About the Authors:
Tiina Turunen
Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland
ORCID logo https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1641-2347
Elisa Poskiparta
Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision
Affiliation: Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland
Christina Salmivalli
Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision
Affiliations Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland, Shandong Normal University, Jinan, China
Pekka Niemi
Roles Conceptualization, Supervision
Affiliation: Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland
Marja-Kristiina Lerkkanen
Roles Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources
Affiliations Department of Teacher Education, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylän, Finland, University of Stavanger, Stavanger, Norway
Introduction
Early adolescence is a period of change in many domains. Physical, cognitive, and social changes occur gradually and at individual pace, but the educational transition from elementary to middle school has a similar schedule for the entire peer group. Although for most adolescents acclimation to middle school is fast and successful [1–4], the transition can also be a stressful experience [5–8]. In middle school, the learning environment becomes more complex due to both academic and social demands. Students from several elementary schools often move to a new, bigger school building with different teacher in each subject. This brings about social challenges, as the size and the structure of the peer groups change, and students are looking for their own niche in the new social structure [9, 10]. Prior transition, a major concern for elementary school students is being bullied in middle school [11–14].
Bullying is deliberate, repeated aggressive behavior that involves a real or perceived power imbalance between the bully and the victim [15, 16]. Transition to a new school can be an opportunity for students involved in bullying to form new, more positive relationships with their peers [17, 18]. On the other hand, this point in time may include further risks for peer problems in new environment. The percentage of students who are being bullied seems to decrease in higher grades in Finland as well as in other countries [19–25]. Although the overall bullying perpetration rate also decreases [9], studies have identified a group of students who report higher rates of bullying perpetration after the transition to middle school [10, 22, 26, 27], or even an increase in aggression and prevalence of bullies in middle school [9], as has also been observed in Finnish schools [24, 25]. Previous studies have shown that in middle school victimization seems to be more targeted than in elementary school: there are more bullies targeting fewer victims [i.e., 24]. These differences have been suggested to be at least partly due to differences between elementary and middle school characteristics, such as varying and more impersonal classes, various subject teachers, competition and social comparisons between peers, and teacher attitudes toward bullying [9].
Albeit being to some extent stressful for everyone, transition to middle school may lead to aggravation and adjustment problems especially for students with specific learning disabilities such as reading difficulties (RDs) and poor reading skills, since these students often come to middle school with a history of poor performance and social problems [i.e., 28]. In addition to being an academic impediment, RDs have been linked to social challenges, such as poor social skills and lack of social competence [29–31], low self-esteem [32], peer rejection [33, 34], as well as bullying involvement [35, 36]. These problems may already have existed throughout the elementary school, and transition to a new middle school adds to the burden. The present study investigates the interplay between reading skills, victimization and bullying across the transition from elementary to middle school in Finnish school context where students are 12 years old when they transit from elementary school (Grade 6) to middle school (Grade 7).
Reading difficulties and bullying
RDs are among the most common learning difficulties affecting 3% to 17.5% of children and adults [37–39]. In the nationwide School Health Promotion study [25], 24% of the students in Grades 4 and 5, and 21.1% of the students in Grades 8 and 9 in Finland reported some or a lot of difficulties in reading in 2019. Furthermore, Kairaluoma et al. [40] identified with a screening test 7.4% of ninth grade students and 3.4% of vocational school students in their sample to be poor readers. This indicates that even in a language with a transparent orthography such as Finnish, reading difficulties persist long into adolescence, as has been reported in less transparent languages like English [41]. Whereas examining bullying among heterogeneous groups of students with a variety of learning problems is relatively common [e.g., 42–48], few studies have examined specific reading difficulties in association with bullying. However, there is some support for RDs and poor reading skills increasing the risk for both victimization and bullying others.
To begin with, struggling with the skill needed in school every day is likely to influence the overall experience of school, making it burdensome and aversive, and affecting emotions [e.g. 49–52] and behavior [53–56] at school. In interviews, children and adolescents with RDs have reported victimization rates from about a third [57], to 50% [32], and up to 85% [58]. Even adults have frequently reported negative memories of victimization due to learning problems [e.g., 59]. Moreover, Turunen et al. [36] found in a nationally representative community sample of elementary and middle school students that over a third of those with self-reported RDs were involved in bullying as victims, bullies, or bully/victims, compared with approximately a fifth of students not reporting RDs. After controlling for gender, grade level, self-esteem, and difficulties in math, RDs specifically increased the risk of being victimized (viewed by peers as victims or bully/victims), but the association between RDs and bullying others (viewed by peers as bullies) was no longer significant.
Poor reading skills have also been related to bullying perpetration, although relevant studies are rare. Kaukiainen and his colleagues [60] found that learning difficulties, defined as difficulties in reading and writing, were associated with bullying, but not with victimization. In the study by Turunen et al. [35], poor readers with comorbid externalizing/internalizing problems were involved as bullies and bully/victims, but not as victims, in the beginning of elementary school. Skilled readers and struggling readers without externalizing/internalizing problems were not at elevated risk to be involved in bullying. The authors concluded that difficulties in reading are important indicators of bullying involvement among children in the beginning of elementary school only when occurring in tandem with externalizing/internalizing problems.
Previous studies have some limitations. Studies relying on interviews provide retrospective information about the phenomenon within samples of individuals with RDs only, without a comparison group that would make results more generalizable [i.e., 58, 59]. Turunen et al. [35] studied associations between word reading skills and bullying involvement in the beginning of elementary school, thus providing information only about young children and not taking into account reading comprehension. Although another study by Turunen et al. [36] included also adolescents by utilizing a community sample of elementary and middle school students, the results were cross-sectional and based on self-reports of reading problems instead of reading test scores. Therefore, it is not yet known whether and how poor reading skills are longitudinally related to bullying involvement in adolescence. Moreover, to our knowledge no previous study has examined reading comprehension in relation to bullying involvement.
Transition to middle school for students with reading difficulties
To our knowledge, adjustment or bullying involvement across the transition to middle school among students with poor reading skills has not been studied before. Regarding academic adjustment, studies examining reading skills of general populations have revealed that during the transition from elementary to middle school, students’ reading achievement may stall or even decline to levels below the elementary school achievement [61]. Also, a more general “dip” in the academic progress after transition has been reported [5, 62], although Akos et al. [63] found evidence of the transition effect as an interruption in academic growth rather than decline in the skills. The interruption was larger for vulnerable students, such as students receiving special education.
Although there is lack of research on middle school transition among students with RDs, a few studies have investigated the transition of students with heterogeneous Special Educational Needs (SEN). Compared with their peers, they have more concerns regarding bullying [1], even though their transition experience may not be less successful than that of their peers [1, 64]. However, most studies on adolescents with SEN report more victimization, adjustment and mental health problems, less social support, and academic challenges during the transitional year [1, 28, 65–69]. They also seem to have more internalizing and externalizing problems, and be rejected by their peers [68]. In addition to SEN, lower academic achievement has been related to poorer transition to middle school [8, 70]. Interestingly, a study by Bailey and Baines [71] suggests that high levels of specific resilience factors (i.e., optimism and support) in primary school may leave SEN students less prepared for middle school. Researchers speculate that these students may underestimate the challenges they face, or may lack the skills to adapt because of previous overreliance on support, and thus experience difficulties in adjusting to their new schools.
Adjustment to middle school seems to involve developing a new set of social skills [72], which may be strenuous for students with RDs, since they often have challenges in social skills and competencies to begin with [29–31]. While the difficulties poor readers experience with studying and peers may not be new, they are likely to become more stressful as adolescence is a period of seeking greater autonomy, identity, and more intimate peer relationships [28]. On top of more complex social and learning environment, less support and warmth from teachers [73], and, at least in Finland, fewer special education services are available in middle school compared with elementary school [37]. Since poor reading skills are related to bullying involvement in younger students [35], and transition to middle school poses a risk for social challenges, question arises whether poor reading skills predict victimization and bullying across the elementary to middle school transition.
The present study
The aim of the present study is to investigate cross-lagged associations between reading skills and involvement in bullying across the transition from elementary to middle school. We examine this separately for reading fluency and comprehension. Our goal is to investigate whether poor reading skills predict later involvement in school bullying, after controlling for externalizing and internalizing problems. The analysis is based on a simple model in which reading difficulties and externalizing/internalizing problems accumulate and form a risk of bullying involvement [35, 74–78], and also co-occur [51, 79]. Because externalizing and internalizing problems have been reported to be common among poor readers [e.g. 49–56], are among the strongest risk factors of bullying and victimization [80], respectively, and this increased risk is apparent also across the transition to middle school [81], they are taken into account in the analyses. Firstly, we are interested in the extent to which poor reading skills in the end of elementary school (Grade 6) increase the risk for victimization or bullying after transition to middle school (Grade 7). Moreover, we examine whether poor reading skills pose a risk for victimization or bullying during middle school, between Grade 7 and Grade 9. Based on the previous research, we hypothesize that poor reading skills predict later bullying perpetration (Hypothesis 1) and victimization (Hypothesis 2). These associations are predicted to be stronger across the transition from elementary to middle school (i.e., between Grades 6 and 7; Hypothesis 3) than during middle school. We hypothesize the associations to be similar for reading fluency and comprehension.
Method
Participants and procedure
The present data came from an extensive longitudinal age cohort study [82], in which a community sample of children (n = 1,880) were followed from kindergarten entry (age M = 74.0 ± 3.6 months) to the end of middle school (Grade 9, age 15). In Finland, nine years comprehensive school will start in the year children turn seven years of age. The transition from elementary to middle school takes place between Grades 6 and 7.
The study includes the entire age cohort in one rural municipality and two medium size towns, plus about a half of the age cohort from one big city during Grades 6, 7, and 9, in order to follow them across the transition to middle school. The final sample consists of 1,824 students (47.3% female), whose parents gave an informed consent for their child to participate and who were still participating the study in Grade 6. Of these students, 1,715 (47.3% female) participated also in T2, and 1,647 (47.1% female) in T3. The longitudinal study [82] has been evaluated and approved by the ethics committee of the University of Jyväskylä (June 6, 2006). In the present study, we used data collected from the students reading fluency and comprehension tests instructed by trained research assistants, as well as questionnaires about externalizing/internalizing problems, bullying, and victimization during regular school lessons at Grade 6 (T1, April 2013), Grade 7 (T2, April 2014), and Grade 9 (T3, April 2016).
Measures
Reading fluency.
There were two group-administered tests for the assessment of reading fluency in each grade (T1, T2, and T3): a word reading fluency task and a word-chain task. First, the word reading fluency task is a subtest of the nationally normed reading test battery [ALLU; 83]. In this speed test with a two-minute time limit, up to a maximum of 80 trials could be taken, each involving a picture and four phonologically similar words, with the task being to draw a line to match the picture with the semantically matching word. Form A of the original test was used in T1, and a similarly structured form B created for the purpose of the current study with phonologically more difficult words was used in middle school (T2 and T3). The score was the number of correct responses, reflecting both fluency in reading the stimulus words and accuracy in making a correct choice from the alternatives. The Kuder–Richardson reliability coefficient was .96 at T1, .93 at T2, and .94 at T3.
Second, a time-limited word-chain test was utilized to assess reading fluency. In T1 and T2, the test consists of silently reading a total of 10 rows of word-chains comprising four to six words written together, and marking the word boundaries with a vertical line [84]. The original test form was used in T1, and a similarly structured form created for the purpose of the current study with phonologically more difficult words was used in T2. The number of correctly drawn lines between words during one minute was calculated in each time point (maximum 40). In T3, similar test form from a different test battery for adolescents and adults was used [85]. There were 25 rows of word-chains and the time-limit was 1 min 30 s (maximum 75 points). The Kuder–Richardson reliability coefficient was .96 at T3, which was the only time point with item-level information in the data. The Pearson correlation coefficient was .65 between T1 and T2, .64 between T1 and T3, and .70 between T2 and T3.
The sums of the reading fluency tests were standardized and the total fluency score was calculated for each time point as an average of the two standardized sum scores. The Cronbach’s alphas of the total scores were .69 at T1, .77 at T2, and .79 at T3.
Reading comprehension.
To assess reading comprehension in T1, a subtest of the nationally normed reading test battery [ALLU; 83] was used. In T2 and T3, a similar standardized reading comprehension test developed for middle school was utilized [YKÄ; 86]. All tests had the same aim, instruction, and number of multiple tasks, but different texts and items. The students were asked to silently read a non-fiction text. They answered 11 multiple‐choice questions and one question in which they had to arrange five statements in the correct sequence based on the information gathered from the text. The text contained 557 words in T1, 452 in T2, and 500 in T3. Number of correct answers was calculated (maximum 12). Each participant completed the task at his or her own pace, but the maximum time allotted was 30 min. The Kuder–Richardson reliability coefficient was .65 at T1, .68 at T2, and .63 at T3.
Self-reported bullying and victimization.
Self-reported bullying and victimization were measured at each grade (T1, T2 and T3) with the global, single item bullying and victimization questions from the revised Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire, OBVQ [87]. The global items have been shown to be valid measures of bullying and victimization [88], and they have also been used extensively in Finland [89, 90]. For example, the global victimization question correlates positively with the nine OBVQ items indicating specific forms of victimization [91], as well as with several expected correlates of victimization [92]. The students were explained the definition of bullying, emphasizing its repetitive nature and the power imbalance between bully and victim, and they could also read the definition on the self-report questionnaire. They were asked how often they had been bullied, and how often they had bullied others at school in the last couple of months. Answer was given on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all, 2 = only once or twice, 3 = two or three times a month, 4 = about once a week, and 5 = several times a week). Bullying and victimization were used as continuous variables in the analyses.
Externalizing and internalizing problems.
Externalizing and internalizing problems were measured at each grade (T1, T2, and T3) using the Finnish version of self-report form of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire [SDQ; 93, 94], which has been shown to be a highly valid screening instrument [95], and to have good psychometric properties among Finnish children and adolescents [96, 97]. The SDQ consists of 25 items rated on a 3-point scale (i.e., 0 = not true; 1 = somewhat true; 2 = certainly true), producing scales for hyperactivity/inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, peer problems, and prosociality. To measure externalizing problems, we used the scales for hyperactivity/inattention (five items, e.g., restless, cannot stay still for long) and conduct problems (five items, e.g., often fights with other children or bullies them). The composite score for externalizing problems for each grade was formed as the mean score of the hyperactivity/inattention and conduct problems scales. To measure internalizing problems we used with the emotional symptoms subscale (5 items) of the questionnaire.
The expected factor structures of the SDQ scales and subscales were tested with confirmatory factor analysis. For externalizing problems, a second-order confirmatory factor model was fitted to the data with hyperactivity and conduct problems factors (five items loading on each) loading on the higher-order externalizing problems factor. The five items of emotional symptoms subscale formed the internalizing problems factor. Residual correlations between the same items were allowed in T1, T2, and T3. The original model did not have an acceptable fit (χ2(874) = 2724.10, CFI = .90, TLI = .88, RMSEA = .03, SRMR = .06) and one item of the conduct problems subscale (“I usually do as I am told”) had a very small factor loading (T1: .10, T2: .03, T3: .06). After omitting this item from the scale, the model fitted the data (χ2(750) = 2215.12, CFI = .91, TLI = .90, RMSEA = .03, SRMR = .05) and the results supported the distinctness of externalizing and internalizing (emotional) problems factors. For the lower level factors, the factor loadings ranged as follows: for hyperactivity from .30 to .67 in T1, from .28 to .71 in T2, and from .27 to .75 in T3, for conduct problems from .52 to .60 in T1, from .54 to .70 in T2, and from .57 to .65 in T3, for internalizing problems (emotional symptoms) from .41 to .72 in T1, from .49 to .71 in T2, and from .48 to .76 in T3. For the second order factor of externalizing problems, factor loadings ranged from .72 to .92 in T1, from .72 to .90 in T2, and from .71 to .85 in T3. Externalizing and internalizing problems factors correlated .54 in T1, .63 in T2, and .57 in T3.
Finally, mean scores for externalizing and internalizing problems were calculated at each time point. Externalizing problems subscale was computed as a mean score of 9 items on the hyperactivity and conduct problem scales ranging from 0 to 2 with an Omega (ω) of .73 at T1, .75 at T2, and .77 at T3. Internalizing problems subscale was computed as a mean score of items on the emotional problems scale ranging from 0 to 2 with an Omega (ω) of .70 at T1, .73 at T2, and .75 at T3.
Analysis strategy
The aim of the present study was to investigate how poor reading skills are related to bullying and victimization across the transition from elementary to middle school, controlling for externalizing and internalizing problems. Two longitudinal cross-lagged panel models with 3 time-points (T1, T2, and T3) were fitted to the data separately for reading fluency (Model A) and comprehension (Model B), utilizing Mplus statistical package [Version 7.4; 87]. With cross-lagged panel model it is possible to estimate the directional influence variables have on each other over time, and thus examine the causal influences between variables [98]. This enabled us to test whether poor reading skills longitudinally predict victimization and bullying, and not the other way around.
For predictor variables in T1, the ICCs for classroom level were .13 for reading fluency, .08 for comprehension, .07 for victimization, .09 for bullying, .01 for externalizing problems, and .03 for internalizing problems. This indicates that some of the variability in T1 variables was due to differences between classrooms. However, because we were not interested in associations between classrooms, and because the design effects of the predictors varying between .15 (externalizing problems) and 1.40 (reading fluency) were below the threshold of 2 [99], multilevel modeling was not utilized. Instead, the differences between classrooms were taken into account by using COMPLEX option [100, 101] that estimates the model at the level of the whole sample but corrects for distortions in standard error estimates caused by the clustering of observations (i.e., between-level variation). In order to take into account the maximum amount of data, T1 classroom was used as a cluster. The variables at T1 were grand-mean centered in order to assess the regression coefficients relative to the population average [102]. The goodness of the fit was evaluated with the following absolute and relative fit indices [103]: 1) Chi‐squared test (χ2/df < 2: good fit); 2) the Comparative Fit Index (CFI, >.90: acceptable; >.95: good fit); 3) the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI, CFI, >.90: acceptable; >.95: good fit); 4) root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA, < .06: good fit; < .08 acceptable); and 5) standardized root mean square residual (SRMR, < .05: good fit; < .08 acceptable).
Missing data for T1 ranged from 0.02% (Reading skills) to 1.1% (Bullying), for T2 from 7.2% (Reading skills) to 8.7% (Externalizing and internalizing problems), and for T3 from 11.0% (Reading skills) to 11.8% (Externalizing and internalizing problems). Little’s MCAR test showed that missing values were randomly distributed for all the variables in T1 (χ2(19) = 23.71, p = .207) and in T2 (χ2(25) = 24.15, p = .511), but not in T3 (χ2(22) = 70.11, p < .001). The students that no longer participated in T3 had weaker reading fluency (t(1818) = 4.70, p < .001, d = .37) and comprehension (t(1819) = 4.22, p <. 001, d = .34), more externalizing problems (t(1080) = 3.34, p < .01, d = -.27), and more bullying perpetration (t(1802) = 3.12, p < .01, d = -.25) in T1 compared to those that remained in the study until T3. There was no difference between those who remained and those who did not in victimization or internalizing problems. In order to use all available data to estimate the model without imputing them, full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation with robust standard errors was used to handle the missing data and to account for the non-normality of some of the study variables in the subsequent analyses [100].
Results
The correlations and descriptive statistics of the study variables are presented in Table 1. Reading fluency and comprehension measures correlate moderately with each other at T1, T2, and T3, but negatively with bullying others, as well as with externalizing problems. However, the correlation between fluency and externalizing problems is only significant in middle school (T2 and T3), not in elementary school (T1). Reading measures also correlate weakly negatively with victimization, especially in T3, but neither of them correlates with internalizing problems. Bullying and victimization variables correlate positively with each other, indicating that some students are both victimized and bully others (bully/victims). In addition, bullying others correlates relatively strongly and positively with externalizing problems and to a lesser extent with internalizing problems, and victimization correlates relatively strongly and positively with internalizing problems, but also with externalizing problems. Finally, externalizing and internalizing problems are intercorrelated. Mean scores suggest that mean levels of bullying and victimization are declining with age, especially from Grade 7 to Grade 9, but the levels of externalizing and internalizing problems stay approximately the same.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Intercorrelations and descriptive statistics of the study variables.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249112.t001
In the longitudinal cross-lagged panel model with 3 time-points (T1, T2, and T3), in addition to stability coefficients for all the constructs (reading fluency/comprehension, victimization, bullying, externalizing problems, and internalizing problems), all cross-lagged paths between the constructs were estimated. The constructs at T1, as well as the residuals of the constructs in T2 and T3 were allowed to correlate within each time point.
First, a cross-lagged panel model was fitted for reading fluency (Model A). Originally, the model did not fit the data (χ2(25) = 319.72, CFI = .94, TLI = .78, RMSEA = .08, SRMR = .02). As suggested by the modification indices, autoregressive paths were added from T1 reading fluency to T3 reading fluency, from T1 externalizing problems to T3 externalizing problems, and from T1 internalizing problems to T3 internalizing problems. After these modifications the model fitted the data very well (χ2(22) = 36.18, CFI = 1.00, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .02, SRMR = .01). This final model with the standardized beta coefficients is shown in Fig 1. Correlations between T1 variables, and residual correlations between T2 and T3 variables are presented in Table 2.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 1. Cross-lagged panel model for reading fluency (Model A), significant standardized coefficients.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249112.g001
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. Correlations between T1 variables, and residual correlations between T2 and T3 variables (Model A).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249112.t002
As depicted in Fig 1, in addition to strong autoregressive paths between reading fluency in different time points, fluency is longitudinally negatively associated with bullying others, both between Grades 6 and 7, and between Grades 7 and 9, so that students with poorer reading fluency report bullying others more at the later time point. Fluency is not longitudinally associated with victimization. The association between earlier fluency and later bullying perpetration is similar between Grades 6 and 7, as it is between Grades 7 and 9, and non-significant between fluency and victimization in both comparisons.
For bullying others, autoregressive paths between T1 and T2, as well as between T2 and T3 are approximately of the same magnitude and the unstandardized coefficients do not differ significantly (Δb = -.06, p = .274), but for victimization, the association between T1 and T2 is significantly stronger than between T2 and T3 (Δb = -.19, p < .001). Besides earlier bullying perpetration, externalizing problems is the strongest predictor of later bullying perpetration. Internalizing problems predict victimization across the transition to middle school (from T1 to T2), but not during middle school (from T2 to T3). Regarding the covariates, Grade 7 (T2) fluency is related negatively with externalizing problems and positively with internalizing problems in Grade 9 (T3). On the other hand, externalizing problems relate to weaker fluency and more bullying perpetration later on.
Victimization and internalizing problems are positively related in each time point concurrently (See Fig 1 and Table 2), but victimization does not predict later internalizing problems. The same is true for victimization and externalizing problems, as well as victimization and bullying. Residuals of fluency are concurrently associated negatively with the residuals of internalizing problems in T2 and T3. In T3, the residuals of fluency and victimization are also negatively associated. The positive concurrent associations between the covariates (externalizing/internalizing problems) and bullying and victimization are significant and can be found in Fig 1.
Second, a cross-lagged panel model was fitted for reading comprehension (Model B). Again, the original model had some poor fit-indices (χ2(25) = 233.87, CFI = .95, TLI = .80, RMSEA = .07, SRMR = .03). Parallel with Model A, adding autoregressive paths from T1 reading comprehension to T3 reading comprehension, from T1 externalizing problems to T3 externalizing problems, and from T1 internalizing problems to T3 internalizing problems resulted in a very well-fitting final model shown in Fig 2 with the standardized beta coefficients (χ2(22) = 33.68, CFI = 1.00, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .02, SRMR = .01). Correlations between T1 variables, and residual correlations between T2 and T3 variables are presented in Table 3.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 2. Cross-lagged panel model for reading comprehension (Model B), significant standardized coefficients.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249112.g002
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 3. Correlations between T1 variables, and residual correlations between T2 and T3 variables (Model B).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249112.t003
As Fig 2 shows, autoregressive paths between reading comprehension in different time points are relatively strong, although not as strong as for fluency. Similarly with fluency, comprehension is longitudinally negatively associated with bullying others, both between Grades 6 and 7, and between Grades 7 and 9, but not with victimization. Grade 6 bullying is negatively associated with comprehension in Grade 7. As regard to covariates, there is a reciprocal negative association between comprehension and externalizing problems both between Grades 6 and 7, and between Grades 7 and 9, but comprehension is not longitudinally associated with internalizing problems. Comprehension is concurrently negatively associated with externalizing problems in each time point, with bullying in T1 and T2, with victimization in T3, and also weakly with internalizing problems in T2 (see Fig 2 and Table 3). The longitudinal and concurrent relations between victimization, bullying, externalizing problems and internalizing problems are similar to Model A.
Discussion
The aim of the present study was to investigate the social concomitants of poor reading ability across the transition from elementary to middle school, which takes place in Finland after Grade 6 (12 years of age). This study is the first one to explore associations between poor reading, bullying, and victimization in adolescence, and the first one to take into account reading comprehension when examining the associations with RDs and bullying involvement.
We found that fluency and comprehension relate to bullying involvement quite similarly. Both fluency and comprehension negatively predicted later bullying perpetration, both from Grade 6 to Grade 7 and from Grade 7 and Grade 9. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was confirmed. Reading skills were negatively associated with bullying perpetration, so that students with poorer reading skills reported bullying others more at the later time point, even when externalizing and internalizing problems were controlled for. This is in accordance with the conclusions of Turunen et al. [35] concerning younger elementary school students, that reading difficulties and externalizing/internalizing problems accumulate and form a risk of bullying perpetration. We replicated this finding with a large sample of adolescents and utilizing reading comprehension measures in addition to fluency measures. Our results support the notion that poor reading may trigger frustration and antisocial behavior also towards peers [35, 36]. On the other hand, since in adolescence bullying is also used as a strategy to increase dominance status [10, 22], this may be the motivation behind poor readers’ bullying perpetration. RDs may be a factor threatening one’s social status, and gaining social dominance by academic means may be difficult. Since SEN students worry more than their peers about being bullied in middle school [1], it is possible that also poor readers are particularly concerned about their position in the new group and compensate their worries by aggressive means, such as bullying others. However, it is important to remember that although the association turned consistent, it is small in magnitude. Not all struggling readers end up frustrated, aggressive, or bullying their peers.
Contrary to our expectation, neither fluency nor comprehension were longitudinally associated with victimization and Hypothesis 2 was rejected. In a previous cross-sectional study by Turunen et al. [36], self-reported reading difficulties were related especially to victimization, when difficulties in math and self-esteem were controlled for. Even though the authors controlled for self-esteem, the determining factor in their study may have been the personal experience of having a difficulty that was then depicted by peers as a vulnerability. In addition, several studies interviewing adolescents and adults with RDs about their experiences at school have retrospectively reported frequent victimization memories [i.e., 58, 59]. It is possible that even relatively sporadic events will be later remembered as continuous victimization experiences, or that other factors besides RDs explain these memories. Since there were no comparison groups in these qualitative studies, it is impossible to know whether students with RDs were victimized more often than their peers without difficulties. Nevertheless, we could not replicate these findings in our adolescent sample longitudinally with standardized reading fluency or comprehension test scores as indicators of RDs. This is in line with the research showing that poor academic performance is a significant predictor of bullying behavior, but the same cannot be said about victimization [80]. It seems that, in adolescence, being a poor reader is not likely to make students easy targets for later victimization. However, reading skills and victimization were negatively associated concurrently in Grade 9 in the current study. The forthcoming transition to high school, and decisions and plans related to that (i.e., choosing academic versus vocational track), may demonstrate the difficulties to peers thus exposing struggling readers to victimization in the final year of compulsory education.
Finally, we predicted that reading skills would predict bullying and victimization more strongly across the transition from elementary to middle school (between Grades 6 and 7) than during middle school (between Grades 7 and 9). This was not the case, and Hypothesis 3 was rejected. For both measures of reading skills, the association with bullying was just as strong between Grades 6 and 7 as it was between Grades 7 and 9, and the association between reading skills and victimization was not significant in either comparison. The transition to middle school does not seem to add to the risk poor reading skills pose to bullying or victimization.
We replicated the previous robust findings that externalizing problems predict later bullying perpetration and internalizing problems predict, although less strongly, victimization [80]. Internalizing problems predicted victimization across the transition to middle school, but not during middle school. It seems that students with internalizing symptoms are at increased risk for being bullied upon entering middle school, but that risk abates in later adolescence. Moreover, victimization was concurrently associated with internalizing problems, but it did not predict later internalizing symptoms. Several studies have reported that victimized students exhibit concurrent and long-term psychological problems such as depression, anxiousness and psychosomatic symptoms [i.e. 20, 104–106]. In the present study, internalizing problems were operationalized as concurrent emotional symptoms (i.e., “I am often unhappy, depressed or tearful” or “I have many fears, I am easily scared”). It seems that in this age group, or at least within this relatively short timeframe of three years, students show these kinds of emotional symptoms when they are being bullied, but previous victimization does not predict emotional symptoms one or two years later.
There are three main limitations in this study. Firstly, due to attrition, the sample size decreased from Grade 6 to Grade 9. To take this into account, full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation with robust standard errors was used to handle the missing data in the cross-lagged panel model [100]. Second, bullying and victimization were self-reported and measured with single items. Alternatively, forms of bullying and victimization [87], or peer reports [107] could have been utilized. However, the global items have been shown to be valid measures of bullying and victimization in previous studies [88]. Also, we did not evaluate the levels of victimization and bullying, or the associations between poor reading and bullying or victimization during elementary school before the transition to middle school. Thus, we have no information about whether victimization or bullying levels decreased before Grade 6 as suggested by previous studies [9, 19–23], or whether poor reading predicts bullying perpetration (or victimization) differently during elementary school than across or after the transition to middle school.
Conclusions, implications for practice, and future directions
This study expanded the earlier findings reporting bullying and victimization experiences of students with learning difficulties and academic challenges [47] by investigating bullying involvement among students with poor performance in both reading fluency and comprehension. More specifically, we studied the interplay between reading skills, victimization, and bullying across the transition from elementary to middle school. In comparison to previous studies on bullying among students with RDs that relied on interviews [i.e., 58] self-reports of RDs [36], or focused on reading fluency in the beginning of elementary school [35], we utilized standardized tests of reading fluency and comprehension in a relatively large, longitudinal sample of adolescents. We found that poor reading skills, both fluency and comprehension, were longitudinally associated with bullying perpetration, but not with victimization, when externalizing and internalizing problems were controlled for. Fluency and comprehension relate to bullying involvement very similarly.
There are some practical implications that can be drawn from these results. Although it is troublesome that poor reading skills increase risk for bullying perpetration, it should be kept in mind that the effects seem to be relatively small and other factors such as externalizing problems increase the risk more than poor reading skills do. That said, poor reading may be one factor contributing to a vicious cycle likely to lead to generalized feelings of dissatisfaction with school. On the other hand, as Vaz et al. [69] point out, transition to middle school brings with it a potential new opportunity for schools to provide support to disadvantaged students, as they continue to be at a disadvantage after the transition. Therefore, supporting reading and reading motivation of these students is one tool for educators to invite these students to a school participation unclouded by social problems.
In the future, exploring whether there are differences between schools and classrooms in how RDs predispose to bullying involvement would be important. It is possible that in some schools or classrooms the risk is realized, whereas in others it is not [108]. Even though we did not find longitudinal associations between reading skills and victimization, differences between classrooms and schools are plausible even regarding this. Thus, what characteristics of schools and/or classrooms make the transition from elementary to middle school smooth and successful for social relations of students with RDs? Moreover, what characteristics increase the risk of social problems? Finally, since several studies have now revealed that poor reading skills comprise a risk for bullying involvement, the logical next step is to examine the effects of antibullying interventions among this subgroup of at-risk students, both in elementary and middle school.
Supporting information
S1 File.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249112.s001
(SAV)
Citation: Turunen T, Poskiparta E, Salmivalli C, Niemi P, Lerkkanen M-K (2021) Longitudinal associations between poor reading skills, bullying and victimization across the transition from elementary to middle school. PLoS ONE 16(3): e0249112. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249112
1. Evangelou , Taggart B, Sylva K, Melhuish E, Sammons , Blatchford IS-. What makes a successful transition from primary to secondary school? London: UK: Institute of Education, University of London; 2008.
2. Gillison F, Standage M, Skevington S. Changes in quality of life and psychological need satisfaction following the transition to secondary school. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2008;78(1):149–62. pmid:17535521
3. Pietarinen J. Transfer to and study at secondary school in Finnish school culture: developing schools on the basis of pupils’ experiences. International Journal of Educational Research. 2000;33(4):383–400.
4. Virtanen TE, Vasalampi K, Torppa M, Lerkkanen M-K, Nurmi J-E. Changes in students’ psychological well-being during transition from primary school to lower secondary school: A person-centered approach. Learning and Individual Differences. 2019;69:138–49.
5. McGee C, Ward R, Gibbons J, Harlow A. Transition to secondary school: A literature review. Ministry of Education, New Zealand; 2003.
6. Sirsch U. The impending transition from primary to secondary school: Challenge or threat? International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2003 Sep 1;27(5):385–95.
7. Waters SK, Lester L, Wenden E, Cross D. A theoretically grounded exploration of the social and emotional outcomes of transition to secondary school. Australian Journal of Guidance and Counselling. 2012;22(02):190–205.
8. West P, Sweeting H, Young R. Transition matters: Pupils’ experiences of the primary-secondary school transition in the West of Scotland and consequences for well-being and attainment. Research Papers in Education. 2010;25(1):21–50.
9. Pellegrini AD. Bullying, victimization, and sexual harassment during the transition to middle school. Educational Psychologist. 2002;37(3):151–63.
10. Pellegrini AD, Bartini M. A longitudinal study of bullying, victimization, and peer affiliation during the transition from primary school to middle school. American Educational Research Journal. 2000;37(3):699–725.
11. Ashton R. Improving the transfer to secondary school: How every child’s voice can matter. Support for Learning. 2008;23(4):176–82.
12. Bailey G, Giles RM, Rogers SE. An investigation of the concerns of fifth graders transitioning to middle school. RMLE Online. 2015;38(5):1–12.
13. Rice F, Frederickson N, Seymour J. Assessing pupil concerns about transition to secondary school: Pupil concerns about transition. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2011;81(2):244–63. pmid:21542817
14. Zeedyk MS, Gallacher J, Henderson M, Hope G, Husband B, Lindsay K. Negotiating the transition from primary to secondary school: Perceptions of pupils, parents and teachers. School Psychology International. 2003;24(1):67–79.
15. Olweus D. Sweden. In: Smith PK, Morita Y, Junger-Tas J, Olweus D, Catalano R, Slee P, editors. The nature of school bullying: A cross-national perspective. London & New York: Routledge; 1999. p. 2–27.
16. Ttofi MM, Farrington DP. Effectiveness of school-based programs to reduce bullying: a systematic and meta-analytic review. Journal of Experimental Criminology. 2011 Mar;7(1):27–56.
17. Farmer TW, Hamm JV, Leung M-C, Lambert K, Gravelle M. Early adolescent peer ecologies in rural communities: bullying in schools that do and do not have a transition during the middle grades. J Youth Adolesc. 2011 Sep;40(9):1106–17. pmid:21667294
18. Holmström MR, Olofsson N, Asplund K, Kristiansen L. Transitions in the Swedish school system and the impact on student’s positive self-reported-health. BMC Public Health. 2014;14(1):1045. pmid:25293672
19. Brendgen M, Girard A, Vitaro F, Dionne G, Boivin M. Personal and familial predictors of peer victimization trajectories from primary to secondary school. Developmental Psychology. 2016;52(7):1103–14. pmid:27253264
20. Haltigan JD, Vaillancourt T. Joint trajectories of bullying and peer victimization across elementary and middle school and associations with symptoms of psychopathology. Developmental Psychology. 2014 Nov;50(11):2426–36. pmid:25313592
21. Olweus D. Bully/victim problems among schoolchildren: Basic facts and effects of a school based intervention program. In: Pepler DJ, Rubin KH, editors. The development and treatment of childhood aggression. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1991. p. 411–48.
22. Pellegrini AD, Long JD. A longitudinal study of bullying, dominance, and victimization during the transition from primary school through secondary school. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2002;20(2):259–80.
23. Shell MD, Gazelle H, Faldowski RA. Anxious solitude and the middle school transition: A diathesis x stress model of peer exclusion and victimization trajectories. Dev Psychol. 2014;50(5):1569–83. pmid:24491212
24. Kärnä A, Voeten M, Little TD, Poskiparta E, Alanen E, Salmivalli C. Going to scale: A nonrandomized nationwide trial of the KiVa antibullying program for grades 1–9. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2011;79(6):796–805. pmid:21967491
25. National Institute for Health and Welfare. School Health Promotion Study. Results. Question-specific tables [Internet]. 2019 [cited 2020 Aug 10]. Available from: https://www.thl.fi/fi/web/thlfi-en/research-and-expertwork/population-studies/school-health-promotion-study.
26. Espelage DL, Hong JS, Rao MA, Thornberg R. Understanding ecological factors associated with bullying across the elementary to middle school transition in the United States. Violence and Victims; New York. 2015;30(3):470–87. pmid:26118267
27. Pepler DJ, Craig WM, Connolly JA, Yuile A, McMaster L, Jiang D. A developmental perspective on bullying. Aggressive Behavior. 2006 Aug 1;32(4):376–84. pmid:18392191
28. Wenz-Gross M, Siperstein GN. Students with learning problems at risk in middle school: Stress, social support, and adjustment. Exceptional Children. 1998;65(1):91–100.
29. Kavale KA, Forness SR. Social skill deficits and learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1996;29(3):226–37. pmid:8732884
30. Parhiala P, Torppa M, Eklund K, Aro T, Poikkeus A-M, Heikkilä R, et al. Psychosocial functioning of children with and without dyslexia: A follow-up study from ages four to nine. Dyslexia. 2015;21(3):197–211. pmid:25428888
31. Vallance DD, Cummings RL, Humphries T. Mediators of the risk for problem behavior in children with language learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1998;31(2):160–71. pmid:9529786
32. Humphrey N, Mullins PM. Self-concept and self-esteem in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs. 2002;2(2).
33. Kiuru N, Poikkeus A-M, Lerkkanen M-K, Pakarinen E, Siekkinen M, Ahonen T, et al. Teacher-perceived supportive classroom climate protects against detrimental impact of reading disability risk on peer rejection. Learning and Instruction. 2012;22(5):331–9.
34. Nabuzoka D, Smith PK. Sociometric status and social behaviour of children with and without learning difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1993;34(8):1435–48. pmid:8294529
35. Turunen T, Kiuru N, Poskiparta E, Niemi P, Nurmi J-E. Word reading skills and externalizing and internalizing problems from Grade 1 to Grade 2—Developmental trajectories and bullying involvement in Grade 3. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2019;23(2):161–77.
36. Turunen T, Poskiparta E, Salmivalli C. Are reading difficulties associated with bullying involvement? Learning and Instruction. 2017;52(Supplement C):130–8.
37. Official Statistics of Finland (OSF). Special education [e-publication]. Appendix table 5. Comprehensive school pupils receiving part-time special education in the academic year 2009–2010 by primary reason for special education. Helsinki: Statistics Finland [Internet]. 2010 [cited 2018 Nov 25]; Available from: http://www.stat.fi/til/erop/2010/erop_2010_2011-06-09_tau_005_en.html.
38. Shaywitz SE. Dyslexia. New England Journal of Medicine. 1998;338(5):307–312.
39. Taanila A, Yliherva A, Kaakinen M, Moilanen I, Ebeling H. An epidemiological study on Finnish school-aged children with learning difficulties and behavioural problems. International journal of circumpolar health. 2011;70(1):59–71. pmid:21342613
40. Kairaluoma L, Torppa M, Westerholm J, Ahonen T, Aro M. The nature of and factors related to reading difficulties among adolescents in a transparent orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2013;17(5):315–32.
41. Shaywitz SE, Fletcher JM, Holahan JM, Shneider AE, Marchione KE, Stuebing KK, et al. Persistence of dyslexia: The Connecticut longitudinal study at adolescence. PEDIATRICS. 1999;104(6):1351–9. pmid:10585988
42. Baumeister AL, Storch EA, Geffken GR. Peer victimization in children with learning disabilities. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal. 2008;25(1):11–23.
43. Estell DB, Farmer TW, Irvin MJ, Crowther A, Akos P, Boudah DJ. Students with exceptionalities and the peer group context of bullying and victimization in late elementary school. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2008;18(2):136–50.
44. Fink E, Deighton J, Humphrey N, Wolpert M. Assessing the bullying and victimisation experiences of children with special educational needs in mainstream schools: Development and validation of the Bullying Behaviour and Experience Scale. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2015;36:611–9. pmid:25462521
45. Glew GM, Fan M-Y, Katon W, Rivara FP, Kernic MA. Bullying, psychosocial adjustment, and academic performance in elementary school. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2005;159(11):1026–31.
46. Nakamoto J, Schwartz D. Is peer victimization associated with academic achievement? A meta-analytic review. Social Development. 2010;19(2):221–42.
47. Rose CA, Monda-Amaya LE, Espelage DL. Bullying perpetration and victimization in special education: A review of the literature. Remedial and Special Education. 2011;32(2):114–30.
48. Saylor CF, Leach JB. Perceived bullying and social support in students accessing special inclusion programming. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities. 2009;21(1):69–80.
49. Arnold EM, Goldston DB, Walsh AK, Reboussin BA, Daniel SS, Hickman E, et al. Severity of emotional and behavioral problems among poor and typical readers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2005;33(2):205–17. pmid:15839498
50. Carroll JM, Maughan B, Goodman R, Meltzer H. Literacy difficulties and psychiatric disorders: evidence for comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46(5):524–32. pmid:15845132
51. Morgan PL, Farkas G, Tufis PA, Sperling RA. Are reading and behavior problems risk factors for each other? Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2008;41(5):417–36. pmid:18768774
52. Willcutt EG, Pennington BF. Psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents with reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41(8):1039–48. pmid:11099120
53. Bennett KJ, Brown KS, Boyle M, Racine Y, Offord D. Does low reading achievement at school entry cause conduct problems? Social Science & Medicine. 2003;56(12):2443–8.
54. Fleming CB, Harachi TW, Cortes RC, Abbott RD, Catalano RF. Level and change in reading scores and attention problems during elementary school as predictors of problem behavior in middle school. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 2004;12(3):130–44.
55. McIntosh K, Sadler C, Brown JA. Kindergarten reading skill level and change as risk factors for chronic problem behavior. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions. 2012;14(1):17–28.
56. Trzesniewski KH, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Maughan B. Revisiting the association between reading achievement and antisocial behavior: New evidence of an environmental explanation from a twin study. Child Development. 2006;77(1):72–88. pmid:16460526
57. Ingesson SG. Growing up with dyslexia: Interviews with teenagers and young adults. School Psychology International. 2007;28(5):574–91.
58. Singer E. The strategies adopted by Dutch children with dyslexia to maintain their self-esteem when teased at school. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2005;38:411–23. pmid:16329442
59. Hellendoorn J, Ruijssenaars W. Personal experiences and adjustment of Dutch adults with dyslexia. Remedial and Special Education. 2000;21(4):227–39.
60. Kaukiainen A, Salmivalli C, Lagerspetz K, Tamminen M, Vauras M, Mäki H, et al. Learning difficulties, social intelligence, and self–concept: Connections to bully–victim problems. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2002;43(3):269–78. pmid:12184482
61. Hopwood B, Hay I, Dyment J. Students’ reading achievement during the transition from primary to secondary school. Australian Journal of Language & Literacy. 2017;40(1):46–58.
62. Galton MJ, Gray J, Ruddick J. The impact of school transitions and transfers on pupil progress and attainment. Nottingham: UK: DfEE; 1999.
63. Akos P, Rose RA, Orthner D. Sociodemographic moderators of middle school transition effects on academic achievement. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2015;35(2):170–98.
64. Forgan JW, Vaughn S. Adolescents with and without LD make the transition to middle school. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2000;33(1):33–43. pmid:15505954
65. Evans D, Borriello GA, Field AP. A review of the academic and psychological impact of the transition to secondary education. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2018 [cited 2019 Apr 1];9. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01482/full. pmid:29410642
66. Hughes LA, Banks P, Terras MM. Secondary school transition for children with special educational needs: a literature review. Support for Learning. 2013 Feb 1;28(1):24–34.
67. Martínez RS. Social support in inclusive middle schools: Perceptions of youth with learning disabilities. Psychology in the Schools. 2006 Feb 1;43(2):197–209.
68. Tur-Kaspa H. The socioemotional adjustment of adolescents with LD in the Kibbutz during high school transition periods. J Learn Disabil. 2002;35(1):87–96. pmid:15490902
69. Vaz S, Parsons R, Falkmer T, Passmore AE, Falkmer M. The impact of personal background and school contextual factors on academic competence and mental health functioning across the primary-secondary school transition. PLOS ONE. 2014;9(3):e89874. pmid:24608366
70. Anderson LW, Jacobs J, Schramm S, Splittgerber F. School transitions: beginning of the end or a new beginning? International Journal of Educational Research. 2000;33(4):325–39.
71. Bailey S, Baines E. The impact of risk and resiliency factors on the adjustment of children after the transition from primary to secondary school. Educational & Child Psychology. 2012;29(1):47–63.
72. January AM, Casey RJ, Paulson D. A meta-analysis of classroom-wide interventions to build social skills: Do they work? School Psychology Review. 2011;40(2):16.
73. Hughes JN, Cao Q. Trajectories of teacher-student warmth and conflict at the transition to middle school: Effects on academic engagement and achievement. Journal of School Psychology. 2018;67:148–62. pmid:29571530
74. Evans GW, Li D, Whipple SS. Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin. 2013;139(6):1342–96. pmid:23566018
75. Rönkä A, Kinnunen U, Pulkkinen L. Continuity in problems of social functioning in adulthood: A cumulative perspective. Journal of Adult Development. 2001 Jul 1;8(3):161–71.
76. Rutter M. Protective factors in children’s responses to stress and disadvantage. Annals of the Academy of Medicine. 1979;8:324–338. pmid:547874
77. Rutter M. Stress, coping and development: Some issues and some questions*. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1981 Oct 1;22(4):323–56. pmid:7287844
78. Sameroff AJ. Identifying risk and protective factors for healthy child development. In: Clarke-Stewart A, Dunn J, editors. Families count Effects on child and adolescent development. Cambridge University Press; 2006. p. 53–76.
79. Vaillancourt T, Brittain HL, McDougall P, Duku E. Longitudinal links between childhood peer victimization, internalizing and externalizing problems, and academic functioning: Developmental cascades. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013 Aug 2;41(8):1203–15. pmid:23907699
80. Cook CR, Williams KR, Guerra NG, Kim TE, Sadek S. Predictors of bullying and victimization in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic investigation. School Psychology Quarterly. 2010;25(2):65–83.
81. Farmer TW, Irvin MJ, Motoca LM, Leung M-C, Hutchins BC, Brooks DS, et al. Externalizing and internalizing behavior problems, peer affiliations, and bullying involvement across the transition to middle school. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 2015;23(1):3–16.
82. Lerkkanen M-K, Niemi P, Poikkeus A-M, Poskiparta E, Siekkinen M, Nurmi J-E. The First Steps Study [Alkuportaat]. Finland: University of Jyväskylä, University of Turku, and University of Eastern Finland.; 2006.
83. Lindeman J. ALLU-Ala-asteen lukutesti [ALLU–Reading Test for Primary School]. University of Turku, Finland: The Center for Learning Research; 1998.
84. Nevala J, Lyytinen H. Sanaketjutesti [Word Chain Test]. Jyväskylä, Finland: Niilo Mäki Instituutti & Jyväskylan yliopiston Lapsitutkimuskeskus; 2000.
85. Holopainen L, Kairaluoma L, Nevala J, Ahonen T, Aro M. Lukivaikeuksien seulontamenetelmä nuorille ja aikuisille [Screening test battery for reading difficulties for adolescents and adults]. Jyväskylä, Finland: Niilo Mäki Institute; 2004.
86. Lerkkanen M-K, Eklund K, Löytynoja H, Aro M, Poikkeus A-M. YKÄ—Luku‐ ja kirjoitustaidon arviointimenetelmä yläkouluun [YKÄ—Reading test for lower secondary school]. Jyväskylä, Finland: Niilo Mäki Instituutti; 2018.
87. Olweus D. The Revised Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire. Research Center for Health Promotion (HEMIL Center). Bergen, Norway: University of Bergen; 1996.
88. Solberg ME, Olweus D. Prevalence estimation of school bullying with the Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire. Aggressive Behavior. 2003;29(3):239–68.
89. Kärnä A, Voeten M, Little TD, Poskiparta E, Kaljonen A, Salmivalli C. A large-scale evaluation of the KiVa antibullying program: Grades 4–6: Evaluation of KiVa antibullying program. Child Development. 2011 Jan;82(1):311–30. pmid:21291444
90. Raaska H, Lapinleimu H, Sinkkonen J, Salmivalli C, Matomäki J, Mäkipää S, et al. Experiences of school bullying among internationally adopted children: Results from the Finnish adoption (FINADO) study. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2012 Aug 1;43(4):592–611. pmid:22362203
91. Salmivalli C, Kärnä A, Poskiparta E. Counteracting bullying in Finland: The KiVa program and its effects on different forms of being bullied. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2011;35(5):405–11.
92. Sainio M, Veenstra R, Huitsing G, Salmivalli C. Victims and their defenders: A dyadic approach. International Journal of Behavioral. 2011 Mar 1;35(2):144–51.
93. Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581–6. pmid:9255702
94. Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40(11):1337–45.
95. Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;177:534–9.
96. Koskelainen M, Sourander A, Kaljonen A. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire among Finnish school-aged children and adolescents. European child & adolescent psychiatry. 2000;9(4):277–84. pmid:11202103
97. Koskelainen M, Sourander A, Vauras M. Self-reported strengths and difficulties in a community sample of Finnish adolescents. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;10(3):180–5.
98. Kearney M. Cross-Lagged Panel Analysis. In: Allen Mike, editor. The SAGE Encyclopedia of Communication Research Methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc; 2017.
99. Muthén BO, Satorra A. Complex sample data in structural equation modeling. Sociological Methodology. 1995;25:267–316.
100. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide [Internet]. Seventh Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998 [cited 2017 Apr 6]. Available from: https://www.statmodel.com/download/usersguide/MplusUserGuideVer_7.pdf.
101. McNeish D, Stapleton LM, Silverman RD. On the unnecessary ubiquity of hierarchical linear modeling. Psychological Methods. 2017 Mar;22(1):114–40. pmid:27149401
102. Enders CK, Tofighi D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods. 2007;12(2):121–38. pmid:17563168
103. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999;6(1):1–55.
104. Hawker DSJ, Boulton MJ. Twenty years’ research on peer victimization and psychosocial maladjustment: A meta-analytic review of cross-sectional studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41(4):441–55. pmid:10836674
105. Kaltiala-Heino R, Rimpelä M, Rantanen P, Rimpelä A. Bullying at school—an indicator of adolescents at risk for mental disorders. Journal of Adolescence. 2000;23(6):661–74. pmid:11161331
106. Reijntjes A, Kamphuis JH, Prinzie P, Telch MJ. Peer victimization and internalizing problems in children: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Child Abuse Negl. 2010 Apr;34(4):244–52. pmid:20304490
107. Salmivalli C, Voeten M. Connections between attitudes, group norms, and behaviour in bullying situations. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2004;28(3):246–58.
108. Kärnä A, Voeten M, Poskiparta E, Salmivalli C. Vulnerable children in varying classroom contexts: bystanders’ behaviors moderate the effects of risk factors on victimization. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2010 Aug 6;56(3):261–82.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2021 Turunen et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
About the Authors: Tiina Turunen Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft * E-mail: [email protected] Affiliation: Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland ORCID logo https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1641-2347 Elisa Poskiparta Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision Affiliation: Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland Christina Salmivalli Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision Affiliations Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland, Shandong Normal University, Jinan, China Pekka Niemi Roles Conceptualization, Supervision Affiliation: Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland Marja-Kristiina Lerkkanen Roles Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources Affiliations Department of Teacher Education, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylän, Finland, University of Stavanger, Stavanger, Norway Introduction Early adolescence is a period of change in many domains. [...]struggling with the skill needed in school every day is likely to influence the overall experience of school, making it burdensome and aversive, and affecting emotions [e.g. 49–52] and behavior [53–56] at school. [36] included also adolescents by utilizing a community sample of elementary and middle school students, the results were cross-sectional and based on self-reports of reading problems instead of reading test scores. [...]it is not yet known whether and how poor reading skills are longitudinally related to bullying involvement in adolescence. [...]to our knowledge no previous study has examined reading comprehension in relation to bullying involvement.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer