Introduction
The Locus Coeruleus (LC) is a small pontine nucleus with the largest group of noradrenergic (NA) neurons in the central nervous system (CNS) (Berridge & Waterhouse, 2003; Dahlstroem & Fuxe, 1964). Its ascending and descending projections modulate neuronal activity in numerous targets throughout the CNS (Loughlin, Foote & Fallon, 1982; Loughlin, Foote & Grzanna, 1986; Loughlin, Foote & Bloom, 1986; Samuels & Szabadi, 2008). Different studies suggest that alterations in the LC, and, hence, in its projection network, may critically contribute to the onset as well as to the course and symptom profile of a range of neurological and psychiatric disorders (Betts et al., 2019; Charney, 1998; Delgado & Moreno, 2000; Gannon et al., 2015; Leonard, 1997). Specifically, the LC has been suggested to be one of the initial sites of appearance of pathologically altered tau aggregates in preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Braak et al., 2011; Braak & Del Tredici, 2015; Zarow et al., 2003), leading to compensatory increases in α2A adrenergic receptor levels in regions receiving noradrenergic input (Andrés-Benito et al., 2017). Moreover, there is evidence that loss of noradrenergic input exacerbates AD progression (Marien, Colpaert & Rosenquist, 2004; Heneka et al., 2006; Grudzien et al., 2007).
One of the major limitations to investigate the role of the LC in brain disorders with in-vivo non-invasive techniques is that the LC is difficult to visualize with conventional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) techniques. However, Sasaki et al. (2006) identified the LC in vivo as two bilateral hyperintensities adjacent to the floor of the fourth ventricle adapting the parameters of a traditional 2D T1-weighted fast spin echo sequence (for instance, using a higher number of averages and a high in-plane resolution with a large slice thickness). The molecular underpinnings of such hyperintense signal are still unknown (Betts et al., 2019), although it was originally thought to be related with the accumulation of neuromelanin in LC neurons.
Neuromelanin is a dark polymer pigment formed as a byproduct of catecholamine metabolism, and it can be found in the LC and substantia nigra pars compacta. The function of this macromolecule is not entirely known, but it has been shown that neuromelanin accumulates throughout life in noradrenergic LC neurons, binding to metals such as iron and copper (Enochs et al., 1989; Enochs et al., 1997; Trujillo et al., 2017). Due to its ability to chelate metals, neuromelanin could be acting as a paramagnetic agent (Tosk et al., 1992), and perhaps neuromelanin could be a paramagnetic agent itself (Shima et al., 1997). These properties initially led some authors to hypothesize that neuromelanin could be causing the T1 shortening effects generating the LC contrast in fast spin echo T1 sequences (Sasaki et al., 2006).
Moreover, using MRI and post mortem histology, Keren et al. (2015) showed that T1-weighted LC hyperintensities co-localized with LC neurons with high neuromelanin concentration. Nevertheless, more recent research suggested that it is unlikely that LC hyperintensities are reflecting neuromelanin accumulation, but rather water content or general neuronal density (Watanabe et al., 2019). In this sense, throughout the manuscript, we will avoid using the term neuromelanin-sensitive MRI, although it may be concluded from the literature that LC MRI contrast ratios (LCCRs) seem to be a valid approximation to infer structural integrity of the LC (Betts et al., 2019).
There have been different reports of an overall lower LC signal in different neuropsychiatric conditions, such as major depressive disorder (MDD) (Sasaki et al., 2010; Shibata et al., 2008), schizophrenia (Shibata et al., 2008), amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) (Takahashi et al., 2015), and Alzheimer’s or Parkinson’s disease (Sasaki et al., 2006; Isaias et al., 2016; Matsuura et al., 2013; Miyoshi et al., 2013; Mukai et al., 2013; Ohtsuka et al., 2014), among others (for a review see Liu et al., 2017). It is not clear, however, to what extent such alterations show diagnostic specificity, which might be useful for clinical purposes (e.g., differential diagnosis, treatment selection, or prediction of clinical course). This could be important for disorders with partially overlapping clinical manifestations, such as late-life MDD and aMCI, two conditions typically appearing between the seventh and the eighth decades of life (Panza et al., 2010; Steffens, 2012).
People suffering from late-life MDD or aMCI have an increased risk for the later development of AD. Nevertheless, while aMCI is a common prodromal stage of AD, with a progression rate to AD estimated between 6% to 25% per year (Petersen et al., 2001) (for a review see Hermida et al., 2012), late-life MDD is not that robustly associated with dementia onset, and results have been more heterogeneous. Overall, research suggests that people suffering from late-life MDD have an approximately twofold increased risk of developing AD (Byers & Yaffe, 2011), although it is not clear whether depression is only a risk factor or may be considered a prodromal symptom of dementia (Byers & Yaffe, 2011). Interestingly, moreover, co-occurrence of aMCI and depression seems to increase even more the probability to develop to AD, suggesting that both conditions have an additive or synergic effect (Modrego & Ferrandez, 2004; Gabryelewicz et al., 2007). In this context, it is not clear whether LC MRI intensity discriminates between these disorders and their comorbidity, and, therefore, to what extent this measurement could contribute to identify subjects at a higher risk of developing a neurodegenerative disorder.
In this study, we aimed at comparing LC MRI intensity between patients with late-life MDD and individuals with aMCI, which, for reference purposes, were also compared to a group of healthy controls (HCs). Moreover, as a secondary aim, we specifically assessed a group of individuals presenting comorbidity between the two diagnoses, and evaluated the modulatory effect on our findings of medication and other clinical variables. According to previous literature, we hypothesized that LC MRI intensity will discriminate between the clinical groups and HCs, and also across patients with late-life MDD, aMCI and a comorbid presentation of both disorders.
Materials & Methods Participants
The sample consisted of 89 individuals between 60 and 76 years of age. These were divided into three groups: 37 patients with a primary diagnosis of late-life MDD (11 males, mean age [standard deviation, SD]: 68 [4.1] years), 21 subjects with aMCI (9 males, mean age [SD]: 71.5 [2.7] years), and 31 HCs from the same geographical area (11 males, mean age [SD]: 67.7 [4.1] years). Subjects from the clinical groups were consecutively recruited from the Psychiatry and Neurology Departments of Bellvitge University Hospital (Barcelona, Spain), and HCs through advertisements and word-of-mouth. All participants were interviewed with the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) (Sheehan et al., 1998). Major depressive disorder diagnoses were established by two experienced psychiatrists (VS and MU) according to DSM-IV-TR criteria, and disorder severity was estimated with the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) (Hamilton, 1960) and the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (Sheikh & Yesavage, 1986; Yesavage et al., 1982), which were, however, not used for diagnostic purposes. Amnestic MCI diagnoses were based on a syndromic categorical cognitive staging approach rather than on the use of biomarkers (Jack et al., 2018). Diagnosis was established by consensus of two experienced neurologists (JG and RR) and one neuropsychologist (IR) following Petersen criteria (Petersen, 2004), including: (1) complaints of memory loss confirmed by informants, (2) objective long-term memory impairments (scores 1.5 SD below mean age and education adjusted normative values in the delayed recall test from the Wechsler Memory Scale III (WMS-III) (Wechsler, 2004) and the brief neuropsychological battery NBACE (Alegret et al., 2012)), (3) preserved general cognitive functioning (i.e., scores higher than 23 for literate people and higher than 18 for illiterates in the Spanish version of the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein, Folstein & McHugh, 1975)), and (4) intact or mildly impaired daily living abilities (Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) (Morris, 1993) scores of 0.5). All participants were also administered the Vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition (WAIS-III) (Wechsler, 1999), to estimate the premorbid intelligence quotient (IQ) (García-Lorenzo et al., 2013). Importantly, aMCI participants did not present past or current comorbidity with MDD, while nine out of the 37 patients with MDD presented a comorbid aMCI diagnosis. In all cases, medication was not changed and was kept at stable doses for at least one month before MRI acquisition. Fourteen out of 21 aMCI subjects were taking antidepressants for conditions other than MDD, such as impaired psychosocial adjustment, insomnia or headache. Table 1 summarizes the sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the study sample, while information about antidepressant medication is provided in Table 2.
MDD patientsn = 37 | aMCI patientsn = 21 | Controlsn = 31 | Statistic(p value)a df | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Sociodemographic characteristics | ||||
Age, years; mean (SD) [range] | 68 (4.1) [60–74] | 71.5 (2.7) [67–76] | 67.7 (4.1) [60–75] | H = 1.38 (0.001) 2 |
Sex, male; n (%) | 11 (29.7) | 9 (42.9) | 11 (35.5) | X2 = 1.03 (0.599) 2 |
Clinical characteristics | ||||
HDRS; mean (SD) [range] | 12.1 (7.1) [1–28] | 3.8 (3) [0–8] | 1.3 (2.8) [0–15] | H = 50.85 (<0.0001) 2 |
GDS; mean (SD) [range] | 5.6 (4.3) [0–13] | 2.3 (2.2) [0–9] | 1.23 (2.3) [0–12] | H = 27.7 (<0.0001) 2 |
MMSE; mean (SD) [range]b | 25.9 (3.3) [13–30] | 25.8 (2.6) [20–29] | 28.9 (1.49) [24–30] | H = 32.68 (<0.0001) 2 |
Long Term Memoryc; mean (SD) [range] | 5.08 (2.54) [0-10] | 1.95 (1.28) [0–4] | 8.32 (1.72) [6–12] | H = 53.93 (<0.0001) 2 |
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.10828/table-1
Notes:
Abbreviations
aMCI
amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment
df
degrees of freedom
GDS
Geriatric Depression Scale
HDRS
Hamilton Depression Rating Scale
MDD
late-life Major Depression Disorder
MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
SD
Standard Deviation
aStatistic value corresponds to Kruskal-Wallis H test for continuous variables and chi-square test for categorical variables.
bThe cut-off in the Spanish version of the MMSE (Lobo et al., 1999) is adjusted for age and schooling years. Scores higher than 23 for literate people and higher than 18 for illiterates indicate preserved general cognitive functioning.
cEvaluated with the delayed recall test from the Wechsler Memory Scale III (WMS-III).
n (%) [dose-range in mg] | ||
---|---|---|
MDD patients n = 37 | aMCI patients n = 21 | |
Tricyclics | ||
Clomipramine | 2 (5.4) [37.5–75] | 0 (0) [0] |
Imipramine | 2 (5.4) [75–175] | 0 (0) [0] |
Amitriptyline | 1 (2.7) [25] | 2 (9.5) [20–25] |
Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) | ||
Sertraline | 1 (2.7) [200] | 0 (0) [0] |
Citalopram | 2 (5.4) [10] | 4 (19.1) [10–20] |
Paroxetine | 3 (8.1) [20–30] | 0 (0) [0] |
Serotonin–norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs) | ||
Venlafaxine | 12 (32.4) [75–300] | 0 (0) [0] |
Duloxetine | 11 (29.7) [60–120] | 0 (0) [0] |
Desvenlafaxine | 2 (5.4) [50–100] | 0 (0) [0] |
Other antidepressants | ||
Mirtazapine | 7 (18.9) [15–30] | 2 (9.5) [7.5–15] |
Bupropion | 5 (13.5) [150–300] | 0 (0) [0] |
Vortioxetine | 1 (2.7) [10] | 0 (0) [0] |
Agomelatine | 2 (5.4) [25] | 0 (0) [0] |
Trazodone | 4 (10.8) [50–100] | 7 (33.3) [50–100] |
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.10828/table-2
Notes:
Abbreviations
aMCI
amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment
MDD
late-life Major Depression Disorder
SD
Standard Deviation
Exclusion criteria for the study participants included: (1) ages <60 or >76 years, (2) past or current diagnosis of other major psychiatric disorders including substance abuse or dependence (except nicotine), (3) intellectual disability/neurodevelopmental disorders, (4) neurological disorders, (5) Hachinski Ischemic Score >5 to exclude individuals with a high probability of vascular-derived cognitive deficits, (6) presence of dementia according to the DSM-IV-TR criteria and/or a CDR score>1, (7) severe medical conditions, (8) electroconvulsive therapy in the previous year, (9) conditions preventing neuropsychological assessment or MRI procedures (e.g., blindness, deafness, claustrophobia, pacemakers or cochlear implants), and (10) gross abnormalities in the MRI scan. We also evaluated the presence of vascular pathology in our brain region of interest (pons), finding no significant alterations preventing accurate image processing and analysis.
The study was approved by The Clinical Research Ethics Committee (CEIC) of Bellvitge University Hospital (reference PR156/15, February 17th 2016) and performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments (revised in 2013). All participants gave written informed consent to participate in the study.
MRI scanning protocol
All scans were performed in a 3T Philips Ingenia Scanner (Koninklijke Philips N.V., Netherlands), using a 32-channel head coil. Following previous reports (Sasaki et al., 2006), we obtained a modified T1-weighted fast spin-echo sequence of the brainstem for LC visualization (see examples of individual participants in Fig. S1). Acquisition parameters were: TR 600 ms, TE 15 ms, 15 slices; 2.5 mm slice thickness, 0 mm gap, matrix size 404 × 250, FOV 170 × 170 mm2, acquisition voxel size 0.42 × 0.68 × 2.5 mm3, reconstructed voxel size 0.39 × 0.39 × 2.5 mm3, FA 900, 6 NEX (online averaging), and a total scan time of 15 min. The sections were acquired in the oblique axial direction perpendicular to the floor of the fourth ventricle, covering from the posterior commissure to the inferior border of the pons. Axial T1-weighted turbo-gradient-echo high-resolution whole-brain anatomical images (233 slices, TR = 10.46 ms, TE = 4.79 ms, flip angle = 8°, FOV = 240 × 240, 0.75 × 0.75 mm isotropic voxels) were additionally obtained for pre-processing purposes and to discard gross structural pathology. Finally, we also obtained a 2D FLAIR sequence in the axial plane (38 slices, TR = 10,000 ms, TE = 140 ms, TI = 2,700 ms, FOV = 230 ×186, 0.8 × 1 × 3 mm voxels, 0.6 mm gap) to assess vascular pathology and other potential radiological abnormalities. Throughout the acquisition protocol, we used foam pads and made sure that patients’ head was comfortably placed within the head coil to avoid excessive movement.
Localization and quantification of the LC
To quantify LC MRI intensity, we used a semi-automated “in-house” algorithm. We focused our analysis on the region of the dorsal pons. Specifically, our approach was based on previous studies (García-Lorenzo et al., 2013) and consisted of two main steps:
A): Delineation of regions of interest (ROIs): We defined four rectangular ROIs onto the Montreal Neurological Institute 152 [MNI 152] template (0.5 mm isotropic resolution) using the Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM 12) software (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Specifically, two of these ROIs were symmetrically located in the left and right areas of the dorsal pons where the LC is expected to be found, adjacent to the floor of the fourth ventricle and extending to the level of the inferior colliculi. Importantly, we explicitly avoided including in these ROIs other hyperintense regions, such as the substantia nigra, to ensure that the LC was the highest signal intensity. The other two ROIs covered the mid portions of the adjacent left and right pontine tegmentum (again, avoiding the inclusion of the substantia nigra), and were used as a reference to standardize LC intensity values (see below). These ROIs, drawn in normalized MNI space, were then denormalized to the native space of each participant. However, due to the small size of the locus coeruleus and the anisotropic voxel size of the images, the LC sensitive sequence was not manipulated and was used as the target image, therefore avoiding interpolation artifacts and preserving the original signal. Consequently, we first linearly coregistered, for each participant and in native space, the whole-brain T1 image to the LC sensitive image. Next, the tissue probability map (TPM) and the four ROIs in MNI space were coregistered (linear coregistration) to the whole-brain T1 image from the previous step, and such individual whole-brain T1 images were non-linearly normalized to the coregistered TPM, which generated a deformation field. The inverse of the deformation field was calculated by using the deformation function in SPM, taking the LC sensitive image as the image to base the inversion on. The four ROIs were then non-linearly denormalized to each participant native space by applying the inverse of the deformation fields with a 4th degree b-spline interpolation. Finally, these ROIs were binarized using a trilinear interpolation and a cut-off value of 0.1 (using the ImCalc function; i1>0.1) in order to better delimitate the ROI area to be extracted; then, each binarized ROI was applied to the LC-sensitive image (ImCalc function; i1.*i2), which effectively removed from these images all the information outside the ROIs. Therefore, the final output of the process were 4 images for participant, in native space, corresponding to the projection of each ROI into the LC-sensitive image. This approach is summarized in Fig. S2. Likewise, Fig. S3 displays the accuracy of the coregistration between the LC, the reference ROI and the brainstem structures in native space.
B): Quantification of the LC signal with a growing algorithm. This second step was implemented using an in-house growing algorithm, programmed in MATLAB version 9.3 (R2017b) (The MathWorks Inc, Natick, Massachusetts), which was applied to the final output images from step A) that encompassed the LC region. Specifically, following García-Lorenzo et al. (2013), who defined LC as “the area of 10-connected voxels with the brightest intensity”, the current algorithm searched throughout these images the 10 clusters of 10 contiguous voxels (face and/or edge and/or vertex contiguity) with the highest intensity (highest mean intensity of the cluster) (see Fig. 1). These clusters were visually inspected for their anatomical correspondence with the region of the LC. In case the cluster with highest intensity was deemed to be outside the LC region, the second cluster was inspected, and so on. Finally, we obtained, for each participant, the 10-voxels cluster of highest signal intensity within the LC region, as well as the mean intensity value and peak coordinate for this cluster (SLC). In agreement with our interest in assessing the intensity of LC, this 10-voxels average represents a good estimation of mean LC intensity and its presumed neural integrity (Liu et al., 2017). This is different from a volume measure of the nucleus; also, we did not discriminate intensities across the different territories of the LC (García-Lorenzo et al., 2013).
Figure 1: Visualization and localization of the LC and the reference region. (A) Axial, coronal and sagittal views of the dorsal pons from the modified fast spin-echo T1 sequence of a representative participant (see text for details). The LC can be identified in each hemisphere as an hyperintense structure (red arrows). (B) Axial, coronal and sagittal views of the dorsal pons from the high-resolution whole-brain T1 sequence of the same participant, depicting, in red, the 10 most intense LC voxels as identified by the growing algorithm. The denormalized ROI of the LC and reference regions are depicted as red (encompassing the 10 most intense LC voxels) and blue contours, respectively. DOI: 10.7717/peerj.10828/fig-1
The mean signal intensity from the reference pontine tegmentum cluster was also obtained for each participant (SREF). This value was used to control for putative differences in global signal intensity across participants. We used the following formula to calculate the LC contrast ratio (CR) (Liu et al., 2017): CR = (SLC – SREF) / SREF. This CR was calculated for each participant and for both LC (left and right), obtaining two different CRs for each participant (CRLeft and CRRight). However, as we did not expect interhemispheric differences, and in order to simplify statistical analyses, we averaged left and right CRs using the following formula: average LCCR= (CRLeft + CRRight) / 2. The average LCCR was the final variable used in the statistical analysis.
Statistical analysis
Differences in demographical and clinical features were contrasted across the three study groups with the Kruskal-Wallis test for continuous variables and the Chi-Square test for categorical variables. Differences in average LCCR were also assessed across the study groups using the Kruskal-Wallis test. Having in mind that LC MRI contrast ratios interact with age (Liu et al., 2019) and that structural and functional differences between men and women have been found in this region (Bangasser, Wiersielis & Khantsis, 2016; Mulvey et al., 2018), average LCCR comparisons were controlled for age and sex. For this, we created a new variable (standardized residual of average LCCR, sra LCCR) extracting the effect of both variables, as non-parametrical tests do not allow nuisance covariates. When the Kruskal-Wallis test showed significant differences across the three groups, pairwise comparisons were assessed using the Mann–Whitney’s U test. Moreover, we used Spearman’s Rho (rs) correlations to evaluate the possible associations between sra LCCR and different clinical features (i.e., dose and duration of treatments, MMSE).
Finally, we analyzed the modulatory effect of treatment (e.g., treatment type, duration or dose) on sra LCCR with the Kruskal-Wallis test and Spearman correlations (for categorical and continuous variables, respectively). When adequate, statistical significance was adjusted with a Bonferroni correction for multiple testing. We also calculated Cliff’s δ statistic (Cliff, 1996) to estimate effect sizes.
Results
Data on sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of study participants are displayed in Table 1. Due to the consecutive recruitment strategy, the groups differed in age (H = 13.38, p = 0.001, degrees of freedom (df) = 2), with the aMCI group showing the highest mean age, while the MDD group did not differ from HCs. Also, we found a significant negative correlation between the right LCCR and age (rs = −0.218, p = 0.04). In any case, as described above, all analyses were controlled for age and sex.
The Kruskal-Wallis test showed significant across-group (MDD n = 37, aMCI n = 21, HCs n = 31) differences in sra LCCR (H = 16.64, df = 2, p = 0.001). In post-hoc analyses, it was observed that MDD subjects displayed a lower sra LCCR in comparison to aMCI (U = 193, Z = − 3.16, p < 0.005, δ (Cliff’s delta effect size) = −0.53) and HCs (U = 281, Z = − 3.60, p < 0.001, δ = −0.51). See Table 3 and Fig. 2A. Importantly, our groups did not differ in the signal intensity from the reference region (H = 3.47, df = 2, p = 0.177), and, therefore, sra LCCR differences were attributable to differences in LC signal intensity.
We did not observe any significant effects of aMCI comorbidity in patients with MDD, since no significant differences were found in sra LCCR between the subgroup of MDD without aMCI (n = 28) and MDD patients with comorbid aMCI (n = 9) (U = 95, Z = − 1.10, p = 0.272, δ = − 0.25). Likewise, both groups significantly differed from HCs and the aMCI group (see Fig. S4). Moreover, as depicted in Fig. S5, we observed no significant correlations between sra LCCR values and MMSE scores for any of the study groups.
When evaluating the possible effects of medication (see Fig. 2B), we observed that patients with MDD taking Serotonin and Norepinephrine Reuptake Inhibitors (SNRIs) (n = 25) had a significantly lower sra LCCR compared to HCs (n = 31) (U = 143, Z = − 4.03, p < 0.001, δ= −0.63) and to aMCI patients (n = 21) (U = 94, Z = − 3.72, p < 0.001, δ= −0.64). However, we did not observe significant differences in sra LCCR between MDD patients taking (n = 25) and not taking (n = 12) SNRIs, although sra LCCR of MDDs patients not taking SNRIs did not differ from HCs (n = 31) (U = 138, Z = − 1.30, p = 0.194, δ= −0.26) or aMCI patients (n = 21) (U = 99, Z = − 1.01, p = 0.312, δ= −0.21). Although it was not possible to properly evaluate the effect of SNRIs due to the small sample size of the group of MDD subjects exclusively taking SNRIs (n = 5), comparing the group of MDD patients taking SNRIs alone with the rest of MDD patients (n = 32) and with patients taking SNRIs and other psychiatric medications (n = 20) we did not find significant differences (U = 60, Z = − 0.89, p = 0.374, δ= −0.25; and U = 43, Z = − 0.48, p = 0.634, δ= −0.14). Importantly, we did not find any significant differences between MDD patients taking and not taking SNRIs in clinical variables such as severity, disease duration or treatment resistance. These results are presented in Table S1.
Average LC Contrast RatioIntensity: mean (SD) | Standardized residual of Average LC Contrast Ratio(sraLCCR)Intensity: mean (SD) | |
---|---|---|
MDD whole group (n = 37) | 0.194 (0.059) | −0.450 (0.0728) |
MDD with aMCI (n = 9) | 0.195 (0.084 | −0.474 (1.016) |
MDD without aMCI (n = 28) | 0.194 (0.052) | −0.442 (0.633) |
aMCI (n = 21) | 0.234 (0.079) | 0.257 (0.946) |
HCs (n = 31) | 0.260 (0.093) | 0.363 (1.096) |
MDD split according to SNRIs treatment | ||
MDD taking SNRIs (n = 25) | 0.180 (0.045) | −0.621 (0.538) |
MDD not taking SNRIs (n = 12) | 0.224 (0.076) | −0.094 (0.947) |
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.10828/table-3
Notes:
Abbreviations
aMCI
amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment
HCs
Healthy Controls
LC
Locus Coeruleus
MDD
late-life Major Depressive Disorder
SD
Standard Deviation
SNRIs
Serotonin and Norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors
Figure 2: Boxplots depicting the average LC Contrast Ratios. Boxplots showing the average LCCR (age and sex adjusted, sra LCCR) for the different study groups. The individual values (dots) are overlaid for reference. (A) Boxplots of the average LCCR for the three main study groups; (B) Boxplots of the average LCCR splitting patients with late-life MDD as a function of SNRIs treatment. aMCI, amnestic type Mild Cognitive Impairment; HCs, Healthy Controls; LCCR, Locus Coeruleus Contrast Ratio; MDD, late-life Major Depressive Disorder. ** p ≤ 0.005, *** p ≤ 0.001. All results remained significant after excluding outlier values. DOI: 10.7717/peerj.10828/fig-2
Importantly, we repeated the analysis by subdividing the MDD group into those who took (n = 31) and those who did not take (n = 6) medications with noradrenergic effect (SNRIs + Tricyclics + Mirtazapine + Vortioxetine + Agomelatine + Bupropion), obtaining similar results: MDD taking noradrenergic medication had a significant lower sra LCCR compared to HCs (n = 31) (U = 209, Z = − 3.82, p < 0.001, δ= −0.57) and the aMCI group (n = 21) (U = 143, Z = − 3.40, p < 0.005, δ= −0.56). Again, although we did not observe significant differences in sra LCCR between MDD patients taking and not taking noradrenergic medications, sra LCCR of MDD patients not taking noradrenergic medications did not differ from HCs (n = 31) (U = 72, Z = − 0.86, p = 0.387, δ= −0.23) or aMCI patients (n = 21) (U = 50, Z = − 0.76, p = 0.448, δ= −0.21). Also, MDD patients exclusively taking noradrenergic medications other than SNRIs (n = 6) did not differ from patients taking SNRIs (n = 25), not taking noradrenergic medications (n = 6), or the HCs and aMCI groups (see Table S2). Finally, we also assessed whether subjects taking adrenergic medication for non-mental health purposes (i.e., beta-blockers; 4 HCs, 1 aMCI and 7 MDD) showed differences in sra LCCRs, finding non-significant effects.
Unfortunately, it was not possible to properly evaluate the effect of Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors (SSRIs) due to the small sample size of the group of MDD subjects exclusively taking SSRIs (n = 3). Nevertheless, comparing the group of MDD patients taking SSRIs alone or in combination with other medications (n = 6) with HCs and patients with aMCI, we did not find significant differences after Bonferroni correction (U = 32, Z = − 2.51, p = 0.012, δ= −0.66, and U = 20, Z = − 2.51, p = 0.012, δ= −0.68; Bonferroni p thresholded at p = 0.0083).
Finally, we did not find significant correlations between dose or duration of any of the treatments and sra LCCR.
Unless otherwise indicated, all the above significant findings survived Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.
Discussion
Our study compared LC MRI signal intensity between two groups of subjects with brain disorders conferring an increased risk for dementia. We observed that average LCCR was significantly lower in late-life MDD patients compared to aMCI and HC groups, but no evidence of additive effects between depression and aMCI. Moreover, such lower LC intensity was associated with the medication used by participants, since significant effects were indeed only observed in the subgroup of MDD patients taking SNRIs.
Our results are in overall agreement with the monoamine hypothesis of depression (Delgado, 2000), which suggests that MDD patients show significant alterations in monoaminergic neurotransmission, and, more specifically, with previous neuroimaging studies reporting a lower LC intensity in patients with MDD compared to HCs (Sasaki et al., 2010; Shibata et al., 2008). Previous reports have shown that NA plays a major role in the pathophysiology of depression (Delgado & Moreno, 2000; Moret & Briley, 2011), with the LC projecting to emotion- and cognition-related areas implicated in the pathophysiology of depression (Stahl, 2008).
In any case, we also observed that this lower LC intensity was specific of MDD patients taking dual action antidepressants (SNRIs). There could be two different explanations for this. On the one hand, if we consider that LC MRI contrast is directly or indirectly associated to the accumulation of neuromelanin compounds (Betts et al., 2019), we could speculate about the nature of such accumulation and the molecular effect of dual antidepressant treatment. Neuromelanin is an autophagic byproduct resulting from the metabolism of NA and other catecholamines (German et al., 1988; Graham, 1979), and its accumulation increases with age in different brain nuclei, such as the substantia nigra and the LC (Clewett et al., 2016). Since SNRIs antidepressants inhibit NA reuptake, the levels of this neurotransmitter are increased at the synaptic cleft, what could lead to a decrease in intracellular NA synthesis and metabolism. Sustained over time, this decrease in NA synthesis and degradation could result in a decreased neuromelanin accumulation in the LC. However, this could have been a strong argument if significant correlations between LC signal intensity and SNRIs treatment dose and duration had been observed, but this was not the case in our study. Moreover, there are no evidences of the existence of an enzymatic mechanism capable of inducing degradation or removing neuromelanin (Fedorow et al., 2006; Halliday et al., 2006), and, therefore, it is not possible to argue that noradrenergic antidepressants may be promoting neuromelanin degradation in LC neurons.
On the other hand, the possibility exists that particular clinical features of the subgroup of MDD subjects who are taking SNRIs might be associated with the lower LCCR in this group of patients. In our sample, MDD patients taking SNRIs did previously fail to respond to treatments with SSRIs. Speculatively, this treatment failure could be related to an impaired NA function reflected in lower MRI LCCR that required the active modulation of NA neurotransmission. Although treatment with dual action antidepressant does not seem to normalize LCCR intensity levels, prospective research on the interaction between antidepressant medication with noradrenergic action and LC signal intensity, carefully controlling for doses and treatment duration, is warranted to elucidate this issue.
Contrary to our initial expectations, we did not observe any significant effect of aMCI on LC signal intensities, which prevents suggesting LC MRI intensity as a putative risk imaging marker for AD. These results contradict previous research showing not only an early loss of LC neurons in AD (Szot et al., 2006), but also lower LC intensity values in aMCI versus HCs (Takahashi et al., 2015). Critically, however, that study did not exclude patients with comorbid depression, while our group of aMCI individuals did not show comorbidity with major depression. Therefore, we propose that MDD symptoms may be actually more relevantly accounting for lower LC signal intensity. In agreement with this, we have also observed that the presence of comorbid aMCI in patients with MDD did not significantly contribute to further lowering LC signal intensities.
This study is not without limitations. First, since we did not obtain data on cerebrospinal fluid or positron emission tomography imaging biomarkers, we used a syndromic categorical cognitive staging approach and we cannot confirm that our patients were indeed from the AD continuum and will ultimately develop AD. Second, longitudinal studies with larger and carefully characterized samples are highly encouraged in order to better explore the interactions over time of group findings with clinical and sociodemographic variables. This will probably allow identifying clinically relevant subgroups of individuals as a function of their LC signal intensities and contribute to better estimate the likelihood of a degenerative course. Third, we are not using a quantitative MRI method, although other MRI sequences, such as Magnetization Transfer (MT), may provide quantitative indices (Ramani et al., 2002; Sled & Pike, 2001) that have been used for assessing LC integrity (Trujillo et al., 2019). As many other groups, we used a Spin Echo (SE) sequence for assessing LC neuronal integrity (Liu et al., 2017), although MT based sequences seem to be a better option to perform absolute signal quantifications (Trujillo et al., 2019) and to more precisely localize the LC with isotropic resolution and increased contrast-to-noise ratio, especially at higher-field strengths (Priovoulos et al., 2018). We performed a relative quantification taking another brainstem region as the reference area to control for intensity fluctuations across participants, and, importantly, groups did not significantly differ in the intensity of this reference region, indicating that our group differences stem from differences in LC intensity. Nevertheless, further studies combining quantitative and non-quantitative approaches are probably needed to perform better estimations of neural integrity in the LC (Betts et al., 2019). Likewise, the present approach did not allow quantifying the volume of the LC, which would have required performing a manual segmentation of the nucleus, and, therefore, we can only infer changes in LC integrity from changes in MRI signal intensity. If such signal intensity is related to neuromelanin accumulation, LC integrity will be probably reflecting preserved adrenergic activity. By contrast, if such signal is related to higher neuronal density or water content, as recently suggested (Watanabe et al., 2019), LC integrity will be reflecting a lack of regional atrophy and/or a good osmotic balance in the context of a normal neuronal metabolism.
In the context of limitations, it is also important to note that there are studies suggesting that age-related variance should be considered when exploring structural abnormalities in the LC (Shibata et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2019). Moreover, these age effects may probably show a non-homogenous distribution along the rostral-caudal axis of the nucleus (Betts et al., 2017; Dahl et al., 2019; Liu et al., 2019). Thus, age-related declines in LCCR have been mainly observed in the rostral part of the LC in healthy subjects (Liu et al., 2019), being such decline in rostral LCCRs related to poorer memory performance in neuropsychological testing (Dahl et al., 2019). Although we controlled for age and sex, our algorithm did not allow distinguishing between the rostral and the caudal parts of the nucleus, which may have partially confounded our findings. Moreover, white matter from the pontine tegmentum, used here as reference, has also been shown to increase its T1 intensity with age, probably as a consequence of the highly protracted cycle of myelination of this region, extending up to 70 or 80 years of age (Yakovlev & Lecours, 1967). Despite our statistical control for age, and although our groups did not differ in the signal intensity of this reference region, this may have also partially confounded our findings.
Conclusions
To sum up, LC signal intensity does not seem to allow identifying subjects with cognitive profiles related to prodromal phases of AD. Conversely, it seems to be associated to late-life MDD. Further research with larger samples should ascertain whether medication aimed at modulating noradrenergic neurotransmission may be playing a role in these findings or, conversely, changes in LC neurons are associated with a particular clinical profile preferentially observed in patients responding to noradrenergic medication. It also remains to be determined if these findings may contribute to optimizing treatment of patients with late-life MDD.
Additional Information and Declarations
Competing Interests
These authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest regarding this work.
Virginia Soria has received grants and served as a consultant or continuing medical education (CME) speaker for Lundbeck, Otsuka, Janssen-Cilag, Exeltis and the Institute of Health Carlos III through the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness. Mikel Urretavizcaya has been funded by the Institute of Health Carlos III through the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness and has received compensation for lectures, advisories, or grants from Janssen-Cilagand Lundbeck. José Manuel Menchón has received grants and served as a consultant, advisor or CME speaker for Janssen-Cilag, Lundbeck, Medtronic, Otsuka, and the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (CIBERSAM).
The rest of authors have nothing to disclose nor have any financial relationships with commercial interests.
Author Contributions
Andrés Guinea-Izquierdo performed the experiments, analyzed the data, prepared figures and/or tables, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, investigation, and approved the final draft.
Mónica Giménez analyzed the data, prepared figures and/or tables, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, and approved the final draft.
Ignacio Martínez-Zalacaín performed the experiments, analyzed the data, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, software implementation, and approved the final draft.
Inés del Cerro performed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, investigation, and approved the final draft.
Pol Canal-Noguer analyzed the data, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, software implementation, and approved the final draft.
Gerard Blasco performed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, and approved the final draft.
Jordi Gascón, Ramon Reñé, Inmaculada Rico, Angels Camins and Mikel Urretavizcaya performed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, data curation, investigation, and approved the final draft.
Carlos Aguilera performed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, data curation, funding acquisition, and approved the final draft.
Isidre Ferrer conceived and designed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, funding acquisition, and approved the final draft.
José Manuel Menchón conceived and designed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, data curation, funding acquisition, and approved the final draft.
Virginia Soria conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, data curation, project administration, investigation, and approved the final draft.
Carles Soriano-Mas conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, data curation, supervision, project administration, funding acquisition, and approved the final draft.
Human Ethics
The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):
The study was approved by The Clinical Research Ethics Committee (CEIC) of Bellvitge University Hospital and performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments (revised in 2013) (Reference PR156/15 (17 February 17 2016).
Data Availability
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
Raw data is available in the Supplemental Files.
Funding
This study was supported by the Agency for Management of University and Research Grants of the Catalan Government (2017SGR1247), the Carlos III Health Institute, Spain (Grant PIE14/00034) and FEDER Funds/European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) (‘A way to build Europe’). Andrés Guinea-Izquierdo was supported by a FPU14/04822 grant. Ignacio Martínez-Zalacaín is supported by a P-FIS grant (FI17/00294) from the Carlos III Health Institute (Spain). Inés del Cerro is supported by CIBERSAM and previously by a PhD FI Grant from AGAUR-Catalan Government (2016FI_B 00712), grant co-funded by the European Social Fund (ESF) “ESF, Investing in your future”. Isidre Ferrer is supported by CIBERNED and CS-M was supported by a Miguel Servet contract from the Carlos III Health Institute (CPII16/00048). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Alegret M, Espinosa A, Vinyes-Junqué G, Valero S, Hernández I, Tárraga L, Becker JT, Boada M. 2012. Normative data of a brief neuropsychological battery for Spanish individuals older than 49. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 34:209-219
Andrés-Benito P, Fernández-Dueñas V, Carmona M, Escobar LA, Torrejón-Escribano B, Aso E, Ciruela F, Ferrer I. 2017. Locus coeruleus at asymptomatic early and middle Braak stages of neurofibrillary tangle pathology. Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology 43:373-392
Bangasser DA, Wiersielis KR, Khantsis S. 2016. Sex differences in the Locus Coeruleus-Norepinephrine system and its regulation by stress. Brain Research 1641(PtB):177-188
Berridge CW, Waterhouse BD. 2003. The locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system: modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews 42:33-84
Betts MJ, Cardenas-Blanco A, Kanowski M, Jessen F, Düzel E. 2017. In vivo MRI assessment of the human locus coeruleus along its rostrocaudal extent in young and older adults. Neuroimage 163:150-159
Betts MJ, Kirilina E, Otaduy MCG, Ivanov D, Acosta-Cabronero J, Callaghan MF, Lambert C, Cardenas-Blanco A, Pine K, Passamonti L, Loane C, Keuken MC, Trujillo P, Lüsebrink F, Mattern H, Liu KY, Priovoulos N, Fliessbach K, Dahl MJ, Maaß A, Madelung CF, Meder D, Ehrenberg AJ, Speck O, Weiskopf N, Dolan R, Inglis B, Tosun D, Morawski M, Zucca FA, Siebner HR, Mather M, Uludag K, Heinsen H, Poser BA, Howard R, Zecca L, Rowe JB, Grinberg LT, Jacobs HIL, Düzel E, Hämmerer D+32 more. 2019. Locus coeruleus imaging as a biomarker for noradrenergic dysfunction in neurodegenerative diseases. Brain 142:2558-2571
Braak H, Del Tredici K. 2015. The preclinical phase of the pathological process underlying sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 138:2814-2833
Braak H, Thal DR, Ghebremedhin E, Del Tredici K. 2011. Stages of the pathologic process in alzheimer disease: age categories from 1 to 100 years. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology 70:960-969
Byers AL, Yaffe K. 2011. Depression and risk of developing dementia. Nature Reviews Neurology 7:323-331
Charney DS. 1998. Monoamine dysfunction and the pathophysiology and treatment of depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59(Suppl 14):11-14
Clewett DV, Lee T-H, Greening S, Ponzio A, Margalit E, Mather M. 2016. Neuromelanin marks the spot: identifying a locus coeruleus biomarker of cognitive reserve in healthy aging. Neurobiology of Aging 37:117-126
Cliff N. 1996. Answering ordinal questions with ordinal data using ordinal statistics. Multivariate Behavioral Research 31:331-350
Dahl MJ, Mather M, Düzel S, Bodammer NC, Lindenbergr U, Kühn S, Werkle-Bergner M. 2019. Rostral locus Coeruleus integrity is associated with better memory performance in older adults. Nature Human Behaviour 3:1203-1214
Dahlstroem A, Fuxe K. 1964. Evidence for the existence of monoamine-containing neurons in the central nervous system. I. Demonstration of monoamines in the cell bodies of brain stem neurons. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica Suppl 232:1-55
Delgado PL. 2000. Depression: the case for a monoamine deficiency. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 61(Suppl 6):7-11
Delgado PL, Moreno FA. 2000. Role of norepinephrine in depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 61(Suppl 1):5-12
Enochs WS, Hyslop WB, Bennett HF, Brown RD, Koenig SH, Swartz HM. 1989. Sources of the increased longitudinal relaxation rates observed in melanotic melanoma. An in vitro study of synthetic melanins. Investigative Radiology 24:794-804
Enochs WS, Petherick P, Bogdanova A, Mohr U, Weissleder R. 1997. Paramagnetic metal scavenging by melanin: MR imaging. Radiology 204:417-423
Fedorow H, Halliday GM, Rickert CH, Gerlach M, Riederer P, Double KL. 2006. Evidence for specific phases in the development of human neuromelanin. Neurobiology of Aging 27:506-512
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. 1975. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research 12:189-198
Gabryelewicz T, Styczynska M, Luczywek E, Barczak A, Pfeffer A, Androsiuk W, Chodakowska-Zebrowska M, Wasiak B, Peplonska B, Barcikowska M. 2007. The rate of conversion of mild cognitive impairment to dementia: predictive role of depression. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 22:563-567
Gannon M, Che P, Chen Y, Jiao K, Roberson ED, Wang Q. 2015. Noradrenergic dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease. Frontiers in Neuroscience 9:220
García-Lorenzo D, Longo-Dos Santos C, Ewencyk C, Leu-Semenescu S, Gallea C, Quattrocchi G, Pita Lobo P, Poupon C, Benali H, Arnulf I, Vidailhet M, Lehericy S+2 more. 2013. The Coeruleus/subcoeruleus complex in rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorders in Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage 136:2120-2129
German DC, Walker BS, Manaye K, Smith WK, Woodward DJ, North AJ. 1988. The human locus coeruleus: computer reconstruction of cellular distribution. Journal of Neuroscience 8:1776-1788
Graham DG. 1979. On the origin and significance of neuromelanin. Archives of Pathology and Laboratory Medicine 103:359-362
Grudzien A, Shaw P, Weintraub S, Bigio E, Mash DC, Mesulam MM. 2007. Locus coeruleus neurofibrillary degeneration in aging, mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging 28:327-335
Halliday GM, Fedorow H, Rickert CH, Gerlach M, Riederer P, Double KL. 2006. Evidence for specific phases in the development of human neuromelanin. Journal of Neural Transmission 113:721-728
Hamilton M. 1960. A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 23:56-62
Heneka MT, Ramanathan M, Jacobs AH, Dumitrescu-Ozimek L, Bilkei-Gorzo A, Debeir T, Sastre M, Galldiks N, Zimmer A, Hoehn M, Heiss W-D, Klockgether T, Staufenbiel M+3 more. 2006. Locus coeruleus degeneration promotes alzheimer pathogenesis in amyloid precursor protein 23 transgenic mice. Journal of Neuroscience 26:1343-1354
Hermida AP, McDonald WM, Steenland K, Levey A. 2012. The association between late-life depression, mild cognitive impairment and dementia: is inflammation the missing link? Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics 12:1339-1350
Isaias IU, Trujillo P, Summers P, Marotta G, Mainardi L, Pezzoli G, Zecca L, Costa A. 2016. Neuromelanin imaging and dopaminergic loss in parkinson’s disease. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 8:196
Jack CR, Bennett DA, Blennow K, Carrillo MC, Dunn B, Budd Haeberlein S, Holtzman DM, Jagust W, Jessen F, Karlawish J, Liu E, Molinuevo JL, Montine T, Phelps C, Rankin KP, Rowe CC, Scheltens P, Siemers E, Snyder HM, Sperling R, Collaborators+11 more. 2018. NIA-AA research framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement 14:535-562
Keren NI, Taheri S, Vazey EM, Morgan PS, Granholm A-CE, Aston-Jones GS, Eckert MA. 2015. Histologic validation of locus coeruleus MRI contrast in post-mortem tissue. Neuroimage 113:235-245
Leonard BE. 1997. The role of noradrenaline in depression: a review. Journal of Psychopharmacology 11:S39-S47
Liu KY, Acosta-Cabronero J, Cardenas-Blanco A, Loane C, Berry AJ, Betts MJ, Kievit RA, Henson RN, Düel E, Cam-CAN, Howard R, Hämmerer D+2 more. 2019. In vivo visualization of age-related differences in the locus coeruleus. Neurobiology of Aging 74:101-111
Liu KY, Marijatta F, Hämmerer D, Acosta-Cabronero J, Düzel E, Howard RJ. 2017. Magnetic resonance imaging of the human locus coeruleus: a systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 83:325-355
Lobo A, Saz P, Marcos G, Día JL, De la Cámara C, Ventura T, Morales Asín F, Pascual FL, Montañés JA, Aznar S. 1999. Revalidación y normalización del miniexamen cognoscitivo (primera versión en castellano del Minimental State Examination) en la población general geriátrica. Medicina Clínica 112:767-774
Loughlin SE, Foote SL, Bloom FE. 1986. Efferent projections of nucleus locus coeruleus: topographic organization of cells of origin demonstrated by three-dimensional reconstruction. Neuroscience 18:291-306
Loughlin SE, Foote SL, Fallon JH. 1982. Locus coeruleus projections to cortex: topography, morphology and collateralization. Brain Research Bulletin 9:287-294
Loughlin SE, Foote SL, Grzanna R. 1986. Efferent projections of nucleus locus coeruleus: morphologic subpopulations have different efferent targets. Neuroscience 18:307-319
Marien MR, Colpaert FC, Rosenquist AC. 2004. Noradrenergic mechanisms in neurodegenerative diseases: a theory. Brain Research Reviews 45:38-78
Matsuura K, Maeda M, Yata K, Ichiba Y, Yamaguchi T, Kanamaru K, Tomimoto H. 2013. Neuromelanin magnetic resonance imaging in Parkinson’s disease and multiple system atrophy. European Neurology 70:70-77
Miyoshi F, Ogawa T, Kitao S-I, Kitayama M, Shinohara Y, Takasugi M, Fujii S, Kaminou T. 2013. Evaluation of Parkinson disease and Alzheimer disease with the use of neuromelanin MR imaging and (123)I-metaiodobenzylguanidine scintigraphy. American Journal of Neuroradiology 34:2113-2118
Modrego PJ, Ferrandez J. 2004. Depression in patients with mild cognitive impairment increases the risk of developing dementia of Alzheimer type: a prospective cohort study. Archives of Neurology 61:1290-1293
Moret C, Briley M. 2011. The importance of norepinephrine in depression. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 7:9-13
Morris JC. 1993. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43:2412-2414
Mukai M, Sugaya K, Yabe I, Goto Y, Yokochi F, Miyamoto K, Cai H, Sasaki H, Matsubara S. 2013. Neuromelanin MRI in a family with mitochondrial parkinsonism harboring a Y955C mutation in POLG1. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders 19:821-824
Mulvey B, Bhatti DL, Gyawali S, Lake AM, Kriaucionis S, Ford CP, Bruchas MR, Heintz N, Dougherty JD. 2018. Molecular and functional sex differences of noradrenergic neurons in the mouse locus coeruleus. Cell Reports 23:2225-2235
Ohtsuka C, Sasaki M, Konno K, Kato K, Takahashi J, Yamashita F, Terayama Y. 2014. Differentiation of early-stage parkinsonisms using neuromelanin-sensitive magnetic resonance imaging. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders 20:755-760
Panza F, Frisardi V, Capurso C, D’Introno A, Colacicco AM, Imbimbo BP, Santamato A, Vendemiale G, Seripa D, Pilotto A, Capurso A, Solfrizzi V+2 more. 2010. Late-life depression, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia: possible continuum? The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 18:98-116
Petersen RC. 2004. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine 256:183-194
Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, Ritchie K, Rossor M, Thal L, Winblad B. 2001. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology 58:1985-1992
Priovoulos N, Jacobs H, Ivanov D, Uludağ K, Verhey F, Poser BA. 2018. High-resolution in vivo imaging of human locus coeruleus by magnetization transfer MRI at 3T and 7T. NeuroImage 168:427-436
Ramani A, Dalton C, Miller DH, Tofts PS, Barker GJ. 2002. Precise estimate of fundamental in-vivo MT parameters in human brain in clinically feasible times. Magnetic Resonance Imaging 20:721-731
Samuels ER, Szabadi E. 2008. Functional neuroanatomy of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus: its roles in the regulation of arousal and autonomic function part I: principles of functional organisation. Current Neuropharmacology 6:235-253
Sasaki M, Shibata E, Ohtsuka K, Endoh J, Kudo K, Narumi S, Sakai A. 2010. Visual discrimination among patients with depression and schizophrenia and healthy individuals using semiquantitative color-coded fast spin-echo T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroradiology 52:83-89
Sasaki M, Shibata E, Tohyama K, Takahashi J, Otsuka K, Tsuchiya K, Takahashi S, Ehara S, Terayama Y, Sakai A. 2006. Neuromelanin magnetic resonance imaging of locus coeruleus and substantia nigra in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroreport 17:1215-1218
Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. 1998. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59(Suppl 20):22-33
Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. 1986. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clinical Gerontologist: The Journal of Aging and Mental Health 5:165-173
Shibata E, Sasaki M, Tohyama K, Kanbara Y, Otsuka K, Ehara S, Sakai A. 2006. Age-related changes in locus ceruleus on neuromelanin magnetic resonance imaging at 3 Tesla. Magnetic Resonance in Medical Sciences 5:197-200
Shibata E, Sasaki M, Tohyama K, Otsuka K, Endoh J, Terayama Y, Sakai A. 2008. Use of neuromelanin-sensitive MRI to distinguish schizophrenic and depressive patients and healthy individuals based on signal alterations in the substantia nigra and locus ceruleus. Biological Psychiatry 64:401-406
Shima T, Sarna T, Swartz HM, Stroppolo A, Gerbasi R, Zecca L. 1997. Binding of iron to neuromelanin of human substantia nigra and synthetic melanin: an electron paramagnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Free Radical Biology and Medicine 23:110-119
Sled JG, Pike GB. 2001. Quantitative imaging of magnetization transfer exchange and relaxation properties in vivo using MRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 46:923-931
Stahl SM. 2008. Stahl’s essential psychopharmacology: Neuroscientific basis and practical applications (3rd edition). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Steffens DC. 2012. Depressive symptoms and mild cognitive impairment in the elderly: an ominous combination. Biological Psychiatry 71:762-764
Szot P, White SS, Greenup JL, Leverenz JB, Peskind ER, Raskind MA. 2006. Compensatory changes in the noradrenergic nervous system in the locus ceruleus and hippocampus of postmortem subjects with Alzheimer’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Journal of Neuroscience 26:467-478
Takahashi J, Shibata T, Sasaki M, Kudo M, Yanezawa H, Obara S, Kudo K, Ito K, Yamashita F, Terayama Y. 2015. Detection of changes in the locus coeruleus in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: high-resolution fast spin-echo T1-weighted imaging. Geriatrics & Gerontology International 15:334-340
Tosk JM, Holshouser BA, Aloia RC, Hinshaw DB, Hasso AN, MacMurray JP, Will AD, Bozzetti LP. 1992. Effects of the interaction between ferric iron and L-dopa melanin on T1 and T2 relaxation times determined by magnetic resonance imaging. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 26:40-45
Trujillo P, Petersen KJ, Cronin MJ, Lin YC, Kang H, Donahue MJ, Smith SA, Claassen DO. 2019. Quantitative magnetization transfer imaging of the human locus coeruleus. NeuroImage 15:191-198
Trujillo P, Summers PE, Ferrari E, Zucca FA, Sturini M, Mainardi LT, Cerutti S, Smith AK, Smith SA, Zecca L, Costa A+1 more. 2017. Contrast mechanisms associated with neuromelanin-MRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 78:1790-1800
Watanabe T, Tan Z, Wang X, Martinez-Hernandez A, Frahm J. 2019. Magnetic resonance imaging of noradrenergic neurons. Brain Structure and Function 224:1609-1625
Wechsler D. 1999. Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para adultos-III. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
Wechsler D. 2004. WMS-III. Escala de Memoria Wechsler - III, TEA. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
Yakovlev PI, Lecours AR. 1967. The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain. In: Minkowski A, ed. Regional development of the brain in early life. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific. 3-70
Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, Leirer VO. 1982. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research 17:37-49
Zarow C, Lyness SA, Mortimer JA, Chui HC. 2003. Neuronal loss is greater in the locus coeruleus than nucleus basalis and substantia nigra in Alzheimer and Parkinson diseases. Archives of Neurology 60:337-341
Andrés Guinea-Izquierdo1,2, Mónica Giménez2, Ignacio Martínez-Zalacaín1,2, Inés del Cerro1,2,3, Pol Canal-Noguer4,5,6, Gerard Blasco7, Jordi Gascón8, Ramon Reñé8, Inmaculada Rico8, Angels Camins7, Carlos Aguilera7, Mikel Urretavizcaya1,2,3, Isidre Ferrer9,10,11, José Manuel Menchón1,2,3, Virginia Soria1,2,3, Carles Soriano-Mas2,3,12
1 Department of Clinical Sciences/School of Medicine, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
2 Department of Psychiatry/Bellvitge University Hospital, Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute (IDIBELL), Hospitalet de Llobregat (Barcelona), Spain
3 Network Center for Biomedical Research on Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain
4 B2SLab/Departament d’Enginyeria de Sistemes, Automàtica i Informàtica Industrial, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Barcelona, Spain
5 Networking Biomedical Research Centre in the subject area of Bioengineering, Biomaterials and Nanomedicine (CIBER-BBN), Madrid, Spain
6 Institut de Recerca Pediàtrica, Hospital Sant Joan de Déu, Esplugues de Llobregat (Barcelona), Spain
7 Imaging Diagnostic Institute (IDI), Bellvitge University Hospital, Hospitalet de Llobregat (Barcelona), Spain
8 Dementia Diagnostic and Treatment Unit/Department of Neurology, Bellvitge University Hospital, Hospitalet de Llobregat (Barcelona), Spain
9 Department of Pathology and Experimental Therapeutics/Institute of Neurosciences, University of Barcelona, Hospitalet de Llobregat (Barcelona), Spain
10 Department of Pathologic Anatomy/Bellvitge University Hospital, Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute-IDIBELL, Hospitalet de Llobregat (Barcelona), Spain
11 Network Center for Biomedical Research on Neurodegenerative diseases (CIBERNED), Madrid, Spain
12 Department of Psychobiology and Methodology in Health Sciences, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Bellaterra (Barcelona), Spain
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2021 Guinea-Izquierdo et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Background
The locus coeruleus (LC) is the major noradrenergic source in the central nervous system. Structural alterations in the LC contribute to the pathophysiology of different neuropsychiatric disorders, which may increase to a variable extent the likelihood of developing neurodegenerative conditions. The characterization of such alterations may therefore help to predict progression to neurodegenerative disorders. Despite the LC cannot be visualized with conventional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), specific MRI sequences have been developed to infer its structural integrity.
Methods
We quantified LC signal Contrast Ratios (LCCRs) in late-life major depressive disorder (MDD) (n = 37, 9 with comorbid aMCI), amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (aMCI) (n = 21, without comorbid MDD), and healthy controls (HCs) (n = 31), and also assessed the putative modulatory effects of comorbidities and other clinical variables.
Results
LCCRs were lower in MDD compared to aMCI and HCs. While no effects of aMCI comorbidity were observed, lower LCCRs were specifically observed in patients taking serotonin/norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs).
Conclusion
Our results do not support the hypothesis that lower LCCRs characterize the different clinical groups that may eventually develop a neurodegenerative disorder. Conversely, our results were specifically observed in patients with late-life MDD taking SNRIs. Further research with larger samples is warranted to ascertain whether medication or particular clinical features of patients taking SNRIs are associated with changes in LC neurons.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer