Introduction
The European Commission has repeatedly highlighted the fact that the Spanish education system has not reached any of the objectives set for secondary students’ achievement (Valle et al., 2009). A recent report of the National Statistics Institute shows that Spain has managed to reduce the rate of schooropout over the last ten years from 31.7% to 17.3% in 2019. However, Spain is stilne of the EU countries with the highest school failure rate for young people aged 18 to 24 (INE, 2019). These youth leave education having completed, at most, only the first part of secondary education (ESO).
The mathematics subject is an expected concern for the Spanish school system, given the fact that math is involved in a wide range of activities and expertise in modern society. Understanding math is an absolute necessity in a society that is becoming ever more complex and technology-dependent. Nowadays, most sciences, including social sciences such as sociology, psychology, and economics, use more and more mathematical tools. Math is used in sport, dietetics, traffic management, population management etc. However, most academic difficulties and failures are concentrated in math. In fact, math acts as a basic “selective filter” in all educational systems. Failures of learning in math are not limited to the least capable. On the contrary, able students who can achieve high scores in other subjects exhibit poor results in math.
Many students who have difficulties with math perceive learning as threatening experiences in which their school performance wile evaluated and their cognitive competence questioned, which is why avoiding learning opportunities is often what they feel te the best choice. On the other hand, successful students face these episodes as opportunities troaden their range of skills and therefore value them. As a result, successful students normally exhibit higher perceiveearning competence, lower or moderate math anxiety, and good achievement compared tow performing students. These higher beliefs of self-efficacy not only increase motivation for learning, but also the process of self-regulation, making it easier to set ambitious learning objectives and facilitating self-monitoring behavior (Rosário et al., 2014). In contrast, students with low confidence in themselves about learning math tend te more anxious about their learning and avoieaning opportunities when they come up (García et al., 2016).
Although we have learned a lot over the past sixty years about anxiety and its relationship with performance in math tasks (Dowker et al., 2016), there is still significant inconsistency in the results of past research about the role of non-cognitive variables such as anxiety and perceived competence in the explanation of math achievement, as well as hypotheticaifferences due to gender or school year (Ching, 2017; García et al., 2016; Hanin & Nieuwenhoven, 2016; Lipnevich et al., 2016). The data generally comes from studies assessing direct effects or the main effects of these variables on math achievement. This study aims to improve our current understanding by examining indirect and interaction effects in addition to principal effects. We examined how the relationship between Math Self-Efficacy (PMA) and Math Achievement (MATH) may be mediater moderatey levels of Math Anxiety (ANX), considering intellectual abilities, gender, and school year.
Math Self-efficacy and Math Achievement
Self-efficacy refers to a person’s beliefs about their abilities to perform certain tasks and achieve goals (Bandura, 1977). According to this definition, these beliefs are specific to a domain, rather than being a global construct, which means that nowadays, self-efficacy is mainly used in a domain-specific sense (e.g., mathematics, or writing). Chen (2003) looked at beliefs of self-efficacy in 107 seventh grade math students and its relation to math achievement. The results of this study showed that those expectations have a powerful, direct, positive effect on the prediction of student performance in mathematics. Similar results have been found with Portuguese students from fifth- to ninth grade (Rosário et al., 2012), with British adolescents (Tosto et al., 2016), and with ninth- and tenth grade students in North America (Stevens et al., 2004).
Using latent class analysis to relate self-efficacy expectations, self-concept, and math anxiety, Kvedere (2014) found evidence for two classes of students: (1) students with a positive mathematical self (high self-efficacy and self-concept anow math anxiety) and (2) students with a negative mathematical self (low perceived competence, low math self-concept, and high anxiety). In this study, and in others (e.g., Jain & Dowson, 2009), the relationship between perceived competence and math anxiety was negative. In contrast, the relationship between perceived competence and achievement seems te reciprocal (Guay et al., 2003; Marsh & Martin, 2011; Valentine et al., 2004). Finally, various studies have shown that this relationship is moderatey gender. Recently, Henschel and Roick (2017) found that perceived competence interacted with gender in the determination of differences in math anxiety.
Math Anxiety and Math Achievement
Math anxiety is characterizey negative feelings towards math, which results in avoiding math classes and subjects basen math skills (Pizzie & Kraemer, 2017). Although it is usually accepted that the relationship between math anxiety and math achievement is curvilinear (Wang et al., 2015), it is usually approached from a linear perspective: as anxiety about a math task increases, execution worsens (Maloney & Beilock, 2012). In addition, while the levef anxiety is usually taken as a cause of math achievement (the debilitating anxiety model) rather than the reverse (the deficit theory), Carey et al. (2016) opted for a relationship with reciprocal effects (the reciprocal theory), even at very early ages (Gunderson et al., 2017). One recently studied additional topic was whether math anxiety and test or exam anxiety were manifestations of a general form of anxiety or the expression of specific anxieties (Mammarella et al., 2018). Similarly, math anxiety has been found to not affect all children or all types of math achievement equally (Ching, 2017).
Cates and Rhymer (2003) examined the relationship between math anxiety, fluency, and the number of errors made by students in basic math operations. The results showed that the group with the highest math anxiety had considerably lower levels of fluency in alf the basic math operation tests. They also found that the relationship between math anxiety and math performance was more intense in middle-school education than in elementary education. Similar results were founy Ikegulu (2003), finding that students with the lowest levels of anxiety were more academically successful than those with moderate to high levels of anxiety. They also reported that students with high levels of math anxiety were much less interested in working on math or math-related activities. Ikegulu’s (2003) study found that when gender was considered along with anxiety, it explained close to 90% of the variance in the prediction of math performance. Female students perceived themselves as more anxious about math and male students perceived themselves te better in their approach to and success with math. The results from Henschel and Roick (2017) were similar, girls exhibited significantly higher levels of (cognitive and affective) anxiety about math than boys, as and the also exhibited perceptions of being less competent in those tasks.
In summary, a negative, significant relationship has consistently been founetween math anxiety and math performance, from -.27 to -.34 (e.g., Chang & Beilock, 2016; Henschel & Roick, 2017; Ma, 1999; Suárez-Pellicioni et al., 2015). Compared to their less anxious classmates, students with high levels of math anxiety demonstrate lower math self-efficacy and poorer math skills (Ashcraft & Moore, 2009). One curious finding is from Hill et al. (2016), who found that despite girls exhibiting more math anxiety than boys (in secondary school), there were no significant differences between them in math achievement. According to the authors, this might indicate that girls have greater potential for math than boys.
Goals of this Study
Although there are many studies that have aimed to find the most important variables to explain math achievement in adolescent and pre-university students, the data they have produced is not consistent. Taking for granted the importance of real capability (for example, intellectual abilities), in recent years interest has grown in non-cognitive variables such as perceived math ability (PMA) and math anxiety (ANX) (e.g., Pitsia et al., 2017). Although data on the importance of PMA in explaining math achievement (MATH) is more consistent than data about the role of anxiety, it is often data on the direct effect of PMA and ANX on MATH. Data from previous research (eg, Jain & Dowson, 2009; Kvedere, 2014) indicated that perceived competence shows a significant and negative relationship with anxiety towards mathematics (the greater competition perceived, the less anxiety, and vice versa), and that anxiety is negatively related with mathematical performance (e.g., Chang & Beilock, 2016; Henschel & Roick, 2017; Ikegulu, 2003; Ma, 1999; Suárez-Pellicioni et al., 2015). Thus, acknowledging these findings it is possible to antecipate that anxiety plays a mediating role in the relationship between perceived competence and mathematical performance. To the best of our knowledge, no research has analyzed the mediator role of anxiety. Moreover, it is also feasible that anxiety may interfere in this relationship depending on the value of anxiety (that is, the intensity and the sign of the relationship between perceived ability and performance may vary depending on the levels of anxiety). The latter has not been addressey literature.
Therefore, in the current study, in addition tirect effects, we analyze the mediator role (indirect effect) or moderator role (conditional effect) of ANX in the effect of PMA on MATH. In these analyses the effect of real competence (intellectual abilities), gender, and school year wile statistically controlled for, due to their confirmed part in explaining the differences in MATH. Figure 1 represents the mediation and moderation models te examined.
Figure 1 Mediation and Moderation Models of Math Anxiety (ANX) in terms of the Relationship between Perceived Math Ability (PMA) and Math Achievement (MATH).
Bearing in minata from previous research, which is abundant in terms of principal effects, but scarce in terms of mediation or moderation effects of anxiety, we have formulated the following three hypotheses:
H1. The relationship between perceived math ability (PMA) and math achievement (MATH) is positive and statistically significant (the higher the PMA, the higher the MATH).
H2. In addition to the above direct effect, there is a statistically significant indirect effect between PMA and MATH, via math anxiety (ANX) (mediation hypothesis: the higher the PMA, the lower the ANX, and the lower the ANX the higher the MATH).
H3. The relationship between PMA and MATH may be conditioney a student’s levef ANX (moderation hypothesis: the higher the levef anxiety, the weaker the relationship between PMA and MATH).
Method
Participants
A totaf 2,245 Spanish secondary school students from various schools in the north of Spain participated in this study (secondary education in Spain consists of four years of compulsory education, called ESO, followey two years of Bachillerato; in this study the participants were all ESO students). The sample selection procedure was not random. Although schools were initially chosen at random, not all the selected schools agreed to participate. In addition, within the schools, a small number of students declined to participate for various reasons (being absent in one or more of the evaluation sessions, lack of permission from the family etc.). Gender subgroups were generally equivalent in number (1,122 girls, 49.97%, χ2 = 0.000, p > .05), although there were differences in terms of school year: 415 in first year (18.3%), 484 in second year (21.4%), 693 in third year (30.6%), and 671 in fourth year (29.7%), χ2(3) = 100.183, p < .05. However, gender percentages in each school year were practically the same: first year (9.4% girls, 9.2% boys), second year (10.1% girls, 11.3% boys), third year (15% girls, 15.6% boys), and fourth year (15.7 girls, 14% boys). Students with special educational needs and students in special support classes were not included in this study.
Measures and Instruments
Perceived math ability and math anxiety. Perceived math ability and math anxiety were determined from the corresponding subscales in the version of the Inventory of Attitudes towards Mathematics (IAM), adapted from the scale of evaluation of attitudes towards mathematics (FSS; Fennema & Sherman, 1976). It uses a five-point Likert-type scale (1 = completely false, to 5 = completely true). The perceived ability subscale has 5 items (for example, “I believe that I could master even the most difficult math tasks”). Math anxiety is measurey 5 items (for example, “When I work with math my mind goes blank and I can’t think clearly”). In the present study, both scales exhibit good reliability (perceived ability: α = .832, anxiety: α = .836), despite the small number of items.
Real ability. To evaluate students’ intellectual abilities we used the Triarchic Intelligence Test (STAT) createy Sternberg (1993). This is a test to measure intellectual abilities according to the Triarchic theory of intelligence (Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test). Its structure is the result of combining the three types of thinking (analytical, creative, and practical) with the (verbal, numerical, and figurative) content. Although it is possible to get a score for each subscale, in this study we only used total test score. As in other studies (e.g., Sternberg et al., 2001), in the present study it has adequate reliability (α = .748) and construct validity (fit for a unifactorial model: GFI = .986, AGFI = .976, CFI = .958, SRMR = .031, RMSEA = .044).
Math achievement. Achievement in mathematics was determined from students’ final marks in mathematics subjects providey the school secretaries, with prior permission from the participating students’ parents.
Procedure
This study was part of a larger piece of research in which various measuring instruments were applied in morning and afternoon sessions. The inventory of attitudes towards mathematics and the STAT were applied three months before the marks in math were awarded. Families of participating students were informey the schools and given assurances of confidentiality, and were required to provide their written consent.
Data Analysis
The data was analyzed in two phases. Firstly, the descriptive characteristics of the variables were calculated, along with a correlation matrix. Secondly, in order to examine the research hypotheses, various regression analyses were performed using the PROCESS module in the SPSS 22 statistical package (Hayes, 2013). The effect sizes were evaluated using Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988): (d < 0.20 = insignificant effect, d ≥ 0.20 & d < 0.50 = small effect, d ≥ 0.50 & d < 0.80 = moderate effect, d ≥ 0.80 = large effect).
Results
Preliminary Analysis
Descriptive data and the correlation matrix are presented in Table 1. Almost alf the correlation pairs were statistically significant. The correlations indicate that girls perform significantly better in mathematics than boys, despite perceiving themselves te less able than boys in this subject and exhibiting less anxiety. In addition, as students advance through the school years, math achievement is lower, they perceive themselves te less capable in math, and math anxiety is lower. The correlations also suggest that math achievement is higher when perceived ability is higher and anxiety is greater. Finally, and unexpectedly, the relationship between perceived math ability and anxiety is positive (the greater the perceived math ability, the higher the anxiety). Looking at the mean values of the variables we see that math achievement is in the middle of the scale, whereas mean perceived ability and math anxiety are higher than the midpoints of the scales. Finally, the three main variables of study (perceived ability, anxiety, and math achievement) exhibit values of asymmetry and kurtosis close to zero, which indicates that the distribution of scores can be considered normal.
Table 1 Pearson Correlation Matrix anescriptive Statistics (Mean, Standareviation, Asymmetry, and Kurtosis)
Note. Gender (girls = 0, boys = 1); school year (first = 1, second = 2, third = 3, fourth = 4); math achievement (1 = minimum, …., 5 = maximum); perceived math ability (1 = minimum, …, 5 = maximum); math anxiety (1 = minimum, …, 5 = maximum); STAT (Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test) (0 = minimum, 31 = maximum).
*p < .05, **p < .0.
Mediation Analysis
To evaluate the mediating role of math anxiety (ANX) in the relationship between math self-efficacy (PMA) and math achievement (MATH) mediation analysis was performed using the PROCESS program in SPSS. Gender, school year, and STAT were used as covariables.
Table 2 gives the data from the mediation analysis. The regression model explains a significant amount of the variance, both in ANX, R2 = .3824, R2adjusted = .139, F(4, 2180) = 93.3240, p < .0001, and especially in MATH, R = .5023, R2adjusted = .251, F(5, 2179) = 147.0921, p < .0001).
Table 2 Analysis of the Mediating Role of Anxiety in the Relationship between Perceived Math Ability and Math Achievement
Note. ANX (math anxiety); PMA (perceived math ability), STAT (Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test), MATH (math achievement); gender (girls = 0, boys = 1); school year (irst = 1, second = 2, third = 3, fourth = 4); MATH (1 = minimum, …., 5 = maximum); PMA (1 = minimum, …, 5 = maximum); ANX (1 = very low, …, 5 = very high); STAT) (0 = minimum, 31 = maximum).
The data in Table 2 supports the first two research hypotheses. Firstly, the relationship between PMA and MATH is statistically significant (b = 0.4797, p < .0001), with a moderate effect size (d = 0.6897), indicating that the higher the PMA, the higher the MATH. Secondly, it gives empirical evidence supporting the medication hypothesis of ANX in the PMA-MATH relationship (indirect effect: b = 0.0493, z = 4.4915, p < .0001). We find that, for example, the higher the PMA, the greater the ANX (b = 0.4265, p < .0001, d = 0.7598) and the higher the ANX, the higher the MATH (b = 0.1156, p < .0001, d = 0.2010), with moderate and small effect sizes, respectively. It indicates that the total effect of PMA on MATH is large (b = 0.5289, p < .0001, d = 0.8210).
Finally, the statistical controf covariables produced interesting results both in terms of ANX (very smalr small effect sizes) and mainly with respect to MATH the effects are statistically significant (smalr moderate effect sizes). Data suggests that (i) girls perform better than boys (although they exhibit less anxiety), (ii) students’ performance is worse as they go up through the school years, and they are less anxious about math, and (iii) the greater a student’s intellectual abilities, the higher their anxiety and the better math achievement.
Moderation Analysis
Using the variables gender, school year, and intellectual abilities as covariables, the moderator role of anxiety in the effect of perceived math ability on math achievement was analyzed. The data support the third study hypothesis: the relationship between PMA and MATH is conditioney the different levels of ANX (b = 0.2043, p < .0001), although the effect size of the interaction is small (d = 0.3769). The increase in the explained variance of the modeue to moderation is statistically significant, F(1, 2178) = 74.9389, p < .0001. Finally, the model explains 27.72% of the variability of MATH, F(6, 2178) = 139.2259, p < .0001). The three covariables included in the model were found te significantly associated with math: gender (b = -0.3471, p < .0001, d = 0.3132), school year (b = -0.1495, p <.0001, d = 0.2715) and STAT (b = 0.0602, p < .0001, d = 0.5705).
Table 3 details the results of the moderation effect of math anxiety. The PROCESS module subdivides the total group of subjects into three subgroups (low, medium, and high) using the cut-off scores corresponding to the 33rd and 66th percentiles in the measure of anxiety. In this way, it is possible to appreciate how the levef anxiety (low, medium, or high) affects the intensity, and sign, of PMA and MATH relationship. Data suggests that at every levef anxiety (low, medium, high) the effect of PMA on MATH is statistically significant, and it increases as the levef anxiety increases. At low levels of ANX the size of the effect of PMA on MATH is small (b = 0.2690, p < .0001, d = 0.2955), at medium levels of ANX, the effect size is moderate (b = 0.4856, p < .0001, d = 0.7124), and with high levels of ANX, the effect size is large (b = 0.7023, p < .0001, d = 0.8049).
Table 3 Conditional Effect of Perceived Math Ability on Math Achievement at Different Levels of Math Anxiety
1 Levels of anxiety: low (minimum score to 33rd percentile), medium (33rd percentile to 66th percentile), high (66th percentile to maximum score).
Discussion
The relationship between perceived math ability and math achievement has been widely studied, producing very consistent data. To a greater or lesser extent, most studies have found the relationship te strong and positive, indicating that the greater the perceived ability, the better the math achievement (and vice versa). What has been less researched is how that relationship can change (in magnitude, direction and type) when possible mediating or moderating variables are considered. One of those variables, which has been repeatedly associated with learning and math achievement, is anxiety. Because of that, in this study we have attempted tearn whether the relationship between perceived math ability and math achievement was mediated, at least partially, by anxiety, or if this relationship may differ (in intensity or direction) depending on anxiety levels. To examine the models of mediation or moderation, we used gender, school year, and students’ real abilities (intellectual abilities) covariables in order to statistically control for their effects. We formulated three hypotheses: (i) that PMA would significantly determine MATH, (ii) that ANX would mediate this association, and (iii) that the relationship between PMA and MATH woule conditioney the levels of ANX. Our data generally confirm these three hypotheses.
Firstly, we found that when PMA is high, MATH is also high, with moderate effect sizes in the mediation model (d = 0.6897) anarge effect sizes in the moderation model (d = 0.963). These results are in line with past research (Guay et al., 2003; Marsh & Martin, 2011; Rosário et al., 2012; Stevens et al., 2004; Tosto et al., 2016; Valentine et al., 2004). Data regarding the covariables in this study are also in line with existing research (e.g., Henschel & Roick, 2017; Hill et al., 2016; Ho et al., 2000; Utsumi & Mendes, 2000; Williams et al., 2016) in that girls, despite reporting lower perceived math ability than boys, exhibit better math achievement. However, both boys and girls, as they go up through the school years, report lower anxiety and exhibit lower math achievement. The effect sizes of gender and school year are small however, both in the mediation and moderation models.
Secondly, data from our study suggest that anxiety partially mediates the relationship between perceived math ability and math achievement, although the effect size of the mediation is small. Nonetheless, this mediation is basen relationships of a different direction to those found in some other studies. While in our study the relationship between PMA and ANX is positive, with a large effect size, indicating that the greater the PMA, the higher the ANX, other studies have found it te negative (e.g., Jain & Dowson, 2009; Kvedere, 2014). Similarly, the relationship between ANX and MATH was positive in our study, albeit with a small effect size, whereas other studies have found it te negative (e.g., Ashcraft & Moore, 2009; Chang & Beilock, 2016; Henschel & Roick, 2017; Ikegulu, 2003; Ma, 1999; Maloney & Beilock, 2012; Ramirez et al., 2016; Suárez-Pellicioni et al., 2015). This sequence of negative relationships was interpreted to mean that the more confidence a student had in their ability to complete math tasks well, the less anxious they would feel when faced with similar tasks in the future, and the less anxious they feel, the better their math achievement. However, in our study we find a series of positive relationships. How can we explain this apparent contradiction with the majority of previous research?; One plausible explanation may be that the higher the perceived ability, the greater the feeling of responsibility, and maybe the greater the fear of failing to perform to the level they believe they are capable of. In turn, this anxiety, rather than blocking the student, may provoke more mobilization of available resources and result in better than expected achievement. In contrast, if perceived ability is low, the fear of failure may be lower, as it is expected, and consequently that reduces anxiety about future tasks. With this combination, low confidence in oneself, together with a lack of anxiety, may lead tow mobilization of required cognitive resources (which often are available), anow math achievement may become a self-fulfilling prophecy. In future studies it woule extremely interesting to nonger look at the relationship between these three variables, but rather the complete explanatory hypotheses, maybe designing longitudionar repeated measure studies that might adequately capture the causal relationships implicit in the two hypotheses. One recent study (Caviola et al., 2017) reviewed past research which looked at how stress induction, manipulation of time pressure, and math anxiety could interfere with and/or condition the selection of strategies to solve arithmetic problems. The researchers concluded that, owing to the scant available literature, the questions coulnly be partially answered, and currently there is not much evidence of clear associations.
Thirdly, we aimed tiscover whether ANX also conditioned the relationship between PMA and MATH, something that previous research had not examined. Our data provide empirical evidence that the levef ANX significantly conditions the intensity of the effect of PMA on MATH. We found that as the levef anxiety increased, the effect of PMA on MATH also intensified. When anxiety was high, the size of the effect of PMA on MATH was alsarge, and when anxiety was low, the effect size was small. This means that when the subject experiences high levels of math anxiety, the importance of their confidence in themselves grows significantly as a determinant of math achievement. In contrast, when anxiety is low, their self-confidence is a much less potent determinant of performance. The data from our study suggests that the intensity of the relationship seen in other studies between PMA and MATH depends to a large extent on the levef ANX. Nevertheless, contrary to what we thought basen data from other studies (e.g., Wang et al., 2015), we did not find moderate levels of anxiety te the best for deploying the necessary competencies for gooearning and subsequent math achievement. Our data suggest that it is when anxiety is greatest that the effect of perceived math ability on math achievement is strongest and vice versa.
In summary, data of our study does not allow us to affirm emphatically whether anxiety is mainly a mediating or moderating variable of the effect of perceived math ability on performance in mathematics. However, the size of the effect of the data obtained in our study seem to support the moderation hypothesis more than that of mediation. While a statistically significant indirect effect (mediation) was obtained, its size is small (pulling to very small). However, the size of the effect varies considerably when considering levels of anxiety (moderation): while perceived competence is minimally related to performance when math anxiety is low, this relationship is large in the presence of high levels of anxiety. Consequently, we can conclude that data of the present study suggests that anxiety mainly plays a moderating role of the effect of perceived ability on math performance. Future research could further clarify this issue, through the use of data collection strategies with greater guarantees or control (perhaps basen an experimentar quasi-experimentaesign).
Implications for Educational Practice
The results of our study confirm those from much other research in which perceived ability is a significant predictor of achievement in a particular task. Data suggests that math teachers should consider it as important to provide students with good (declarative and procedural) knowledge in class as it is to help them (create optimal conditions) tevelop expectations of self-efficacy and results (Foley et al., 2017). In other words, it is essential to ensure that, through daily practice, students gain confidence in themselves about working and succeeding with math, because without that the student will face those daily tasks feeling insecure, lacking expectations of good results, and consequently will progressively become less involved, and stop learning and achieving in math tasks.
Working on the levef perceived ability is crucial to positively affect students’ levels of responsibility, and in this way improve achievement in this academic area. Nonetheless, it is very likely that student anxiety comes from other sources in addition tevels of perceived ability. Potential sources include, for example, a teaching process which is not an optimal fit for a student’s pace or style of learning, a teacher’s feedback methods, or the interaction of these factors with the student’s character or personality, it may be parents’ expectations of their child’s performance, or the pressure they are under to achieve results that may be at the limit of their capabilities (Cohen & Rubinstein, 2017). Cohen and Rubenstein (2017) found that in order tetter understand the etiology of math anxiety, multiple facets of the children’s upbringing must be considered.
Our data shows that when levels of anxiety are high, it significantly increases the potentiaf PMA to mobilize cognitive and motivational resources needed for good achievement compared tow levels of anxiety. However, it is possible that high levels of anxiety due to variables that are external to the student (family upbringing patterns or specific teaching process variables) do not necessarily lead tetter cognitive and motivational involvement of the student in math, and consequent better achievement. This would probably be because this socially determined anxiety does not impact the levels of perceived personal responsibility for future success. For this reason, it woule interesting tesign studies that address this general hypothesis. Or more specific questions such as how are the social, cognitive and neural facets of anxiety related?; Dowker et al. (2016) concluded that a need for many more interdisciplinary, longitudinal, and intervention studies was able to respond to this anther equally complex questions.
Limitations of the Study
This research, despite presenting apparently consistent data, from a broad sample of students, does have some limitations which shoule considered when drawing conclusions or generalizing the results. One limitation that is very common to research in education is the use of self-reports to gather information on some variables. In our case, we used two subscales of the Inventory of Attitudes towards Mathematics tbtain data on PMA and ANX. It woule useful to examine the extent to which results obtained through other procedures, or using measuring instruments other than self-reports, agree with the results in our study. Similarly, it shoule borne in mind that the MATH measure was obtained from the school marks in this subject. It is well known that school marks contain more information than mere student achievement. It is possible that the results of this study may change significantly if standardized tests of math achievement were used (for example, PISA style tests), producing stronger relationships. Finally, another limitation of our study was the strategy used to collect data. Although data was collected at twifferent time points (MATH after PMA and ANX), the design of the study was not experimental, which means it is not possible to interpret data in terms of causality (although it is done occasionally in describing the results).
Cite this article as: Pérez-Fuentes, M. del C., Núñez, A., Molero, M. del M., Gázquez, J. J., Rosário, P. & Núñez, J. C. (2020). The role of anxiety in the relationship between self-efficacy and math achievement. Psicología Educativa, 26(2), 137-143. https://doi.org/10.5093/psed2020a7
Funding: This research received external funding from the Government of the Principality of Asturias, Spain. European Regionaevelopment Fund (Research Groups Program FCGRUPIN-IDI/2018/000199).
Ashcraft, M. H., & Moore, A. M. (2009). Mathematics anxiety and the affective drop in performance. Journaf Psychoeducational Assessment, 27, 197-205. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282908330580
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191
Carey, E., Hill, F., Devine, A., & Szücs, D. (2016). The chicken or the egg?; The direction of the relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(1987). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01987
Cates, G. L., & Rhymer K. N. (2003). Examining the relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance: An instructional hierarchy perspective. Journaf Behavioral Education, 12, 23 - 34. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022318321416
Caviola, S., Carey, E., Mammarella, I. C., & Szucs, D. (2017). Stress, time pressure, strategy selection and math anxiety in mathematics: A review of the literature. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(1488). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01488
Chang, H., & Beilock, S. L. (2016). The math anxiety-math performance link and its relation to individual and environmental factors: A review of current behavioural and psychophysiological research. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 33-38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.04.011
Chen, P. P. (2003). Exploring the accuracy and predictability of the self-efficacy beliefs of seventh- grade mathematics students. Learning and Individuaifferences, 14(1), 77-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2003.08.003
Ching, B. H. H. (2017). Mathematics anxiety and working memory: Longitudinal associations with mathematical performance in Chinese children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51, 99-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.06.006
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2ª ed.). Erlbaum.
Cohen, L. D., & Rubinsten, O. (2017). Mothers, intrinsic math motivation, arithmetic skills, and math anxiety in elementary school. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(1939). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01939
Dowker, A., Sarkar, A., & Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics anxiety: What have we learned in 60 years?; Frontiers in Psychology. 7(508). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508
Fennema. E., & Sherman, J. A. (1976). Fennema-Sherman Mathematics Attitudes Scales: Instruments designed to measure attitudes towards the learning of mathematics by males and females. JSAS Catalog of Selecteocumentes in Psychology, 6(1), 3b. https://www.jstor.org/stable/748467
Foley, A. E., Herts, J. B., Borgonovi, F., Guerriero, S., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2017). The math anxiety-performance link: A global phenomenon. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 52-58. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416672463
García, T., Rodríguez, C., Betts, L., Areces, D., & González-Castro, P. (2016). How affective-motivational variables and approaches tearning predict mathematics achievement in upper elementary levels. Learning and Individuaifferences, 49, 25-31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.05.021
Guay, F., Marsh, H. W., & Boivin, M. (2003). Academic self-concept and academic achievement: Developmental perspectives on their causardering. Journaf Educational Psychology, 95, 124-136. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.124
Gunderson, E. A., Park, D., Maloney, E. A., Beilock, S. L., & Levine, S. C. (2017). Reciprocal relations among motivational frameworks, math anxiety, and math achievement in early elementary school. Journaf Cognition anevelopment, 19, 21-46. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2017.1421538
Hanin, V., & Nieuwenhoven, C. van (2016). The influence of motivational and emotional factors in mathematicaearning in secondary education. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée, 66, 127-138. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2016.04.006
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation and conditional process analysis. A regression based approach. Guilford Press.
Henschel, S., & Roick, T. (2017). Relationships of mathematics performances, control and value beliefs with cognitive and affective math anxiety. Learning and Individuaifferences, 55, 97-107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.009
Hill, F., Mammarella, I., C., Devine, A., Caviola, S., Passplunghi, M. Ch., & Szücs, D. (2016). Maths anxiety in primary and secondary school students: Gender differences, developmental changes and anxiety specificity. Learning and Individuaifferences, 48, 45-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.02.006
Ho, H., Senturk, D., Lam, A. G., Zimmer, J. M., Hong, S., Okamoto, Y., Chiu, S.-Y., Nakazawa, Y., & Wang, Ch. P. (2000). The affective and cognitive dimensions of math anxiety: A cross-national study. Journaf Research in Mathematics Education, 31, 362-379.
Ikegulu, N. T. (2003, June 26). The relationships among gender, mathematics anxiety-apprehension, and academic to achievement: Research findings anevelopment of an instrument to assess mathematics anxiety and apprehension. Lamar University Beaumont, TX 77710-1295.
Jain, S., & Dowson, M. (2009). Mathematics anxiety as a function of multidimensional self-regulation and self-efficacy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 240-249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2009.05.004
Kvedere, L. (2014). Mathematics self-efficacy, self-concept, and anxiety among 9th grade students in Latvia. 5th World Conference on Educational Sciences, 116, 2687-2690. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.01.636
Lipnevich, A. A., Preckel, F., & Krumm, S. (2016). Mathematics attitudes and their unique contribution to achievement: Going over and above cognitive ability and personality. Learning and Individuaifferences, 47, 70-79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2015.12.027
Ma, X. (1999). A meta-analysis of the relationship between anxiety toward mathematics and achievement in mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 520-540. https://www.jstor.org/stable/749772
Maloney, & Beilock, S. L. (2012). Math anxiety: Who has it, why it develops, and how to guard against it. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 404-406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.008
Mammarella, I. C., Donolatoa, E., Caviolab, S., & Giofrèc, D (2018). Anxiety profiles and protective factors: A latent profile analysis in children. Personality and Individuaifferences, 124, 201-2018.
Marsh, H. W., & Martin, A. J. (2011). Academic self-concept and academic achievement: Relations and causardering. British Journaf Educational Psychology, 81, 59-77. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709910X503501
Pitsia, V., Biggart, A., & Karakolidis, A. (2017). The role of students’ self-beliefs, motivation and attitudes in predicting mathematics achievement: A multilevel analysis of the Programme for International Student Assessment data. Learning and Individuaifferences, 55, 163-173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.014
Pizzie, R. G., & Kraemer, D. J. M. (2017). Avoiding math on a rapid timescale: Emotional responsivity and anxious attention in math anxiety. Brain and Cognition, 118, 100-107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2017.08.004
Ramirez, G., Chang, H., Maloney, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2016). On the relationship between math anxiety and math achievement in early elementary school: The role of problem solving strategies. Journaf Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 83-100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.014
Rosário, P., Lourenço, A., Paiva, O., Rodrigues, A., Valle, A., & Tuero-Herrero, E. (2012). Predicción del rendimiento en matemáticas: Efecte variables personales, socioeducativas y del contexto escolar. Psicothema, 24, 289-295.
Rosário, P., Núñez, J. C., Trigo, L., Guimarães, C., Fernández, E., Cerezo, R., Fuentes, S., Orellana, M., Santibáñez, A., Fulano, C., Ferreira, A., & Figueiredo, M. (2014). Transcultural analysis of the effectiveness of a program to promote self-regulateearning in Mozambique, Chile, Portugal, and Spain. Higher Education Research anevelopment, 34, 173-187. https://doi.org/10.1080/07294360.2014.935932
Stevens, T., Olivarez, A., Lan, W. Y., & Tallent-Runnels, M. K. (2004). Role of self-efficacy and motivation in mathematics performance across ethnicity. The Journaf Educational Research, 97, 208-221. https://doi.org/10.3200/JOER.97.4.208-222
Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test. Yale University.
Sternberg, R. J., Castejon, J. L., Prieto, M.D., Hautamäki, J. & Grigorenko E. (2001). Confirmatory analysis of the Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test in three international samples. European Journaf Psychological Assessment, 1, 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1027//1015-5759.17.1.1
Suárez-Pellicioni, M., Núñez-Peña, M. I., & Colomé, A. (2015). Math anxiety: A review of its cognitive consequences, psychophysiological correlates, anrain bases. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16, 3-22. https//doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0370-7
Tosto, M. G., Asbury, K., Mazzocco, M. M. M., Petrill, A., & Kovas, Y. (2016). From classroom environment to mathematics achievement: The mediating role of self-perceived ability and subject interest. Learning and Individuaifferences, 50, 260-269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.07.009
Utsumi, M. C., y Mendes, C. R. (2000). Researching the attitudes towards mathematics in basic education. Educational Psychology. 20(2), 237- 243. https://doi.org/10.1080/713663712
Valentine, J. C., DuBois, D. L., & Cooper, H. (2004). The relations between self-beliefs and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychologist, 39, 111-133. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3902_3
Valle, A., Núñez, J. C., Cabanach, R., González-Pienda, J. A., Rodríguez, S., & Rosário, P. (2009). Academic goals anearning quality in higher education students. Spanish Journaf Psychology, 12(1), 96-105. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1138741600001517
Wang, Z., Lukowski, S. L., Hart, S. A., Lyons, I. M., Thompson, L. A., Kovas, Y, Mazzocco, M. M., Plomin, R., & Petrill, S. A. (2015). Is mathematical anxiety always bad for math learning: The role of math motivation. Psychological Science, 26, 1863-1876. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615602471
Williams, K. E., White, S. L. J., & MacDonald A. (2016). Early mathematics achievement of boys and girls: Differences in early self-regulation pathways explain later achievement?; Learning and Individuaifferences, 51, 199-209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.09.006
María del Carmen Pérez-Fuentes1, Andrea Núñez1, María del Mar Molero1, José J. Gázquez1, Pedro Rosário2, and José C. Núñez3
1Universidad de Almería, Spain; 2Universidade do Minho, Portugal; 3Universidad de Oviedo, Spain
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2020. This work is licensed under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/deed.es_ES (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
This study aims to improve current understanding of how the relationship between perceived math ability and math achievement may be mediated or moderated by levels of anxiety about math, considering intellectual abilities, gender, and school year. In this study, participants were 2,245 Spanish students (7th to 10th grade). All completed the FennemaSherman Mathematics Attitudes Scales and the Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test. The main results showed that (i) perceivedcompetence is a potent predictor of achievement (the higher the perceived competence, the better the performance in mathematics), (ii) anxiety about mathematics significantly mediates the relationship between perceived competence and math achievement (although the effect is small), and (iii) levels of anxiety condition the intensity of the effect of perceived competence on math achievement (at high levels of anxiety the effect size of the relationship between perceived competence and achievement is large, whereas at low levels of anxiety the effect is small).