About the Authors:
Mark R. Libowitz
Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Magnetic Resonance Program, Huntington Medical Research Institutes, Pasadena, California, United States of America
ORCID logo https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6143-772X
Ke Wei
Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Validation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Magnetic Resonance Program, Huntington Medical Research Institutes, Pasadena, California, United States of America
Thao Tran
Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision
Affiliation: Magnetic Resonance Program, Huntington Medical Research Institutes, Pasadena, California, United States of America
Karen Chu
Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software
Affiliation: Magnetic Resonance Program, Huntington Medical Research Institutes, Pasadena, California, United States of America
Kristina Moncrieffe
Roles Formal analysis, Investigation
Affiliation: Fuller Graduate School of Psychology, Pasadena, California, United States of America
Michael G. Harrington
Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Molecular Neurology Program, Huntington Medical Research Institutes, Pasadena, California, United States of America
Kevin King
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Barrow Neurological Institute, Phoenix, Arizona, United States of America
Introduction
Diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is increasingly focused on discovering biomarkers for early detection [1]. Early detection of AD is crucial as future treatments will likely focus on preventing AD or slowing its progression rather than reversing AD’s neuronal damage [2]. Individuals receiving early diagnosis of pre-clinical AD changes may benefit from initiating health measures to preserve existing cognitive function [1]. Low cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) ß-amyloid42 (Aβ42) and elevated CSF Tau proteins are two biomarkers that have been established in the 2011 diagnostic guidelines of AD [3]. Previous research identified these two biomarkers as a diagnostic marker of AD in CSF based on Aβ42/Tau ratio [4–6] with a lower ratio present in AD pathology as compared to normal pathology. Obtaining CSF for analysis is invasive, however, and positron emission tomography for amyloid or tau involve radiation exposure and are cost prohibitive for screening. MRI examinations of the brain are increasingly common for the workup of memory loss but are not routinely used to screen for risk of dementia. Significant additional value may be provided from these exams if they were used to help suggest presence of AD-related pathology. We hypothesized that reduced CSF Aß42 and increased CSF Tau protein levels would show significant association with lower regional brain volumetric assessment.
In addition to CSF biomarkers, determining the relationship between regional brain volume and neuropsychological testing can help define the brain biological changes that underlie AD symptomatology. As a secondary measure, outcomes from two neuropsychological examinations were used in the present study, the Boston Naming Test [7] and the Stroop Interference Test [8]. Boston Naming is a visual confrontation naming test [9, 10] that is used as a test for semantic memory [11]. The Stroop Interference test is commonly used as a measure of executive function [12].
Five brain regions were chosen to test correlation with CSF and cognitive correlates of brain atrophy in AD [9, 13]. These regions include the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, precuneus, posterior cingulate and, as a negative control, the occipital lobe. The prefrontal cortex is closely linked with cognitive executive functions [14–18] and we specifically predicted it would correlate most closely with performance on the Stroop Interference Test. The hippocampus is involved in registration and retrieval of semantically and lexically associated words [19–22] and was expected to have the closest correlation with the Boston Naming Test. The precuneus was included due to its early degeneration with aging and in early stage AD patients [23] and its association with poor performance on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment in older adults [24]. The posterior cingulate also demonstrates early involvement in preclinical stages of AD [25, 26] with continued neurodegeneration as AD progresses [27]. Finally, the occipital lobe was chosen as a control as it is not heavily involved in early AD pathology. We hypothesized that decreased regional brain volumes and a lower CSF Aß42/Tau ratio would suggest presence of deficits on these two sensitive neuropsychological tests of cognitive dysfunction.
Our study therefore aimed to identify the associations between CSF Aß42 and Tau protein levels with regional brain volumes and then to evaluate the optimal use of each of these markers in predicting impairments on neuropsychological examination among participants in a brain aging study ranging from cognitively healthy (CH) to AD.
Materials and methods
Cohort and overall study design
We evaluated 113 participants (41 males and 72 females, mean age of 76.5 ± 8.8 years) who gave written informed consent in this IRB approved study at Huntington Medical Research Institutes and underwent brain MRI from 2011–2018. Individual participant data is available in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative public database (http://adni.loni.usc.edu/). Most (n = 98) participants underwent both neuropsychological examination and lumbar punctures to obtain CSF for Aβ42 and Tau protein assessment. Nine participants underwent lumbar puncture but could not complete cognitive testing (primarily due to presence of dementia) and 2 underwent cognitive testing but did not provide education level, as shown in Fig 1, resulting in 107 with lumbar puncture and 96 with neuropsychological testing. Participant demographics are also listed in Fig 1. Classification of cognitive status was determined in clinical conference with a minimum of three faculty clinicians who reviewed each participant’s results on a battery of neurocognitive exams, MRI and Lumbar Puncture as previously described [4].
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 1. Cohort study design of non-exclusive subsets undergoing lumbar puncture and neuropsychological testing.
Flow chart representation of participants included in this study resulting in two non-exclusive subsets. M = Male, F = Female, CH = Cognitively Healthy, MCI = Mild Cognitive Impairment, AD = Alzheimer’s disease, 1.5T = GE Signa HDx 1.5T scanner, 3T = GE Signa HDx 3T scanner.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254332.g001
MR image acquisition and analysis
All participants underwent successful MRI examination in either a GE signa HDx 1.5 Tesla or 3 Tesla scanners. 3D fast spoiled gradient echo (FSPGR) were obtained with echo time 2.41ms, repetition time 6.75ms, inversion time 600ms, slice thickness 1.2mm; matrix 256 x 256, FOV 24cm, flip angle 8 degrees. Images were analyzed with FreeSurfer V6 [available for download at http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/] to obtain brain volumes. Prefrontal cortex regions combined the volumes of the frontal pole, superior frontal, caudal middle frontal, rostral middle frontal, lateral orbito-frontal, medial orbito-frontal, pars opercularis, pars orbitalis and pars triangularis regions. Occipital regions combined the volumes of the cuneus, fusiform, lateral occipital, lingual, and pericalcarine regions.
Cerebrospinal fluid analysis, cognitive status, and neuropsychological examination
CSF Aß42 and Tau protein levels were determined via lumbar puncture in 107 participants [4]. Cognitive status was classified at consensus clinical conferencing, including medical history and examination, and an extensive AD neurocognitive battery that tested 6 cognitive domains [4]. Participants were administered the Stroop Interference and Boston Naming Test by supervised and trained graduate neuropsychology students [4].
Statistics
Analysis was performed using JMP Pro, version 15.0.0 (SAS institute, Inc., Cary, NC). Statistical significance was set at P = .05 and two-tailed for all tests. For primary analysis, the p values were adjusted for multiple comparison testing using the Benjamini and Hochberg false discovery rate [28]. All variables were evaluated for normality, and CSF Aβ42 and Tau levels were log transformed for statistical analysis to obtain a more normal distribution. Age, sex, intracranial volume and scanner type (GE Signa HDx 1.5T and 3T scanners) were considered as adjustment factors for regional volumetric analysis using general linear models. Residual values for regional brain volumes not explained by age, sex, scanner type and intracranial volume were then derived. The derived equations are listed at the beginning of the results section.
First, we assessed the logarithmic CSF Aβ42 and Tau protein levels as independent variables to predict residual values for the dependent variables of the hippocampus, occipital, prefrontal, posterior cingulate, and precuneus volumes. Each analysis underwent false discovery rate adjustment of p-values. We expected both decreased Aβ42 and increased Tau CSF protein levels to show association with lower regional volumes for each region besides the occipital lobe.
In secondary analysis, we utilized a stepwise best fit model optimizing stepwise Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) [29] that included as potential predictors each regional brain volume, log Aβ42, log Tau, Aβ42/Tau ratio, education level, sex and age with scores on the Stroop Interference and Boston Naming test as the dependent outcome variables. We predicted Boston Naming scores would be higher as a function of greater hippocampus volume and that better Stroop Interference scores would correlate with larger prefrontal volume. Lastly, we utilized a stepwise BIC [29] to determine the best predictor of normal, MCI, and AD consensus cognitive status coded as ordinal categorical variables.
Results
Regional brain volumes were first adjusted to remove the influence of sex (male = 0, female = 1), scanner type (3T = 0, 1.5T = 1) and intracranial volume using the following equations:
Lower CSF Aβ42 levels were significantly associated with hippocampus (p = 0.017), prefrontal cortex (p = 0.002), precuneus (p = 0.007), posterior cingulate (p = 0.002), and occipital lobe (p = 0.035) volumes. These significant associations withstood false discovery rate adjustment for the hippocampus (p = 0.043), prefrontal cortex (p = 0.010), precuneus (p = 0.024), and posterior cingulate (p = 0.010), but not for the occipital lobe (p = 0.070). Increased CSF Tau protein levels were not significantly associated with any regional brain volume (Table 1).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Log CSF Aβ42 and Tau predicting regional brain volumes adjusted for age, sex, scanner type and intracranial volume.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254332.t001
For those administered the Boston Naming and Stroop Interference test (N = 96) the best fit model identified higher education (p < 0.001), female sex (p = 0.002), and higher hippocampus volume (p < 0.001) as significant predictors of Stroop Interference scores (p < 0.001; adjusted r2 = 0.264) and identified higher education (p = 0.0198), higher hippocampus volume (p = 0.002), higher posterior cingulate volume (p = 0.019), lower Aβ42/Tau ratio (p < 0.001), and age (p = 0.0271) as significant predictors of the Boston Naming Test (p < 0.001; adjusted r2 = 0.218). Lastly, in a best predictive model minimizing BIC, Stroop Interference Test score (p < 0.001), Boston Naming Test score (p < 0.001), prefrontal cortex volume (p = 0.002), and hippocampus volume (p = 0.007) were the best predictors for presence of MCI or AD (p < 0.001; r2 = 0.37). Notably, the Stroop Interference Test was the most significant factor in this model with a p value of 6.5 x 10−10.
Discussion
In order to evaluate a group of participants in a brain aging study at our institution we performed lumbar punctures, MR imaging, and neuropsychological testing. To begin we set out to determine if CSF Aß42 or Tau levels were associated with hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, precuneus, posterior cingulate, or occipital lobe volumes. We had predicted that both a decrease in CSF Aß4 and an increase in CSF Tau would be associated with all regional brain volumes with exception of the occipital lobe. We found that a decrease in CSF Aß42 and not an increase in CSF Tau levels to be significantly associated (surviving a false discovery rate adjustment) with the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, precuneus, and posterior cingulate. As previously discussed, the ratio of CSF Aß42 to Tau protein levels has been utilized as biomarker of AD (citations). The results from our cohort provide evidence that decreased CSF Aß42 is more predictive of the regional brain volumes of interest than increased CSF Tau levels. Further, our cohort consisted of participants with a range of cognitive statuses from CH to AD. Therefore, our results suggest that decreased CSF Aß42 levels serve as a stronger predictor than increased CSF Tau levels earlier in the process of brain ageing. Finally, CSF Tau levels have been shown to have greater increases as dementia progresses [3], it is likely that this association would survive false discovery rate adjustment as our cohort ages.
Through exploratory analysis we also found that in our cohort several factors were significant predictors of the Stroop Interference and Boston Naming test (Table 2). Smaller brain volumes identified those likely to have deficits on cognitive testing. In order of significance: higher ratio of Aß42 to Tau, smaller hippocampus volume, smaller posterior cingulate volume, and education level were all significant predictors of the Boston Naming Test, which assesses semantic memory [11]. Worse performance on the Stroop Interference test was best predicted by education level, low hippocampus volume, and the male sex. Unexpectedly prefrontal cortex volume was not predictive of Stroop Interference score, although it is typically associated with executive functions [12] thought to reside primarily in the frontal lobe.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. Stepwise best fit model reveals best predictors of Stroop interference and Boston naming test raw scores taking into account regional brain volume, CSF protein levels, education level, age, and sex.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254332.t002
Our results show a significant role for the hippocampus in cognitive tests involving both the Stroop Interference and Boston Naming tests. This is concordant with an fMRI study that showed a broad neural network including working memory is required during the Stroop Interference Test [30]. Working memory has also been shown to involve a broad neural network including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex as well as the hippocampus [30–35]. Future studies should examine whether this network is affected by AD pathology to further establish a link between hippocampus volume and executive function during Stroop Interference testing as demonstrated in this study.
Finally, we found that the lower Stroop Interreference Test score optimally predicted MCI and AD in our cohort which was further aided by lower Boston Naming Test score, prefrontal volume, and hippocampus volume using a selective regression model. Executive function and working memory are known to decline in the progression of MCI and AD (26550575). Considering both the Stroop Interference Test [12] as well as the prefrontal cortex are associated with cognitive executive function [14–18], working memory has shown involvement of a neural network that includes the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus, and the hippocampus was strongly associated with the Boston Naming Test in our cohort it stands to reason that these four factors served as the most optimal predictors of cognitive status. Still considering the evidence of CSF levels of Aß42 and Tau protein level’s association with AD we were surprised to find they did not help further improve prediction of cognitive status. Again, our cohort consists of participants with a range of cognitive statuses, thus it may be that these biomarkers would further improve predictions of cognitive status as this cohort ages.
There were several limitations to our study. This is an observational cross-sectional study that does not imply causation. Abnormal amyloid and tau are not specific for AD neuropathology and may occur with other neurodegenerative conditions. We used information from two MRI scanners of different strengths, 1.5 and 3 Tesla which results in a systematic bias in the brain volumes obtained. Differences between scanners is a common barrier to implementation of quantitative standards for brain volumetry. By accounting for the differences between 1.5 and 3 Tesla scans we found there to be a systematic shift that we could isolate and adjust and still identify significant differences with other clinical markers. Crucially, this allowed us to include data from both scanners in each subset. This work helps to show that in spite of technical differences, it is possible to identify meaningful associations in evaluating cohorts assessed on different scanners. This is important as in a clinical setting it is common that a different scanner will be used for a patient or participant and our findings suggest that significant differences in brain volumes may still be identified after appropriate adjustment.
Conclusion
In this brain aging study, we were able to identify lower MRI regional volumes related to abnormal CSF levels of Aß42 but not to Tau; this disparity may be due to amyloid becoming abnormal earlier in the disease course and the lack of individuals with more advanced levels of dementia in this study. MRI regional volumes also showed added utility in predicting cognitive performance: hippocampus and posterior cingulate volume helped improve prediction of Boston naming test score alongside Aß42/Tau Ratio; hippocampal volume predicted score on Stroop without any added value for CSF Aß42and Tau levels. Interestingly, performance on Stroop interference testing and prefrontal volumes were the best predictors of MCI and AD status in our cohort, with no added predictive value from CSF Aß42and Tau levels. This work suggests the importance of future work considering the timing and interdependence of changes in brain volumes and CSF levels of Aß42 and Tau in neurodegeneration and impairment of neural networks in understanding deficits in neuropsychological testing.
Citation: Libowitz MR, Wei K, Tran T, Chu K, Moncrieffe K, Harrington MG, et al. (2021) Regional brain volumes relate to Alzheimer’s disease cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers and neuropsychometry: A cross-sectional, observational study. PLoS ONE 16(7): e0254332. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254332
1. Association As. 2018 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2018;14(3):367–429.
2. Risacher SL, Saykin AJ. Neuroimaging and other biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease: the changing landscape of early detection. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9:621–48. Epub 2013/01/10. pmid:23297785; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3955298.
3. Jack CR Jr., Albert MS, Knopman DS, McKhann GM, Sperling RA, Carrillo MC, et al. Introduction to the recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):257–62. Epub 2011/04/26. pmid:21514247; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3096735.
4. Harrington MG, Chiang J, Pogoda JM, Gomez M, Thomas K, Marion SD, et al. Executive function changes before memory in preclinical Alzheimer’s pathology: a prospective, cross-sectional, case control study. PloS one. 2013;8(11):e79378. Epub 2013/11/22. pmid:24260210; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3832547.
5. Sjogren M, Vanderstichele H, Agren H, Zachrisson O, Edsbagge M, Wikkelso C, et al. Tau and Abeta42 in cerebrospinal fluid from healthy adults 21–93 years of age: establishment of reference values. Clin Chem. 2001;47(10):1776–81. Epub 2001/09/25. pmid:11568086.
6. Wang L, Fagan AM, Shah AR, Beg MF, Csernansky JG, Morris JC, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid proteins predict longitudinal hippocampal degeneration in early-stage dementia of the Alzheimer type. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2012;26(4):314–21. Epub 2011/12/14. pmid:22156755; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3309103.
7. Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Craft S, Fagan AM, et al. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):280–92. Epub 2011/04/26. pmid:21514248; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3220946.
8. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of experimental psychology. 1935;18(6):643.
9. Blennow K, de Leon MJ, Zetterberg H. Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet. 2006;368(9533):387–403. Epub 2006/08/01. pmid:16876668
10. Williams BW, Mack W, Henderson VW. Boston Naming Test in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 1989;27(8):1073–9. Epub 1989/01/01. pmid:2797414.
11. Sawrie SM, Martin RC, Gilliam FG, Faught RE, Maton B, Hugg JW, et al. Visual confrontation naming and hippocampal function: A neural network study using quantitative (1)H magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Brain. 2000;123 (Pt 4):770–80. Epub 2000/03/29. pmid:10734008.
12. Golden CJ, Freshwater SM. Stroop color and word test. 1978.
13. Kern S, Zetterberg H, Kern J, Zettergren A, Waern M, Hoglund K, et al. Prevalence of preclinical Alzheimer disease: Comparison of current classification systems. Neurology. 2018;90(19):e1682–e91. Epub 2018/04/15. pmid:29653987; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5952969.
14. Stuss DT, Levine B. Adult clinical neuropsychology: lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:401–33. Epub 2001/12/26. pmid:11752491.
15. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord. 2008;23(3):329–42; quiz 472. Epub 2007/12/07. pmid:18058946; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2535903.
16. Lezak M, Howieson D, Loring D. Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford Univer. Press Google Scholar. 1995.
17. Raz N. Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. 2000.
18. Reuter-Lorenz PA. Cognitive neuropsychology of the aging brain. 2000.
19. Bartha L, Brenneis C, Schocke M, Trinka E, Koylu B, Trieb T, et al. Medial temporal lobe activation during semantic language processing: fMRI findings in healthy left- and right-handers. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003;17(2):339–46. Epub 2003/07/26. pmid:12880904.
20. Bartha L, Marien P, Brenneis C, Trieb T, Kremser C, Ortler M, et al. Hippocampal formation involvement in a language-activation task in patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2005;46(11):1754–63. Epub 2005/11/24. pmid:16302855.
21. Bonelli SB, Powell R, Thompson PJ, Yogarajah M, Focke NK, Stretton J, et al. Hippocampal activation correlates with visual confrontation naming: fMRI findings in controls and patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res. 2011;95(3):246–54. Epub 2011/05/20. pmid:21592730; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3229979.
22. Pihlajamaki M, Tanila H, Hanninen T, Kononen M, Laakso M, Partanen K, et al. Verbal fluency activates the left medial temporal lobe: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Ann Neurol. 2000;47(4):470–6. Epub 2000/04/13. pmid:10762158.
23. Karas G, Scheltens P, Rombouts S, van Schijndel R, Klein M, Jones B, et al. Precuneus atrophy in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease: a morphometric structural MRI study. Neuroradiology. 2007;49(12):967–76. Epub 2007/10/24. pmid:17955233
24. Srinivasa RN, Rossetti HC, Gupta MK, Rosenberg RN, Weiner MF, Peshock RM, et al. Cardiovascular Risk Factors Associated with Smaller Brain Volumes in Regions Identified as Early Predictors of Cognitive Decline. Radiology. 2016;278(1):198–204. pmid:26218598; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4699491.
25. Minoshima S, Foster NL, Kuhl DE. Posterior cingulate cortex in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet. 1994;344(8926):895. Epub 1994/09/24. pmid:7916431.
26. Minoshima S, Giordani B, Berent S, Frey KA, Foster NL, Kuhl DE. Metabolic reduction in the posterior cingulate cortex in very early Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. 1997;42(1):85–94. Epub 1997/07/01. pmid:9225689.
27. Brun A, Gustafson L. Distribution of cerebral degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease. A clinico-pathological study. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr (1970). 1976;223(1):15–33. Epub 1976/12/31. pmid:828039.
28. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological). 1995;57(1):289–300.
29. Wang L, Li R. Weighted Wilcoxon-type smoothly clipped absolute deviation method. Biometrics. 2009;65(2):564–71. Epub 2008/07/24. [pii] pmid:18647294; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2700846.
30. Peterson BS, Skudlarski P, Gatenby JC, Zhang H, Anderson AW, Gore JC. An fMRI study of Stroop word-color interference: evidence for cingulate subregions subserving multiple distributed attentional systems. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45(10):1237–58. Epub 1999/06/01. pmid:10349031.
31. Bates JF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Prefrontal connections of medial motor areas in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1993;336(2):211–28. Epub 1993/10/08. pmid:7503997.
32. Gevins A, Smith ME, McEvoy L, Yu D. High-resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: effects of task difficulty, type of processing, and practice. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7(4):374–85. Epub 1997/06/01. pmid:9177767.
33. Goldman-Rakic PS. Anatomical and functional circuits in prefrontal cortex of nonhuman primates. Relevance to epilepsy. Adv Neurol. 1995;66:51–63; discussion -5. Epub 1995/01/01. pmid:7771311.
34. Rakic PG. Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behaviour by representational memory. Handbook of physiology: The nervous system. 5: American Physiological Society Bethesda; 1987. p. 373–417.
35. Selemon LD, Goldman-Rakic PS. Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior. J Neurosci. 1988;8(11):4049–68. Epub 1988/11/01. pmid:2846794; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6569486.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2021 Libowitz et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
We hypothesized that automated assessment of brain volumes on MRI can predict presence of cerebrospinal fluid abnormal ß-amyloid42 and Tau protein levels and thus serve as a useful screening test for possible Alzheimer’s disease. 113 participants ranging from cognitively healthy to Alzheimer’s disease underwent MRI exams to obtain measurements of hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, precuneus, parietal cortex, and occipital lobe volumes. A non-exclusive subset (n = 107) consented to lumbar punctures to obtain cerebrospinal fluid for ß-amyloid42 and Tau protein assessment including cognitively health (n = 75), mild cognitively impaired (n = 22), and Alzheimer’s disease (n = 10). After adjustment for false discovery rate, ß-amyloid42 was significantly associated with volumes in the hippocampus (p = 0.043), prefrontal cortex (p = 0.010), precuneus (p = 0.024), and the posterior cingulate (p = 0.002). No association between Tau levels and regional brain volume survived multiple test correction. Secondary analysis was performed to determine associations between MRI brain volumes and CSF protein levels to neuropsychological impairment. A non-exclusive subset (n = 96) including cognitively healthy (n = 72), mild cognitively impaired (n = 21), and Alzheimer’s disease (n = 3) participants underwent Stroop Interference and Boston Naming neuropsychological testing. A higher score on the Boston Naming Test was optimally predicted in a selective regression model by greater hippocampus volume (p = 0.002), a higher ratio of ß-amyloid42 to Tau protein levels (p < 0.001), greater posterior cingulate volume (p = 0.0193), age (p = 0.0271), and a higher education level (p = 0.002). A better performance on the Stroop Interference Test was optimally predicted by greater hippocampus volume (p = 0.0003) and a higher education level (p < 0.001). Lastly, impaired cognitive status (mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease) was optimally predicted in a selective regression model by a worse performance on the Stroop Interference Test (p < 0.001), a worse performance on the Boston Naming Test (p < 0.001), along with lower prefrontal cortex volume (p = 0.002) and lower hippocampus volume (p = 0.007).
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer