It appears you don't have support to open PDFs in this web browser. To view this file, Open with your PDF reader
Abstract
Age-related hearing loss typically affects the hearing of high frequencies in older adults. Such hearing loss influences the processing of spoken language, including higher-level processing such as that of complex sentences. Hearing aids may alleviate some of the speech processing disadvantages associated with hearing loss. However, little is known about the relation between hearing loss, hearing aid use, and their effects on higher-level language processes. This neuroimaging (fMRI) study examined these factors by measuring the comprehension and neural processing of simple and complex spoken sentences in hard-of-hearing older adults (n = 39). Neither hearing loss severity nor hearing aid experience influenced sentence comprehension at the behavioral level. In contrast, hearing loss severity was associated with increased activity in left superior frontal areas and the left anterior insula, but only when processing specific complex sentences (i.e. object-before-subject) compared to simple sentences. Longer hearing aid experience in a sub-set of participants (n = 19) was associated with recruitment of several areas outside of the core speech processing network in the right hemisphere, including the cerebellum, the precentral gyrus, and the cingulate cortex, but only when processing complex sentences. Overall, these results indicate that brain activation for language processing is affected by hearing loss as well as subsequent hearing aid use. Crucially, they show that these effects become apparent through investigation of complex but not simple sentences.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 University of Oldenburg, Institute of Dutch Studies, Oldenburg, Germany (GRID:grid.5560.6) (ISNI:0000 0001 1009 3608); University of Oldenburg, Cluster of Excellence “Hearing4all”, Oldenburg, Germany (GRID:grid.5560.6) (ISNI:0000 0001 1009 3608); University of Cambridge, Department of Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, Cambridge, UK (GRID:grid.5335.0) (ISNI:0000000121885934)
2 University of Oldenburg, Biological Psychology, Department of Psychology, Department for Medicine and Health Sciences, Oldenburg, Germany (GRID:grid.5560.6) (ISNI:0000 0001 1009 3608); University of Oldenburg, Cluster of Excellence “Hearing4all”, Oldenburg, Germany (GRID:grid.5560.6) (ISNI:0000 0001 1009 3608)
3 University of Oldenburg, Applied Neurocognitive Psychology, Department of Psychology, Oldenburg, Germany (GRID:grid.5560.6) (ISNI:0000 0001 1009 3608); University of Oldenburg, Cluster of Excellence “Hearing4all”, Oldenburg, Germany (GRID:grid.5560.6) (ISNI:0000 0001 1009 3608)
4 University of Oldenburg, Institute of Dutch Studies, Oldenburg, Germany (GRID:grid.5560.6) (ISNI:0000 0001 1009 3608); University of Oldenburg, Cluster of Excellence “Hearing4all”, Oldenburg, Germany (GRID:grid.5560.6) (ISNI:0000 0001 1009 3608)