1. Introduction
Sustaining mental health and avoiding mental ill-health has become a significant challenge in the modern world. In 2017, it was estimated that 792 million people worldwide (i.e., 10.7% of the world’s population) lived with a mental health disorder [1]. The current COVID-19 pandemic has exacerbated mental health challenges globally [2,3,4], including for elite athletes [5,6]. Assessing mood fluctuations over time is a long-established strategy for monitoring mental health [7] and establishing appropriate test norms for mood scales is a critical aspect of the process. Test norms help to determine the relative standing of an individual who has taken a test, providing the basis for comparing the raw scores of an individual to what is normal for a given population [8].
Mood profiling, whereby individual mood scores are plotted against test norms to create a graphical profile, is used to identify commonly occurring patterns of mood responses. This is often the case in investigations of the antecedents, correlates, and behavioural consequences of moods; in particular, to assess relationships between moods, performance, and psychological wellbeing of athletes and exercisers [9,10]. In this context, studies using the Brunel Mood Scale (BRUMS) [11,12] or the Profile of Mood States (POMS) [13,14] have identified several distinct mood profiles. For example, the iceberg profile, characterized by high scores for vigour, combined with low scores for tension, depression, anger, fatigue, and confusion, has been associated with positive mental health and good athletic performance [15,16]. The inverse iceberg profile, characterized by below average vigour scores, combined with above average tension, depression, anger, fatigue, and confusion scores, is associated with underperformance and risk of pathogenesis [17]. A third mood profile, termed the Everest profile, is characterized by higher vigour scores than the iceberg profile and lower scores for tension, depression, anger, fatigue, and confusion, and is associated with superior performance [18].
More recently, four additional mood profiles were reported in the literature, referred to as the inverse Everest, shark fin, surface, and submerged profiles [19]. The inverse Everest profile is characterized by low vigour, high tension and fatigue, and very high depression, anger, and confusion; the shark fin profile by below average tension, depression, anger, vigour, and confusion, combined with high fatigue; the surface profile by average scores on all mood dimensions; and the submerged profile by below average scores on all mood dimensions. These four profiles, together with the iceberg and inverse iceberg profiles, have been replicated in several different contexts, including English-speaking and Italian-speaking sport participants [20,21], among a representative sample of the Singaporean population [22], and among the large, heterogeneous sample interrogated in the present study [23], suggesting that the profile clusters are robust across different language and cultural contexts. Figure 1 shows the prevalence of the six mood profiles reported by Terry and colleagues [23].
Mood profiling scales, such as the BRUMS, have many applications globally. The measure has been used to, for example, quantify affective responses to music in Australia [24]; screen for risk of post-traumatic stress disorder among military personnel in South Africa [25]; evaluate population-level mental health and monitor the psychological wellbeing of cardiac rehabilitation patients in Brazil [26,27]; manage performance anxiety and prevent injuries among adolescent ballet dancers in Japan [28]; and assess youth for elevated suicide risk in the USA [29]. A variety of BRUMS applications are prevalent in sport, including performance prediction; monitoring athlete mindset; assessing mood responses to poor performance, training load, injury, or long haul travel; screening for risk of overtraining, eating disorders, and other pathogenic conditions; and as a general catalyst for discussion between an athlete and sport psychologist [18].
Since the original norms for the BRUMS were generated, much larger samples of the population have been tested. Previous analysis of the large, heterogeneous dataset used in the present investigation [23] showed that the mean scores deviated significantly from existing normative data on all subscales, highlighting the need for refinement of the norms. Further, significant between-group differences were evident for sex, age group, and educational status, suggesting that separate norms should be considered for specific demographic groups. Finally, it has long been established that athletes typically report mood profiles that vary from other populations [12,30], pointing to the need for athlete-specific norms.
The primary purpose of the current study was to improve existing norms for the BRUMS [31] using a large internet sample of participants. The secondary purpose was to discuss how mood profiling can be used to promote sustainable mental health among athletes. Our study was predicated on the production of improved norms for the BRUMS and therefore no other measures of mood were considered.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
A heterogeneous sample of 15,692 participants was recruited, which included approximately equal representation of males (n = 8450; 53.8%) and females (n = 7242; 46.2%). The age group distribution of participants was 18–24 yr (n = 9765; 62.2%), 25–35 yr (n = 3212; 20.5%), 36–45 yr (n = 1348; 8.6%), 46–55 yr (n = 916; 5.8%), and 56+ (n = 451; 2.9%). The ethnic composition of the sample was Caucasian (n = 8465; 53.9%), Asian (n = 2987; 19.0%), African (n = 945; 6.0%), Middle Eastern (n = 384; 2.4%), Indigenous (n = 147; 0.9%), and Other (n = 2764; 17.6%). A total of 3120 participants (19.9%) of the sample were athletes, and 12,572 were non-athletes (80.1%).
2.2. Measures
The 24-item Brunel Mood Scale (BRUMS) was used to assess mood responses. It has six subscales (tension, depression, anger, vigour, fatigue, and confusion) of four mood descriptors each, with respondents rating how they feel “right now” on a 5-point Likert-type scale anchored by 0 = not at all and 4 = extremely. During its development, the BRUMS showed robust psychometric properties across samples of adult students, adult athletes, young athletes, and schoolchildren [11,12], and has since demonstrated psychometric integrity for use with many other population groups. It has been translated and validated in several languages and cultural contexts, including Afrikaans [32], Brazilian Portuguese [33], Chinese [34], Czech [35], French [36], Hungarian [37], Italian [37,38], Japanese [39], Malay [40,41], Persian [42], Singaporean [43], Spanish [44], and Turkish [45]. The BRUMS does not provide a comprehensive assessment of mood in its broadest sense [46], so researchers using the measure are cautioned not to extrapolate findings beyond the six specific mood dimensions assessed. The In The Mood website [47] facilitates completion of the BRUMS online, providing an instant calculation and interpretation of individual mood responses. An automated report is generated, including raw and standard scores, reference to normative scores, a graphical representation of the individual mood profile, and suggested evidence-based mood regulation strategies, where appropriate. Adult participants (≥18 years) were recruited using a snowballing strategy and directed to the In The Mood website during the period March 2011 to January 2018. Data analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27 [48].
3. Results
3.1. Data Screening
The In The Mood website requires respondents to answer all items prior to submission, and therefore no missing values were detected. The full range of possible subscale scores from 0–16 was evident for all subscales. Data were screened for implausible responses (e.g., scoring 0 or 16 on every subscale) and cases deleted where identified. Significant deviation from univariate normality was evident for some subscales (e.g., depression), which is common for negative mood score distributions [12]. Given the very large sample size, it was judged that the level of non-normality identified would not unduly affect the findings.
3.2. Between-Group Differences and Generation of Revised Norms
Previous findings [23] showed that mean mood scores for the sample overall deviated significantly from existing normative data on all six subscales, with effects sizes ranging from 0.07 to 0.29. Although the effect sizes were small [49], differences of this magnitude in such a large dataset were judged to warrant the generation of revised tables of normative data. Revised norms in the form of standard T-scores were produced using the formula T = 50 + 10z [50].
Single-factor MANOVAs of mood scores by sex (male/female) and athletic status (athlete/non-athlete) produced significant main effects for sex (Wilks’ Λ = 0.968; F = 85.70, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.032) and athletic status (Wilks’ Λ = 0.976; F = 64.42, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.024; see Table 1). Tension, fatigue, and confusion scores were significantly higher among females (p < 0.001), whereas anger (p = 0.003) and vigour scores (p < 0.001) were significantly higher among males. No sex differences were found for depression. Athletes reported significantly higher scores than non-athletes (p < 0.001) for tension, depression, anger, vigour, and confusion. No athletic status difference was found for fatigue. Effect sizes were mostly negligible, and the maximum variance explained by any univariate comparison was 1.8%. A 2-way MANOVA showed a significant multivariate interaction effect (F = 5.34, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.002). Significant interactions were found for fatigue (p < 0.001) and confusion (p = 0.004). Female athletes reported the highest fatigue scores whereas male athletes reported the lowest. Female athletes reported higher confusion scores than the other three groups. Given the number of significant between-group differences, separate norms were generated for each of the four groups.
The generation of group-specific norms was restricted in this study to sex and athletic status. Separate norms by age group and educational status were not generated because (a) between-group differences were generally very small for age and education, and more importantly because (b) the resultant large number of highly specific tables of norms would have yielded very little practical value.
Table 2, Table 3, Table 4, Table 5 and Table 6 contain revised norms for the overall sample, for male and female athletes, and for male and female non-athletes. The norms reflect differences in raw scores both within and across groups. For example, among female athletes, a T-score of 50 equates to a raw score of 4 for tension but a raw score of 8 for vigour (see Table 3). Similarly, a T-score of 50 equates to a raw score of 5 for fatigue among male athletes but a raw score of 6 among female athletes (see Table 2 and Table 3). Figure A1 in Appendix A shows the overall norms in a format that allows the profile for an individual or group to be plotted graphically. Equivalent profile sheets for male and female athletes and male and female non-athletes are contained in Figure A2 to Figure A5.
4. Discussion
The mental health of athletes, especially at the elite level, has come under intense scrutiny in recent years [51,52,53,54,55], although the International Olympic Committee consensus statement on mental health among elite athletes [55] pointed out that “there are no evidence-based or consensus-based guidelines for diagnosis and management of mental health symptoms and disorders in elite athletes” (p. 667). Given this gap in the provision of reliable guidelines for diagnosis of mental health issues, strategies such as mood profiling that provide indicators of increased risk of psychopathology become especially valuable in protecting mental wellbeing in a sustainable way. This point has been emphasized in recent calls for early intervention frameworks to be implemented in elite sport environments: “Early detection of, and intervention for, mental health symptoms is essential in the elite sporting context” [56]. Indeed, psychological measures have relevance as part of broader health-screening programs for athletes, aimed at maintaining both physical and mental health and, in this context, mood profiling has been shown to have utility in predicting athletic injury [57,58].
The prevalence of mental health symptoms and disorders among elite athletes broadly reflects the prevalence rates found in the general population [59]. A recent meta-analysis showed the prevalence among current elite athletes ranged from 19% for alcohol misuse to 34% for anxiety/depression, and from 16% for distress to 26% for anxiety/depression among former elite athletes [60]. With prevalence rates at such alarmingly high levels, regular screening of athlete populations for mental ill-health indicators represents an essential element of high-performance environments [52]. Mood profiling may also have a role to play in the detection of potentially life-threatening mental illnesses, such as eating disorders. Although involvement in sport may offer some protection against disordered eating, sub-clinical and clinical eating disorders are generally shown to be more prevalent in athletes than the general population, more prevalent among female athletes than male athletes, and more prevalent in leanness-dependent and weight-dependent sports than in other sports [61,62,63,64]. Further, mood disorders, especially depression and anxiety, are among the most common comorbidities associated with eating disorders [65] and therefore screening for such symptoms via mood profiling offers a quick and simple method for identifying candidates for follow-up clinical interview.
In terms of the specific mood profiles identified in this paper (Figure 1) and previously [19,20,21,22,23], the inverse Everest and inverse iceberg profiles, both typified by high or very high scores for tension, depression, anger, fatigue, and confusion, would logically represent reliable indicators of elevated risk of mental health concerns. However, such profiles may represent acute emotional responses to a specific incident (e.g., a devastating sporting defeat or a debilitating injury) after which emotions might subside quite quickly, rather than a more enduring state of mind indicative of mental ill-health. Hence, it is recommended that periodic mood profiling repeated over time provides a better indicator of mental wellbeing. To discuss the practicalities of how mood profiling can be used, reference is made below to several real-life examples from the first author’s experiences of working with elite athletes over several decades, including providing on-site psychological support at nine Olympic Games and more than 100 other international sporting events.
Figure 2 shows the mood profiles of a medal-winning athlete during an Olympic Winter Games. The athlete initially reported a submerged profile during the 15-day to 12-day pre-competition phase, with scores for all mood dimensions sitting below the athlete population mean. Having worked with the athlete over several years, this profile was recognizable as him feeling flat and emotionally drained from inter-continental travel and a period of hard physical training but represented no threat to mental wellbeing. During the 9-day to 6-day pre-competition phase, scores for depression and anger spiked, signalling the need for some form of intervention. Discussions with the athlete identified the source of the feelings as relating to a family issue that he felt powerless to resolve, given the impending Olympic competition and his physical distance from home. Considering the truism that “at the Olympics, everything is a performance issue” [66], the situation represented both a threat to mental wellbeing and to successful performance. Once strategies that at least partly resolved the family issue has been implemented, his mood improved incrementally towards the crucial second day of competition when the more desirable iceberg profile was evident, except for tension being marginally higher than the norm.
Figure 3 shows the mood profiles of an Olympic champion throughout the Games period. Mood profiling commenced the day after arrival in the Olympic city and continued until the morning of competition. The profiles show an early adjustment to the effects of long-distance travel and associated jetlag, in the form of a shark fin profile. Next, an extended period of stable positive mood is evident, in the form of successive iceberg profiles, with a slight increase in tension as competition approached. The optimum mood for this athlete had been identified at previous competitions and therefore the challenge was to identify when the optimum mood was achieved and to maintain it using a variety of preplanned strategies. The athlete had found the previous Olympic Games to be emotionally overwhelming and therefore he opted out of the Olympic Village environment, instead staying in a rented house with a fellow athlete, the team coaches, and the team psychologist (the first author). The athlete used his own transport, shopped for his own food, and a “home away from home” environment was created, which helped to stabilize his mood. Notably, his standard scores for depression, anger, and confusion represented raw scores of zero on those mood dimensions throughout the entire Games period.
Figure 4 shows the post-competition mood profiles of an underperforming Olympian. His mood the day after a disappointing Olympic competition showed an extreme inverse Everest profile, representing a clear threat to his mental wellbeing. Successive profiles taken in the following days showed the progress of his emotional recovery, although even his most positive profile during the period, at 12 days post-competition, approximated an inverse iceberg, representing a continuing elevated mental health risk. Even years later, the memory of that underperformance still haunts the athlete but thankfully his mental wellbeing remains intact.
Figure 5 shows the mood profiles of a 6-time world champion over a 2-year period, during which time she received a clinical diagnosis of chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) [17,67]. Having received advice from an eminent sports medic that “her body was telling her that it’s time to stop competing”, she experienced a rapid decline in mental health, reflected in an inverse Everest profile two years out from her eventual return to competition. She tried multiple unsuccessful clinical and alternative therapies over the next year, during which time her mental ill-health continued, and her mood profiles remained extremely negative.
She eventually committed to an extended period of reverse therapy [68], which treats CFS as a disorder of the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis and offers an educative process focusing on, for example, the link between emotions and health, and finding a balance between training load and life demands. She recounted how the same personal strengths that helped her become world champion—commitment, single-mindedness, focus, and dedication—contributed to the demise of her physical and mental health [69]. Her vigilance, bordering on obsession, in always doing the right thing in training, diet, sleep patterns, and so on, drained her happiness and love of sport to the point where she felt permanently exhausted.
Through reverse therapy, her mental and physical health progressively improved, reflected in more positive mood profiles, until she was ready to return to competition. Her mood responses leading into the world championship showed rising vigour scores and decreasing fatigue scores. Depression and anger scores remained stable at minimum levels. Confusion was eliminated by race day as her race plan became more clearly understood and well-rehearsed. Tension fluctuated but remained within the normal range. Her prerace iceberg profile was consistent with her previous successful performances and she achieved one of the most convincing victories of her career.
In summary, mental health issues are likely to be experienced in one form or another by 30–50% of elite athletes during their lifetime [60,70,71]. Symptoms tend to first manifest in the late teens to early twenties age range, often with recurrent episodes later in life [71]. Common mental health issues experienced by athletes include depression, anxiety, eating disorders, chronic fatigue, alcohol misuse, and sleep disorders [51,60,70,71]. Each of these conditions is likely to manifest in mood decrements, and hence negative mood profiles (i.e., inverse Everest, inverse iceberg, and to a lesser extent the shark fin profile) indicate elevated risk of a potential mental health issue. Factors that protect against mental health issues among elite athletes include positive interpersonal relationships within sport and athletes’ private lives, adequate recovery from the physical demands of training and competition that are an inherent part of elite sport, and feelings of autonomy over sporting careers and lives more generally [72]. Mood profiling has a long history of use to help guard against risk of overtraining and burnout among athletes [16,17] and as a general monitor of psychological wellbeing [18,73]. The examples provided above describe how mood profiling has been used with specific elite athletes to help safeguard both the performance and mental health of the individuals concerned. Table 7 contains recommendations to applied practitioners for using mood profiling in the pursuit of sustainable mental health for athletes.
Although the focus of this paper has been on mental health among athletes, especially at the elite level, the same principles of using mood profiling to monitor emotional responses apply to everyday life and to non-athletes. Results of previous studies [4,19,21,22] have confirmed that the same six mood profile clusters evident among athletes are also evident in the normal population, and across different language and cultural groups. More specifically, the six mood profile clusters shown in Figure 1 have also been identified among Italian athletes and exercisers [21], among athletes and non-athletes in Singapore [22] and China [82], and among youth athletes in Brazil [83]. Evidence has accrued related to the benefits of the iceberg profile for performance in sport [84,85] and academic examinations [86,87]. The iceberg profile has also long been associated with athlete mental health [7,73]. Conversely, an increased risk of mental ill-health has been established for athletes and nonathletes reporting the inverse iceberg and especially the inverse Everest profiles [17,25]. Little is known, however, about the antecedents, correlates, and behavioural consequences of the shark fin, submerged, and surface profiles, and further investigation of those profiles is warranted. Finally, it is recommended that researchers and applied practitioners who use the BRUMS for mood assessment apply the revised tables of normative data reported herein rather than those reported previously [12,31].
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, P.C.T. and R.L.P.-S.; methodology, P.C.T.; validation, P.C.T. and R.L.P.-S.; formal analysis, P.C.T. and R.L.P.-S.; investigation, P.C.T. and R.L.P.-S.; resources, P.C.T.; data curation, R.L.P.-S.; writing—original draft preparation, P.C.T. and R.L.P.-S.; writing—review and editing, P.C.T. and R.L.P.-S.; visualization, P.C.T. and R.L.P.-S.; project administration, P.C.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research received no external funding.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Southern Queensland (approval numbers H11REA023, H13REA169, H16REA015).
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement
Data supporting the reported results can be accessed at
Acknowledgments
The intellectual contribution of the late Gerry Tehan to an earlier stage of this project is acknowledged. We thank the three reviewers for their constructive feedback, which helped to improve the quality of our manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Appendix A
Figure A1
BRUMS Profile Sheet (Population Norms).
[Figure omitted. See PDF]
Figure A2
BRUMS Profile Sheet (Male Athletes).
[Figure omitted. See PDF]
Figure A3
BRUMS Profile Sheet (Female Athletes).
[Figure omitted. See PDF]
Figure A4
BRUMS Profile Sheet (Male Non-Athletes).
[Figure omitted. See PDF]
Figure A5
BRUMS Profile Sheet (Female Non-Athletes).
[Figure omitted. See PDF]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Figures and Tables
Figure 4. Post-competition mood profiles of an underperforming athlete at the Olympic Summer Games.
Comparison of the BRUMS scores by sex and athlete/non-athlete status.
Source | n (%) | M | SD | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|
Sex (Wilks’Λ = 0.968, F (6, 15685) = 85.70 †, partial η2 = 0.032) | ||||
Male | 8450 (53.8) | |||
Tension | 47.11 † | 8.60 | (46.92, 47.29) | |
Depression | 52.09 | 12.07 | (51.83, 52.34) | |
Anger | 52.44 § | 10.58 | (52.21, 52.66) | |
Vigour | 50.41 † | 9.54 | (50.21, 50.61) | |
Fatigue | 51.98 † | 9.39 | (51.78, 52.18) | |
Confusion | 51.34 † | 10.54 | (51.11, 51.56) | |
Female | 7242 (46.2) | |||
Tension | 48.35 | 9.14 | (48.14, 48.56) | |
Depression | 52.47 | 12.43 | (52.19, 52.76) | |
Anger | 51.94 | 10.12 | (51.71, 52.17) | |
Vigour | 48.00 | 9.40 | (47.79, 48.22) | |
Fatigue | 53.64 | 9.78 | (53.42, 53.87) | |
Confusion | 52.00 | 11.12 | (51.74, 52.25) | |
Athlete Status (Wilks’Λ = 0.976, F (6, 15685) = 64.42 †, partial η2 = 0.024) | ||||
Athletes | 3120 (19.9) | |||
Tension | 48.43 † | 9.05 | (48.12, 48.74) | |
Depression | 52.88 † | 12.24 | (52.45,53.31) | |
Anger | 53.70 † | 11.17 | (53.33,54.06) | |
Vigour | 51.89 † | 9.28 | (51.56,52.23) | |
Fatigue | 52.48 | 9.30 | (52.14,52.82) | |
Confusion | 52.21 † | 10.49 | (51.83,52.59) | |
Non-athletes | 12,572 (80.1) | |||
Tension | 47.50 | 8.82 | (47.34, 47.65) | |
Depression | 52.11 | 12.23 | (51.90, 52.33) | |
Anger | 51.84 | 10.13 | (51.66, 52.02) | |
Vigour | 48.65 | 9.51 | (48.49, 48.82) | |
Fatigue | 52.82 | 9.68 | (52.65, 52.98) | |
Confusion | 51.50 | 10.88 | (51.31, 51.69) |
§p < 0.008, † p < 0.001.
Table 2BRUMS normative scores for the whole sample (n = 15,692).
Raw Score | T-Score | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tension | Depression | Anger | Vigour | Fatigue | Confusion | |
0 | 41 | 43 | 43 | 31 | 37 | 42 |
1 | 44 | 46 | 46 | 33 | 39 | 45 |
2 | 47 | 50 | 49 | 36 | 42 | 48 |
3 | 50 | 53 | 53 | 38 | 44 | 52 |
4 | 53 | 56 | 56 | 41 | 47 | 55 |
5 | 56 | 59 | 59 | 44 | 49 | 58 |
6 | 59 | 62 | 62 | 46 | 52 | 61 |
7 | 62 | 65 | 65 | 49 | 54 | 64 |
8 | 65 | 69 | 69 | 52 | 57 | 68 |
9 | 68 | 72 | 72 | 54 | 59 | 71 |
10 | 71 | 75 | 75 | 57 | 62 | 74 |
11 | 74 | 78 | 78 | 59 | 65 | 77 |
12 | 77 | 81 | 81 | 62 | 67 | 81 |
13 | 80 | 84 | 85 | 65 | 70 | 84 |
14 | 83 | 88 | 88 | 67 | 72 | 87 |
15 | 86 | 91 | 91 | 70 | 75 | 90 |
16 | 89 | 94 | 94 | 72 | 77 | 93 |
BRUMS normative scores for male athletes (n = 2243).
Raw Score | T-Score | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tension | Depression | Anger | Vigour | Fatigue | Confusion | |
0 | 41 | 43 | 42 | 27 | 37 | 41 |
1 | 44 | 46 | 45 | 29 | 40 | 45 |
2 | 47 | 49 | 48 | 32 | 42 | 48 |
3 | 50 | 53 | 51 | 35 | 45 | 52 |
4 | 53 | 56 | 54 | 38 | 48 | 55 |
5 | 56 | 59 | 57 | 40 | 50 | 58 |
6 | 59 | 63 | 60 | 43 | 53 | 62 |
7 | 62 | 66 | 63 | 46 | 56 | 65 |
8 | 65 | 69 | 66 | 48 | 59 | 68 |
9 | 68 | 72 | 69 | 51 | 61 | 72 |
10 | 71 | 76 | 72 | 54 | 64 | 75 |
11 | 74 | 79 | 75 | 56 | 67 | 79 |
12 | 77 | 82 | 78 | 59 | 69 | 82 |
13 | 80 | 85 | 81 | 62 | 72 | 85 |
14 | 83 | 89 | 84 | 65 | 75 | 89 |
15 | 86 | 92 | 87 | 67 | 78 | 92 |
16 | 89 | 95 | 90 | 70 | 80 | 96 |
BRUMS normative scores for female athletes (n = 877).
Raw Score | T-Score | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tension | Depression | Anger | Vigour | Fatigue | Confusion | |
0 | 39 | 42 | 42 | 28 | 35 | 40 |
1 | 42 | 45 | 45 | 31 | 38 | 43 |
2 | 45 | 48 | 48 | 34 | 40 | 46 |
3 | 48 | 51 | 51 | 36 | 43 | 50 |
4 | 50 | 54 | 54 | 39 | 45 | 53 |
5 | 53 | 57 | 57 | 42 | 47 | 56 |
6 | 56 | 60 | 60 | 45 | 50 | 59 |
7 | 59 | 63 | 63 | 47 | 52 | 63 |
8 | 62 | 66 | 66 | 50 | 55 | 66 |
9 | 65 | 69 | 69 | 53 | 57 | 69 |
10 | 67 | 72 | 72 | 55 | 60 | 72 |
11 | 70 | 75 | 75 | 58 | 62 | 76 |
12 | 73 | 78 | 78 | 61 | 65 | 79 |
13 | 76 | 81 | 81 | 64 | 67 | 82 |
14 | 79 | 84 | 84 | 66 | 70 | 85 |
15 | 82 | 87 | 87 | 69 | 72 | 92 |
16 | 84 | 90 | 90 | 72 | 75 | 95 |
BRUMS normative scores for male non-athletes (n = 6207).
Raw Score | T-Score | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tension | Depression | Anger | Vigour | Fatigue | Confusion | |
0 | 42 | 44 | 43 | 30 | 37 | 42 |
1 | 45 | 47 | 46 | 33 | 40 | 45 |
2 | 48 | 50 | 50 | 35 | 42 | 49 |
3 | 51 | 53 | 53 | 38 | 45 | 52 |
4 | 54 | 56 | 56 | 41 | 47 | 55 |
5 | 57 | 59 | 59 | 43 | 50 | 59 |
6 | 60 | 63 | 62 | 46 | 53 | 62 |
7 | 64 | 66 | 66 | 48 | 55 | 65 |
8 | 67 | 69 | 69 | 51 | 58 | 68 |
9 | 70 | 72 | 72 | 54 | 60 | 72 |
10 | 73 | 75 | 75 | 56 | 63 | 75 |
11 | 76 | 79 | 79 | 59 | 65 | 78 |
12 | 79 | 82 | 82 | 62 | 68 | 82 |
13 | 82 | 85 | 85 | 64 | 70 | 85 |
14 | 86 | 88 | 88 | 67 | 73 | 88 |
15 | 89 | 91 | 91 | 69 | 75 | 91 |
16 | 92 | 95 | 95 | 72 | 78 | 95 |
BRUMS normative scores for female non-athletes (n = 6365).
Raw Score | T-Score | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tension | Depression | Anger | Vigour | Fatigue | Confusion | |
0 | 41 | 43 | 43 | 32 | 36 | 42 |
1 | 44 | 47 | 46 | 35 | 39 | 45 |
2 | 47 | 50 | 50 | 37 | 41 | 48 |
3 | 50 | 53 | 53 | 40 | 44 | 51 |
4 | 53 | 56 | 57 | 43 | 46 | 55 |
5 | 55 | 59 | 60 | 45 | 49 | 58 |
6 | 58 | 62 | 63 | 48 | 51 | 61 |
7 | 61 | 65 | 67 | 51 | 54 | 64 |
8 | 64 | 68 | 70 | 53 | 56 | 67 |
9 | 67 | 72 | 73 | 56 | 59 | 70 |
10 | 70 | 75 | 77 | 59 | 61 | 73 |
11 | 73 | 78 | 80 | 61 | 64 | 76 |
12 | 76 | 81 | 83 | 64 | 66 | 80 |
13 | 79 | 84 | 87 | 67 | 69 | 83 |
14 | 82 | 87 | 90 | 69 | 71 | 86 |
15 | 85 | 90 | 93 | 72 | 74 | 89 |
16 | 88 | 93 | 97 | 75 | 76 | 92 |
Recommendations for the use of mood profiling with athletes.
Recommendation | Explanation |
---|---|
Profile moods regularly. | A single mood profile has limited value compared to mood trends over time. Weekly profiles act as a useful monitor of general well-being but more frequent, even daily, profiling may be beneficial in major competitions, when moods may be more volatile [18]. Mood stability is proposed to be advantageous for performance [74]. |
Ensure individual follow-up. | One-to-one discussion is recommended to better understand the context of a mood profile. Reasons underlying specific mood scores (e.g., elevated angerdue to a disagreement with a teammate; increased fatigue due to inadequate sleep) can be addressed to help guide future actions or interventions [18]. |
Consider referral of athletes with very negative profiles. | Inverse iceberg and, especially, inverse Everest profiles suggest risk of psychopathology. Such profiles repeated over time may indicate the need for evaluation of an individual by a clinical specialist [25]. |
Explore adaptive and maladaptive mood regulation strategies. | Effective mood regulation is highly individualized, with many potential regulation strategies available [74,75]. Some may be effective in the short term (e.g., using music to relax or arouse [76]) and others may have longer term mental health benefits (e.g., relaxation/mindfulness training, yoga [77,78]). Some habitual coping strategies may pose a threat to health (e.g., excessive alcohol consumption [79]) or to performance (e.g., negative self-talk [80]). Identifying the adaptive mood regulation strategies that work best for individuals is a key element in effective mood profiling. |
Refer to relevant norms. | Use the most appropriate and recently available tables of normative data to interpret raw scores. Interpreting raw scores against inappropriate norms (e.g., interpreting a male athlete’s scores against non-athlete norms) produces incorrect standardized T-scores and a potentially misleading overview of the athlete’s mood, which, in turn, may lead to ineffective or unnecessary intervention [81]. |
Do not extrapolate beyond the mood dimensions assessed. | The BRUMS assesses six specific dimensions of mood and does not provide a comprehensive assessment of the mood construct [23]. The measure provides a useful method of screening for potential mental ill-health indicators and/or threats to performance but is not a diagnostic tool and should not be used as such. |
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2021 by the authors.
Abstract
Mood responses are a well-established mental health indicator. Gauging mental health status over time often involves periodic mood assessment using a standardized measure, a process referred to as mood profiling. Comparison of observed mood scores against relevant normative data is central to effective mood profiling. The primary purpose of our study was to improve existing norms for the Brunel Mood Scale (BRUMS) using a large internet sample. The secondary purpose was to discuss how mood profiling can be used to promote sustainable mental health primarily among athletes but also with relevance to non-athletes. The BRUMS was completed via the In The Mood website by 15,692 participants. Significant differences between observed mean scores and existing normative data were evident for all six mood dimensions, prompting norm refinement. Specific group norms were generated to address sex differences in mood responses and differences by athlete/nonathlete status. The revised tables of normative data for the BRUMS should be used by researchers in future investigations of mood responses and by applied practitioners seeking to monitor mood responses as an indicator of mental health status. Applications of mood profiling with elite athletes are exemplified, along with recommendations for using mood profiling in the pursuit of sustainable mental health.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details

1 Centre for Health Research, University of Southern Queensland, Toowoomba 4350, Australia
2 School of Psychology and Counselling, University of Southern Queensland, Toowoomba 4350, Australia;