer, E. H., Vrij, A., & Merckelbach, H. (2016). Strong, but wrong: Lay people’s and police officers’ beliefs about verbal and nonverbal cues teception. Plos One, 11(6). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0156615
Bond, C. F., Jr., & DePaulo, B. M. (2006). Accuracy of deception judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 214-234. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2
Bond, C. F., Jr., Howard, A. R., Hutchison, J. L., & Masip, J. (2013). Overlooking the obvious: Incentives tie. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35, 212-221. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2013.764302
Boston Globe (2018). Welcome to the quiet skies. Retrieved from http://apps.bostonglobe.com/news/nation/graphics/2018/07/tsa-quiet-skies/
Boudry, M., Blancke, S., & Pigliucci, M. (2015). What makes weireliefs thrive?; The epidemiology of pseudoscience. Philosophical Psychology, 28, 1177-1198. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2014.971946
Borkenau, P., Mauer, N., Riemann, R., Spinath, F. M., & Angleitner, A. (2004). Thin slices of behavior as cues of personality and intelligence. Journaf Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 599-614. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.4.599
Bronstein, M. V., Pennycook, G., Bear, A., Rand, G., & Cannon, T. D., (2018). Belief in fake news is associated with delusionality, dogmatism, religious fundamentalism, and reduced analytic thinking. Journaf Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.09.005
Bunard, S. (2018). Vos gestes disent tout haut ce que vous pensez tout bas "Your gestures say out loud what you are thinking". Linselles, France: Sobook.
Burgoon, J. K., Guerrero, L. K., & Floyd, K. (2010). Nonverbal communication. Boston, MA: Pearson.
Carney, D. R., Colvin, C. R., & Hall, J. A. (2007). A thin slice perspective on the accuracy of first impressions. Journaf Research in Personality, 41, 1054-1072. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.01.004
Chin, J.M., & Workewych, L. (2016). The CSI effect. In M. Dubber (Ed.), Oxford handbooks online. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Cohen, M. H., & Eisenberg, D. M. (2012). Potential physician malpractice liability associated with complementary and integrative medical therapies. Annals of Internal Medicine, 136, 596-603. https://doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-136-8-200204160-00009
Cohen, M. H., & Kemper, K. J. (2005). Complementary therapies in pediatrics: A legal perspective. Pediatrics, 115, 774-780. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2004-1093
Collignon, R. (2012). Synergologie: interview de Christine Gagnon, experte du domaine "Synergology: Interview with Christine Gagnon, expert in the field". Décodeur du nonverbal. Retrieved from http://www.decodeur dunonverbal.fr/christine-gagnon/ (retrieven November 25, 2012).
Cellule SCAN. (n.d.). Devenir SCAN "Become SCAN". (retrieven November 25, 2012). https://cellulescan.com/devenir-scan-formateur/
Colwell, L. H., Miller, H. A., Miller, R. S., & Lyons, P. M. (2006). US police officers’ knowledge regarding behaviors indicative of deception: Implications for eradicating erroneous beliefs through training. Psychology, Crime & Law, 12, 489-503. https://doi.org/10.1080/10683160500254839
Committee on Science, Space, and Technology. (2011). Behavioral science and security: Evaluating TSA’s SPOT program. Retrieved from https://www.govinfo.gov/content/pkg/CHRG-112hhrg65053/pdf/CHRG-112hhrg65053.pdf
Denault, V. (2015). Communication non verbale et crédibilité des témoins "Nonverbal communication and the credibility of witnesses". Cowansville, Montreal: Yvon Blais.
Denault, V. (2017). Le «langage» non verbaes témoins, quanes pseudosciences s’invitent au tribunal "The “body language” of witnesses, when pseudosciences are invited in the courtroom". ScriptUM: La revue du Colloque VocUM 2015, 2, 96-118.
Denault, V. (2018). Developing critical thinking in a worlf irrationaeliefs. Autoethnographic perspectives from a former advocate of pseudoscience on transitioning to evidence-based academia. Manuscript in preparation.
Denault, V., & Dunbar, N. (2017). Nonverbal communication in courtrooms: Scientific assessments or modern trials by ordeal?; The Advocates’ Quarterly, 47, 280-308.
Denault, V., & Jupe, L. M. (2017). Justice at risk! An evaluation of a pseudoscientific analysis of a witness’ nonverbaehavior in the courtroom. Journaf Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 29, 221-242. https://doi.org/10.1080/14789949.2017.1358758
Denault, V., Larivée, S., Plouffe, D., & Plusquellec, P. (2015). La synergologie, une lecture pseudoscientifique du langage corporel "Synergology, a pseudoscientific reading of body language". Revue de psychoéducation, 43, 425-455. https://doi.org/10.7202/1039262ar
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues teception. Psychologicaulletin, 129, 74-112. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74
Eyberg, S. M., Nelson, M. M., & Boggs, S. R. (2008). Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children and adolescents with disruptive behavior. Journaf Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37, 215-237. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410701820117
Foster, H. P., Schwartz, J., & DeRenzo, E. (2002). Reducing legal risk by practicing patient-centered medicine. Archive of Internal Medicine, 162, 1217-1219. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinte.162.11.1217
Fowler, K. A., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Patrick, C. J. (2009). Detecting psychopathy from thin slices of behavior. Psychological Assessment, 21(2), 68-78. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014938
Freckelton, I. (2016). Scholarly misconduct: Law, regulation and practice. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Gagnon, C. (n.d.a). Formation groupe élite "Elite group training". Retrieved from https://www.christinegagnon.ca/fr/domaine-de-la-securite/formation-groupe-elite
Gagnon, C. (n.d.b). Qu’est-ce que la synergologie?; "What is synergology?;" Retrieved from http://www.christinegagnon.ca/fr/synergologie/qu-est-ce-que-la-synergologie (retrieven September 4, 2018).
Gagnon, C. (2015). La synergologie est là pour rester "Synergology is here to stay". https://www.christinegagnon.ca/fr/blogue/la-synergologie-est-la-pour-rester
Gagnon, C. (2018). Les agents «dernier cri»: lire la menace "The “cutting-edge” agents: Read the threat". Retrieved from https://www.christinegagnon.ca/fr/blogue/scan-les-agents-dernier-cri-lire-la-menace
Gagnon, C., & Martineau, C. (2010). Voir mentir "Seeing a lie". Le Gardeur: Éditions Propulsion.
Gambrill, E. (2005). Critical thinking in clinical practice: Improving the quality of judgments anecisions. New York, NY: Wiley.
Garrido, E., Masip, J., & Herrero, C. (2004). Police officers’ credibility judgments: Accuracy and estimated ability. International Journaf Psychology, 39, 254-275. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000411
Gauchat, G. (2012). Politicization of science in the public sphere: A study of public trust in the United States, 1974 to 2010. American Sociological Review, 77, 167-187. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122412438225
Granhag, P. A., & Strömwall, L. A. (Eds.) (2004). The detection of deception in forensic contexts. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Granhag, P. A., Vrij, A., & Verschuere, B. (Eds.) (2015). Detecting deception: Current challenges and cognitive approaches. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Gudjonsson, G. H. (2014). Vulnérabilités mentales et fausses confessions "Mental vulnerabilities and false confessions". In M. St-Yves (Ed.), Les entrevues d’enquête: L’essentiel (pp. 199-236). Cowansville, Montreal: Yvon Blais.
Hall, J. A., & Knapp, M. L. (Eds.) (2013). Nonverbal communication. Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.
Harrigan, J. A. (2005). Proxemics, kinesics and gaze. In J. A. Harrigan, R. Rosenthal & K. R. Scherer (Eds.). The new handbook of methods in nonverbaehavior research (pp. 137-198). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Harrigan, J. A., Rosenthal, R., & Scherer, K. R. (2005). The new handbook of methods in nonverbaehavior research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Hartwig, M., & Bond, C. F., Jr. (2011). Why die-catchers fail?; A lens model meta-analysis of human lie judgments. Psychologicaulletin, 137, 643-659. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023589
Hartwig, M., & Bond, C. F., Jr. (2014). Lie detection from multiple cues: A meta-analysis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 661-676. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3052
Hauch, V., Sporer, S. L., Michael, S. W., & Meissner, C. A. (2016). Does training improve detection of deception?; A meta-analysis. Communication Monographs, 43, 283-343. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650214534974
Horgan, A. J., Russano, M. B., Meissner, C. A., & Evans, J. R. (2012). Minimization and maximization techniques: Assessing the perceived consequences of confessing and confession diagnosticity. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 18, 65-78. https://doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2011.561801
Horvath, F., Jayne, B., & Buckley, J. (1994). Differentiation of truthful aneceptive criminal suspects in behavior analysis interviews. Journaf Forensic Sciences, 39, 793-807. https://doi.org/10.1520/JFS13657J
Howard, P. (2004). Hard won lessons: How police fight terrorism in the United Kingdom. New York, NY: Manhattan Institute for Policy Research.
Inbau, F. E., Reid, J. E., Buckley, J. P., & Jayne, B. C. (2013). Criminal interrogation and confessions. Sudbury, MA: Jones anartlett Publishers.
Institut Européen de Synergologie. (2015, November 7). Info Synergo!!! "Online video". Retrieved from https://www.facebook.com/InstitutEuropeenDeSynergologie/videos/598476310301856/ (retrieven July 28, 2017)
Institut Québécois de Synergologie. (2016). Dire n’importe quoi "To say anything". Retrieved from http://www.institutquebecoisdesynergologie.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/04/Dire-nimporte-quoi_Final.pdf
Institut Québécois de Synergologie. (2018). Qu’est-ce que la Synergologie?; "What is synergology?;" Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?;v=79AAVIf7-Fg
Institut Québécois de Synergologie. (n.d.). Programme "Program". Retrieved from http://www.institutquebecoisdesynergologie.com/programme/
Jarry, J. (2016, May 21). Vlog 10: Lie to me, synergology "Online video". Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?;v=F2kvuLG_57c
Jarry, J. (2018, January 12). Lies and nonverbal communication "Online video". Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?;v=qIgwbE7XUC8
John E. Reid & Associates. (n.d.a). Behavior analysis interview. Retrieved from http://www.reid.com/services/r_behavior.html
John E. Reid & Associates. (n.d.b). Interviewing and interrogation. Retrieved from http://www.reid.com/training_programs/interview_overview.html
John E. Reid & Associates. (n.d.c). Training programs. Retrieved from http://www.reid.com/training_programs/r_training.html
Jupe, L. M., & Denault, V. (2018). Science or pseudoscience?; A distinction that matters for police officers, lawyers and judges. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Kassin, S. (2015). The social psychology of false confession. Social Issues and Policy Review, 9(1), 25-51. https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12009
Kassin, S. M., & Fong, C. T. (1999). I’m innocent! Effects of training on judgments of truth aneception in the interrogation room. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 499-516. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022330011811
Kassin, S. M., & Gudjonsson, G. H. (2004). The psychology of confessions: A review of the literature and issues. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, 33-67. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2004.00016.x
Kassin, S. M., & Sukel, H. (1997). Coerced confessions and the jury: An experimental test of the “harmless error” rule. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 27-46. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024814009769
Knapp, M. L., Hall, J. A., & Horgan, T. G. (2014). Nonverbal communication in human interaction. Boston, MA: Wadsworth.
Kozinski, A. (2015). Criminaaw 2.0. Georgetown Law Review, 44, iii-xliv.
LaFree, G., & Freilich, J. D. (2018). Government policies for counteracting violent extremism. Annual Review of Criminology, 2, 383-404. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-criminol-011518-024542
Lagacé, P. (2015). La caution des organisations "The endorsement of the organizations". Retrieved from http://plus.lapresse.ca/screens/da1d760d-953b-43fa-b350-2f8f1c25c8a2__7C__pk7AaYX07uQi.html
Lardellier, P. (2008). Pour en finir avec la «synergologie»: Une analyse critique d’une pseudoscience du «décodage du non-verbal» "To put an end to “synergology”: A critical analysis of a pseudoscience of “non-verbaecoding”". Communication, 26, 197-223.
Lardellier, P. (2017). Enquête sur le business de la communication non verbale: Une analyse critique des pseudosciences du «langage corporel» "Investigation into the nonverbal communication business: A critical analysis of the pseudosciences of “body language”". Cormelles-le-Royal, France: Éditions Management & Société.
Levine, T. R., Serota, K. B., & Shulman, H. C. (2010). The impact of Lie to Me on viewers’ actual ability tetect deception. Communication Research, 37, 847-856. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650210362686
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Landfield, K. (2008). Science and pseudoscience in law enforcement: A user-friendly primer. Criminal Justice anehavior, 35, 1215-1230. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854808321526
Loranger, J., & Loranger, J. (2019). L’évaluation de la scientificité et le mythe des pseudosciences "The evaluation of scientificity and the myth of pseudosciences". Repentigny, Quebec: Les Éditions Propulsion.
Lum, C., & Koper, C. S. (2015). Evidence-based policing. In R. Dunham & G. Alperts (Eds.), Critical issues in policing (pp. 1-12). Longrove, IL: Waveland Press.
Majima, Y. (2015). Belief in pseudoscience, cognitive style and science literacy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29, 552-559. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3136
Mann, H., Garcia-Rada, X., Houser, D., & Ariely, D. (2014). Everybody else is doing it: Exploring social transmission of lying behavior. Plos One, 9(10). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0109591
Mann, S., Vrij, A., & Bull, R. (2002). Suspects, lies and videotape: An analysis of authentic high-stake liars. Law and Human Behaviour, 26, 365-376. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015332606792
Mann, S., Vrij, A., & Bull, R. (2004). Detecting true lies: Police officers’ ability tetect suspects’ lies. Journaf Applied Psychology, 89, 137-149. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.89.1.137
Manusov, V., & Patterson, M. L. (Eds.) (2006). The Sage handbook of nonverbal communication. Thousanaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Masip, J. (2017). Deception detection: State of the art and future prospects. Psicothema, 29, 149-159. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2017.34
Masip, J., Barba, A., & Herrero, C. (2012). Behavior analysis interview and common sense. A study with novice and experiencefficers. Psychiatry, Psychology anaw, 19, 21-34. https://doi.org/10.1080/13218719.2010.543402
Masip, J., & Herrero, C. (2013). “What would you say if you were guilty?;” Suspects’ strategies during a hypotheticaehavior analysis interview concerning a serious crime. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 60-70. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2872
Masip, J., Herrero, C., Garrido, E., & Barba, A. (2011). Is the behavior analysis interview just common sense?; Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 593-604. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1728
Mast, M. S., Gatica-Perez, D., Frauendorfer, D., Nguyen, L., & Choudhury, T. (2015). Social sensing for psychology automated interpersonaehavior assessment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 154-160. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414560811
Matsumoto, M., Hwang, H. C., & Frank, M. G. (Eds.) (2016). APA handbook of nonverbal communication. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Meissner, C. A., & Kassin, S. M. (2002). He’s guilty! Investigator bias in judgments of truth aneception. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 469-480. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020278620751
Monnin, C. (2009). Impact de la communication voco-visuelle dans le management sur la motivation des collaborateurs "Impact of vocal-visual communication in management on employee motivation" (unpublisheoctoraissertation). École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Suisse.
Moore, N.-J., Hickson, M., & Stacks, D. W. (2014). Nonverbal communication: Studies and applications. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Nisbet, E. C., Cooper, K. E., & Garrett, R. K. (2015). The partisan brain: How dissonant science messages lead conservatives aniberals to (dis)trust science. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 658(1), 36-66. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716214555474
Office of Inspector General (2016). Verification review of transportation security administration’s screening of passengers by observation techniques/behavior detection and analysis program. Retrieved from https://www.oig.dhs.gov/assets/VR/FY16/OIG-16-111-VR-Jul16.pdf
Okpych, N. J., & Yu, J. L.-H. (2014). A historical analysis of evidence-based practice in social work: The unfinished journey toward an empirically grounded profession. Social Service Review 88, 3-58. https://doi.org/10.1086/674969
Oltmanns, T. F., Friedman, J. N. W., Fiedler, E. R., & Turkheimer, E. (2004). Perceptions of people with personality disorders basen thin slices of behavior. Journaf Research in Personality, 38, 216-229. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00066-7
Ormerod, T. C., & Dando, C. J. (2015). Finding a needle in a haystack: Toward a psychologically informed method for aviation security screening. Journaf Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 76-84. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000030
Patterson, M. L. (2011). More than words: The power of nonverbal communication: Barcelona, España: Editorial Aresta.
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2015). On the reception anetection of pseudo-profounullshit. Judgment anecision Making, 10, 549-563.
Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G., (2018). Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explainey lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.011
Plusquellec, P., & Denault, V. (2018). The 1000 most cited papers on visible nonverbaehavior: A bibliometric analysis. Journaf Nonverbaehavior, 42, 347-377. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-018-0280-9
Porter, S., & ten Brinke, L. (2009). Dangerous decisions: A theoretical framework for understanding how judges assess credibility in the courtroom. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 14, 119-134. https://doi.org/10.1348/135532508X281520
Reinhard, M.-A., Sporer, S. L., & Scharmach, M. (2013). Perceived familiarity with a judgmental situation improves lie detection ability. Swiss Journaf Psychology, 72(1), 53-61. https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000098
Reinhard, M.-A., Sporer, S. L., Scharmach, M., & Marksteiner, T. (2011). Listening, not watching: Situational familiarity and the ability tetect deception. Journaf Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 467-484. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023726
Rochat, N., Delmas, H., Denault, V., Elissalde, B., & Demarchi, S. (2018). La synergologie révisée par les pairs, analyse d’une publication "Peer-reviewed synergology, analysis of a publication". Revue québécoise de psychologie, 39, 247-266. https://doi.org/10.7202/1051231ar
Russano, M. B., Meissner, C. A., Narchet, F. M., & Kassin, S. M. (2005). Investigating true and false confessions within a novel experimental paradigm. Psychological Science, 16, 481-486. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01560.x
Shen, F. X., & Gromet, D. M. (2015). Red states, blue states, anrain states: Issue framing, partisanship, and the future of neurolaw in the United States. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 658(1), 86-101. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716214555693
Sherman, L. W. (2013). The rise of evidence-based policing: Targeting, testing, and tracking. Crime and Justice, 42, 377-451. https://doi.org/10.1086/670819
Shermer, M. (2002). Why people believe in weird things. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company.
Shipman, M. (2014). The limitations of social research (4th ed.). London, UK: Routledge.
Snook, B., Eastwood, J., & Barron, W. T. (2014). The next stage in the evolution of interrogations: The PEACE model. Canadian Criminaaw Review, 18, 219-239.
Sporer, S. L., & Schwandt, B. (2006). Paraverbal indicators of deception: A meta-analytic synthesis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 421-446. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1190
Sporer, S. L., & Schwandt, B. (2007). Moderators of nonverbal indicators of deception: A meta-analytic synthesis. Psychology, Public Policy, anaw, 13(1), 1-34. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.13.1.1
Stillman, T. F., Maner, J. K., & Baumeister, R. F. (2010). A thin slice of violence: Distinguishing violent from nonviolent sex offenders at a glance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 298-303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.12.001
Story, M. (2018). Au-delà des mots: Guide de la communication non verbal "Beyond words: A guide to nonverbal communication". Paris, France: Maxima.
St-Yves, M. (2006). Psychology of rapport: Five basic rules. In T. Williamson (Ed.), Investigative interviewing: Rights, research, regulation (pp. 87-106). Portland, OR: Willan Publishing.
St-Yves, M., & Meissner, C. A. (2014). L’entrevue de suspects "The interview of suspects". In M. St-Yves (Ed.), Les entrevues d’enquête: L’essentiel (pp. 153-198). Cowansville, Montreal: Yvon Blais.
Sweet, D. M., Meissner, C. A., & Atkinson, D. J. (2017). Assessing law enforcement performance in behavior-based threat detection tasks involving a concealed weapon or device. Law and Human Behavior, 41, 411-421. https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000243
Synergology, the Official Website. (n.d.a). Décrypter le langage corporel avec la discipline synergologie "Decipher body language with the discipline of synergology". Retrieved from http://non-verbal.synergologie.org/nonverbal/synergologie
Synergology, the Official Website. (n.d.b). La Synergologie, discipline scientifique de lecture des gestes "Synergology, a scientific discipline for reading gestures". Retrieved from http://non-verbal.synergologie.org/nonverbal/synergologie/la-perspective-synergologique
Synergologie, the Official Website. (n.d.c). Le Synergologue analyse et interprète la communication non verbale: Savoir décrypter le mensonge en observant votre interlocuteur "The Synergologist analyses and interprets non-verbal communication: Knowing how tecipher lies by observing your interlocutor". Retrieved from http://formation.synergologie.org/formations/qui-se-forme-a-la-synergologie/les-metiers-de-la-justice
Synergology, the Official Website. (n.d.d). Les centres de formation officiels "The official training centres". Retrieved from http://www.synergologie.org/sites-partenaires
Synergology, the Official Website. (n.d.e). Page d’accueil "Home page". Retrieved from http://www.synergologie.org/
The Globaeception Research Team. (2006). A worlf lies. Journaf Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37(1), 60-74. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022105282295
The Intercept. (2015). Exclusive: TSA’s secret behavior checklist to spot terrorists. Retrieved from https://theintercept.com/2015/03/27/revealed-tsas-closely-held-behavior-checklist-spot-terrorists/
Tickle-Degnen, L., & Rosenthal, R. (2009). The nature of rapport and its nonverbal correlates. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 324-329. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0104_1
Thoma, A., & Eaves, F. F. (2015). A brief history of evidence-based medicine (EBM) and the contributions of Dr David Sackett. Aesthetic Surgery Journal, 35, 261-263. https://doi.org/10.1093/asj/sjv130
Turchet, P. (2004). La synergologie "Synergology". Montréal, Québec: Les Éditions de l’Homme.
Turchet, P. (2009). Le langage universeu corps "The universaody language". Montréal, Québec: Éditions de l’Homme.
Turchet, P. (2010). Lie to me, synergologie et émotions "Lie to me, synergology and emotions". Retrieved from https://philippe.turchet.synergologie.org/2010/05/14/lie-to-mesynergologie-et-emotions/
Turchet, P. (2012). The secrets of body language: An illustrated guide to knowing what people are really thinking and feeling. New York, NY: Skyhorse Publishing.
Turchet, P. (2013). Langue maternelle et langue seconde: approche par l’observation gestuelle "Native language and seconanguage: approach through gesture observation". Langages, 192(4), 29-43. https://doi.org/10.3917/lang.192.0029
Turchet, P. (2017). Identification de ruptures de compréhension dialogique en contexte interculturel à partir d’indices corporels "Identification of breaks in dialogical understanding in an intercultural context basen bodily cues" (Thèse de doctorat). Université Paris Nanterre, France. Retrieved from https://bdr.parisnanterre.fr/theses/internet/2017/2017PA100174/2017PA100174.pdf
U.S. Government Accountability Office. (2010). Aviation security: Efforts to validate TSA’s passenger screening behavior detection program underway, but opportunities exist to strengthen validation and address operational challenges. Retrieved from https://www.gao.gov/products/GAO-10-763
U.S. Government Accountability Office. (2011). TSA is taking steps to validate the science underlying its passenger behavior detection program, but efforts may not be comprehensive. Retrieved from https://www.gao.gov/assets/130/125918.pdf
U.S. Government Accountability Office. (2012). Progress and challenges faced in strengthening three key security programs. Retrieved from https://www.gao.gov/assets/590/589587.pdf
U.S. Government Accountability Office. (2013). Aviation security: TSA shoulimit future funding for behavior detection activities. Retrieved from https://www.gao.gov/products/GAO-14-159
U.S. Government Accountability Office. (2017). Aviation security: TSA does not have valid evidence supporting most of the reviseehavioral indicators used in its behavior detection activities. Retrieved from https://www.gao.gov/products/GAO-17-608R
Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies aneceit: Pitfalls anpportunities. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Vrij, A., & Fisher, R. P. (2016). Which lie detection tools are ready for use in the criminal justice system?; Journaf Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5, 302-307. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.06.014
Vrij, A., Granhag, P. A., & Porter, S. (2010). Pitfalls anpportunities in nonverbal and verbaie detection. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 11, 89-121. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100610390861
Vrij, A., Hartwig, M., & Granhag, P. A. (2019). Reading lies: Nonverbal communication aneception. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 295-317. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103135
Vrij, A., Mann, S., & Fisher, R. (2006). An empirical test of the Behaviour Analysis Interview. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 329-345. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9014-3
Vrij, A., Mann, S., Jundi, S., Hillman, J., & Hope, L. (2014). Detection of concealment in an information-gathering interview. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 860-866. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3051
Vrij, A., Meissner, C. A., Fisher, R. P., Kassin, S. M., Morgan III, C. A., & Kleinman, S. M. (2017). Psychological perspective on interrogation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 927-955. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617706515
Walton, D. N. (1987). The ad hominem argument as an informal fallacy. Argumentation, 1, 317-331. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00136781
Ware, M. (2008). Peer review: benefits, perceptions and alternatives. Publishing Research Consortium, 20. Retrieved from http://publishingresearch.org.uk
Weisbuch, M., & Ambady, N. (2011). Thin-slice vision. In R. B. Adams, N. Ambady, K. Nakayama, & S. Shimojo (Eds.), The science of social vision (pp. 228-247). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Williamson, P. (2016). Take the time and effort to correct misinformation. Nature, 540(7632), 171. https://doi.org/10.1038/540171a
Winter, J. (2015). Exclusive: TSA’s ‘behavior detection’ program targeting undocumented immigrants, not terrorists. Retrieved from https://theintercept.com/2015/04/06/exclusive-tsa-behavior-detection-program-targeting-immigrants-terrorists/
Winter, J., & Abelson, J. (2018). TSA says it nonger tracks regular travelers as if they may be terrorists. Retrieved from https://www.bostonglobe.com/news/nation/2018/12/15/curtails-quiet-skies-passenger-surveillance/2lRAv2AwjGpUcgq08mHaPM/story.html
Vincent Denault1, 2, Pierrich Plusquellec2, 3, 4, Louise M. Jupe5, Michel St-Yves6, 7, Norah E. Dunbar8, Maria Hartwig9, Siegfried L. Sporer10, Jessica Rioux-Turcotte11, Jonathan Jarry12, Dave Walsh13, Henry Otgaar14, 15, Andrei Viziteu5, Victoria Talwar16, David A. Keatley17, Iris Blandón-Gitlin18, Clint Townson19, Nadine Deslauriers-Varin20, Scott O. Lilienfeld21, 45, Miles L. Patterson22, Igor Areh23, Alfred Allan24, Hilary Evans Cameron25, 26, Rémi Boivin7, Leanne ten Brinke27, Jaume Masip28, Ray Bull29, Mireille Cyr30, Lorraine Hope5, Leif A. Strömwall31, Stephanie J. Bennett32, Faisal Al Menaiya5, Richard A. Leo33, Annelies Vredeveldt34, Marty Laforest11, Charles R. Honts35, Antonio L. Manzanero36, Samantha Mann5, Pär-Anders Granhag31, Karl Ask31, Fiona Gabbert37, Jean-Pierre Guay7, Alexandre Coutant38, Jeffrey Hancock39, Valerie Manusov40, Judee K. Burgoon41, Steven M. Kleinman42, Gordon Wright37, Sara Landström31, Ian Freckelton43, Zarah Vernham5, Peter J. van Koppen34, 44
1Département de communication, Université de Montréal, Canada.;
2Centre d’études en sciences de la communication non verbale, Canada.;
3École de psychoéducation, Université de Montréal, Canada.;
4Centre d’études sur le stress humain, Canada.;
5Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, United Kingdom.;
6Sûreté du Québec, Canada.;
7École de criminologie, Université de Montréal, Canada.;
8Department of Communication, University of California, Santa Barbara, United States.;
9Department of Psychology, John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York, United States.;
10Department of Psychology and Sports Science, University of Giessen, Germany.;
11Département de lettres et communication sociale, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Canada.;
12McGill Office for Science and Society, McGill University, Canada.;
13Leicester De Montfort Law School, De Montfort University, United Kingdom.;
14Leuven Institute of Criminology, Catholic University of Leuven, Belgium.;
15Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Forensic Psychology Section, Maastricht University, The Netherlands.;
16Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Canada.;
17School of Law, Murdoch University, Australia.;
18Department of Psychology, California State University, Fullerton, United States.;
19Department of Communication, Michigan State University, United States.;
20École de travail social et de criminologie, Université Laval, Canada.;
21Department of Psychology, Emory University, United States.;
22Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri-St. Louis, United States.;
23Faculty of Criminal Justice and Security, University of Maribor, Slovenia.;
24School Arts and Humanities, Edith Cowan University, Australia.;
25Osgoode Hall Law School, York University, Canada.;
26Trinity College, University of Toronto, Canada.;
27Department of Psychology, University of Denver, United States.;
28Department of Social Psychology and Anthropology, University of Salamanca, Spain.;
29Department of Criminology and Law, University of Derby, United Kingdom.;
30Département de psychologie, Université de Montréal, Canada.;
31Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Sweden.;
32Institute of Criminal Justice Studies, University of Portsmouth, United Kingdom.;
33Hamill Family Professor of Law and Social Psychology, University of San Francisco, United States.;
34Department of Criminal Law and Criminology, VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands.;
35Department of Psychological Science, Boise State University, United States.;
36Department of Experimental Psychology, Complutense University of Madrid, Spain.;
37Forensic Psychology Unit, Goldsmiths University of London, United Kingdom.;
38Département de communication sociale et publique, Université du Québec à Montréal, Canada;
39Department of Communication, Stanford University, United States.;
40Department of Communication, University of Washington, United States.;
41Center for the Management of Information, University of Arizona, United States.;
42Operational Sciences International, United States;
43Law Faculty, University of Melbourne, Australia.;
44Department of Criminal Law and Criminology, Maastricht University, The Netherlands.;
45School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Australia.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2020. This work is licensed under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/deed.es_ES (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
For security and justice professionals (e.g., police officers, lawyers, judges), the thousands of peer-reviewed articles on nonverbal communication represent important sources of knowledge. However, despite the scope of the scientific work carried out on this subject, professionals can turn to programs, methods, and approaches that fail to reflect the state of science. The objective of this article is to examine (i) concepts of nonverbal communication conveyed by these programs, methods, and approaches, but also (ii) the consequences of their use (e.g., on the life or liberty of individuals). To achieve this objective, we describe the scope of scientific research on nonverbal communication. A program (SPOT; Screening of Passengers by Observation Techniques), a method (the BAI; Behavior Analysis Interview) and an approach (synergology) that each run counter to the state of science are examined. Finally, we outline five hypotheses to explain why some organizations in the fields of security and justice are turning to pseudoscience and pseudoscientific techniques. We conclude the article by inviting these organizations to work with the international community of scholars who have scientific expertise in nonverbal communication and lie (and truth) detection to implement evidence-based practices.