Introduction
Teaching is an intense interpersonal profession that requires a complex set of cognitive and socio-emotional skills, and language is one of the main means to deliver it to students. Five decades of well-documented research highlights the role of teacher’s language on learning and student’s psychosocial development (Kalinowski et al., 2020), from which various characteristic features have been recognized. One of them embraces its academic properties, characterized by a more complex syntax, precise vocabulary, greater words number, more sophisticated sentences, and a larger lexical variety (Schleppegrell, 2004). Recent studies have moved toward describing disciplinary language in the early childhood classroom (Dickinson et al., 2014), mostly showing that early childhood educators’ (hereinafter ECEs) verbal competencies are an important factor to be associated to a variety of children’s learning outcomes. In other words, their ability to contingently give a verbal response has a deep influence on children behavior. For instance, such influence is portrayed in a meta-analytic study that summarize research on ECEs’ verbal competence and quality literacy-focused practices, broadly supporting children language development (Markussen-Brown et al., 2017). ECEs’ verbal responsiveness to children has a crucial role in their teaching efficacy (Justice et al., 2018), and studies that approach children language development advocate for the relevant role of educators’ complex syntax as a source of linguistic information for new word learners (Farrow et al., 2020). Similarly, posing questions during shared reading forecast children's expressive, receptive, and pragmatic language skills (Muhinyi & Rowe, 2019), and/or playful and verbally explicit interactions with children predict greater language ability and expressive language already from 12 months of age (Cuellar & Farkas, 2018). Specific cognitive abilities are as well stimulated by ECEs’ verbal competence and teaching ability, such as print concept, letter naming, and/or phonological abilities, indicating greater levels of ECEs’ knowledge are associated to more desirable classroom language and literacy practices (Ciesielski & Creaghead, 2020). All these examples are a good reflection of their role in children learning by the use of disciplinary language as an essential professional competence.
Despite the manifold research on teachers’ language, ECEs verbal competence had been approached by far more formal linguistic aspects, than through noncognitive ones. Thus, searching for evidence has to undertake an indirect approach to identify studies that, from their methodological scope, reach the affective dimension. For instance, it is argued that ECEs instructional ability is consistently associated to their professional qualifications, as well as with a large specter of influence on children behavior (Hu et al., 2017). Evidence in this regard suggests preschoolers adapt their exploratory strategies to the task structure verbally given by their ECEs (Ruggeri et al., 2019), and accordingly, questions asked about other’s emotions may involve later prosocial behavior (Brownell et al., 2012). Likewise, ECEs’ affective minimalizing linguistic behavior, a kind that purposefully distances ECEs from child’s current emotions, discourages the child to express them, identifying its detrimental influence on social and emotional competence development (King & La Paro, 2018).
The specific state of affairs in Latin-American countries is not far from what is currently known about ECEs linguistic competency, but with a lesser number of available studies which makes this topic an attractive research line to follow. Exemplary studies that can be highlighted show formal linguistic variables had been equally examined, as for instance, quantity, syntactic complexity and lexical diversity of ECEs’ speech to children (Pizarro et al., 2019). Also, children have greater opportunities for verbally interacting with ECEs in activities that include dancing, and imply fewer verbal commands to address children (Strasser et al., 2018), and on the contrary, in ECEs’ reads-aloud and the expansion of the number of words, complexity and lexical diversity that they use and children learn (Rosemberg, 2013, as cited in Pizzaro et al., 2019). The above research line offers interesting evidence to follow on the characteristics and modulatory effect of ECEs’ language on children’s learning processes and behavioral outcomes.
Teacher language-responsiveness framework
As well as teacher language cannot be simply reduced to a communicative function for just thinking and knowledge acquisition, a broader perspective has to be thought-out in sight of the noncognitive elements of verbal communication in teacher-students’ interactions. A solid base for these statements is found in a number of theoretical models that had reached a more integrated perspective on the understanding of cognitions and emotions as both part of one neurocognitive and affective system, to mention a couple of them: the cognitive-motivational theories as presented by Reisenzein (2019), or most specifically to this article, the Crick and Dodge’s model of social cognitive and emotion processes to children’s social competence (Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). Therefore, verbal competencies that address an affective matter can be equally understood as embedded in both domains, or following what is suggested by Hinojosa and co-researches (2019), language and emotions can be understood as: “(…) a complex set of semantic features in a distributed sensory, motor, language, and affective networks in the brain (…)” (p.1).
A theoretical model that describes teachers’ verbal competency and rest on above theoretical wider background is the one proposed by Prediger (2019b), and the language-responsiveness framework. It advises for conceptualizing content-specific’s study subjects and teachers verbal response by considering those verbal competencies, that help to cope with students’ situational learning demands in real or simulated classroom situations. This framework unveils a useful methodological approach for capturing students’ learning pathways (Prediger & Zindel, 2017), from where an interplay of teachers’ underlying cognitive abilities, pedagogical strategies, and personal preferences that aims to gradually enact students’ verbal abilities in a given learning topic. Although some authors have emphasized unravelling teachers’ language-sensitivity is a complex endeavor that requires the consideration of research results from both domain: learning and teaching (Hajer & Norén, 2017), this can be tried by a progressive modulation of teachers’ verbal response under the notion that the more speakers engage in a dialogue, the more that moves away from a noticeable event to more complex and explicit meanings. This is achieved according to five core abilities in order of increasing involvedness and cognitive complexity (Prediger, 2019a – Table 1).
Table 1: Teachers’ language-responsiveness framework. Based on Prediger (2019a).
Teachers’ abilities |
Definitions |
Noticing |
Recognition of students’ learning needs and language resources in their utterances and written products. |
Demanding |
Efforts in asking for explaining or initiating student talk in a cognitively and discursively rich learning situation. |
Supporting |
Efforts in asking from students to fulfil demands slightly above their current proficiency level. |
Developing |
Pedagogical interventions aim to develop students’ skill to a longer-term perspective. |
Identifying |
Demands to notice and support student’s attentional focus on crucial rather than peripherical demands. |
This theoretical framework serves as a tool to describe with a degree of precision the kind of approximation teachers show when verbally addressing children’s learning demands. Thus, it could be also of help in inquiring into ECEs’ linguistic strategies to access such demands when connoting affective related issues.
Study context
According to the Subsecretaria de Educación Parvularia [SEP] - Undersecretary of Preschool Education, the regions where this study was undertaken (Atacama and Antofagasta, northern regions of Chile), had, until august of 2020 a number of 1,588 ECEs working in establishments which are government dependent, yet another considerable number in private centres with official state recognition. In recent years various documents have tried to guarantee quality educational standards, within which a number of technical-pedagogical recommendations have been set down that imply professional competencies to be developed by ECEs – such as the use of disciplinary language (SEP, 2019). Along that, the newest Chilean curricular guidelines for early childhood education (SEP, 2018), a recent document that sets up the core elements in curriculum design, explicitly expresses the relevance of the affective domain, through the consideration of dimensions such as personal development, identity, and autonomy developing, all dimensions that require from these professionals an adequate and effective verbal approximation to children.
The well-recognized role of ECEs professional’s verbal competencies and the need of alignment with above-mentioned educational policies, prompted the researchers’ interest to expand a prior study on a quantitative characterization of ECEs socio-affective words production (Lería Dulčić et al., in press). The present research was conceived then to explore from the same data-base, the affective domain through the recognition of ECEs’ recursive qualitative contents and characteristic verbal referencing and phrasings, along the degree of expertise when addressing those contents. Although cited studies throughout this article well-accept the influence of teachers’ language on children’s learning, giving many hints about the influence of ECEs’ language on their teaching quality and children’s learning outcomes, it does not clarify yet which linguistic strategies are predominantly used, particularly when using affective connoted language. Therefore, this study aims to fill this gap and become a contribution to the understanding of the intersection between ECEs’ affective-speech and the verbal competencies that are implied in addressing children with affective signifying content. Moreover, there are no available studies that had specifically scrutinized this topic in Latin-American countries, a point from which this study starts.
Research questions
Two research questions are posed to address the above-mentioned inquiry: (1) Do cross-semantic spoken regularities in ECEs daily inside-classroom activities arise, that are susceptible to be differentially classified as groups of affective-speech by its contents, meanings, phrasing, and connotations? (2) Can the emerged groups be understood as reflecting differential degrees of affective-speech’ language-responsiveness?
Method
Design
A cross-sectional and associative mixed design was selected to examine qualitative information in mutual consideration of quantitative data from a priori selected categories. This was conducted by following a sequential explanatory approach in which quantitative results inform the qualitative method, then qualitative findings are mobilized back to interpret quantitative results according to an associative logic (Pluye & Hong, 2014). This strengthens the explanatory possibilities of both methodologies, offering an empirical structure in which to pose the qualitative data.
Participants
A number of 20 research subjects was selected by convenience depending on the availability to the researchers (all ECEs, female; X̅=28.6 years old; age range 21–40); from 14 different educational institutions of two nearby cities in northern Chile. All participants were Spanish native speakers, and completed four years bachelor’s degree in initial education, actually work in the locations where this study was conducted, and have more than one year of professional experience.
Instruments and information gathering
Continuous audio-recordings of daily ECEs educational activities within the classroom were collected, during five to seven work-days in the time period of three to four weeks, following the suggestions given by Cunningham and co-researches (2019), about the advantages of this kind of measurement. ECEs teaching groups go from 24 to 60 months from three different educational levels. The data were processed by:
Data under scrutiny
As presented above, quantitative data refers to the number of words from seven preselected categories extracted from authors’ prior data-base according to LIWC2015: Affect; Positive Emotions; Negative Emotions; Anxiety; Sadness; Anger; and Pleasure. Words belonging to these categories helped to look after sentences across the source transcriptions that suggest a clear affective connotation. Although all these categories are to some extent self-explanatory, they can be further checked out in Pennebaker and co-researches (2015).
Qualitative data came from above-detected sentences that, from a structural perspective, must fulfil some distinctive criteria to form a category of meaning: (a) to be constituted by one or more of the preselected words; and (b) to explicitly suggest any of the conditions stated by Fontaine and co-researches (2013), about an emotional response: an appraisal component that may trigger it; a distinguishing tendency toward action; facial movements and voice qualities; physiological reactions; and/or a subjective component.
Procedure and analysis
A few steps were undertaken to collect and analyse both kind of data, quantitative and qualitative:
Ethical concerns
A presentation letter and initial meetings were held to explain the study purpose, anonymity, audio storage, and afterward deletion. In some cases, the permission of recording was granted by an agreement for scientific research with the main author’s institution. Signed consents were collected.
Findings
This section firstly exposes the quantitative results from which the qualitative information was built up to give a context to the qualitative data, and then list the emerging nodes associated to the affective domain. Secondly, the identification of teachers’ predominant skills according to Prediger’s model is presented.
Lexical densities from preselected affective categories
The total number of collected words from our original study was 308.277 (X̅=15.413,85; SD=8.455,38); through 190 hours of effective audio-recording, and an average of 9,6 hours per participant. For this present study, a subtotal of 6049 words was extracted to work with, corresponding to the sum of words from seven preselected affective semantics categories from LIWC2015. Some descriptive statistics for this original data were: Affect (X̅=3.6(.63); t=25.55; p≤.00); Positive emotions (X̅=2.94(.54); t=24; p≤.00); Negative emotions (X̅=.65(.32); t=8.83; p≤.00); Anger (X̅=.24(.19); t=5.55; p≤.00); Anxiety (X̅=.14(.09); t=6.85; p≤.00); Sadness (X̅=14(.08); t=7.38; p≤.00); and, Pleasure (X̅=1.35(.43); t=13.96; p≤.00). The list of selected words from these categories is extensive, but can be briefly exemplified by words like watch-out, ready, well-done, attention, damage, and hug.
Affective-speech’s cross-semantic regularities
A large number of sentences came into sight that allude to a distinguishable affective connotation across participants’ speech, from where it was possible to re-classify them into a second and then a third higher order of associativity. This results in 8 categories or nodes that simultaneously reflect general and particular features in ECEs affective-speech, described below in table 2. The coding process was conducted from/to: Words → Sentences → Groups of sentences by contents’ similarity → Nodes or meta-categories (Table 2). The first two steps were done by LIWC2015, and the last two ones by the experts.
Table 2: Sequential codding of affective-speech nodes’ building
Affective semantic categories |
Words (1) |
First-phase coding (2) |
Second-phase coding (3) |
Nodes (4) |
Affect |
2315 |
7429 |
30 |
8 |
Positive emotion |
1374 |
6384 |
21 | |
Negative emotion |
813 |
1389 |
16 | |
Anxiety |
219 |
407 |
11 | |
Anger |
283 |
514 |
16 | |
Sadness |
180 |
586 |
16 | |
Pleasure |
865 |
3783 |
21 | |
Σ |
6049 |
20492 |
131 |
(1) Sum of words for each affective preselected category. (2) Number of times selected words were located within the text, and from which some sentences were extracted and then grouped. (3) Number of inbuilt groups of affective connoting sentences. (4) Final meta-grouping after criteria of relevance and unanimity.
A brief referential description is presented in table 3 to depict the nodes’ main characteristics.
Table 3: Nodes of ECEs’ affective-speech
Nodes |
Focus on |
Positive v/s negative features |
Positive verbal nuances in phrasings that encourage, support and/or reinforce children (e.g.: morning greetings; noticing achievements; and/or enhancing the expression of affect). Also, focus posed on calling, sometimes alarming children to be aware of preventing dangerous situations, bad behavior, fight situations, outbursts, and rude language. Affective-speech oriented to decrease children’s behaviors, for example: repeatedly ask to sit down; be silent; order bestrewed material; keep at the proper thematic corners; be patient; not to behave impulsively or in a hard-set way. Affective-speech oriented to notice children’s inner state and discomfort (e.g.: flu, cough, stomachache, fever, among others). |
Teaching v/s learning features |
Affective-speech enriches teaching/learning dynamics by emotionally giving “color” to an educative goal, as observable in story-telling that evokes experiential vivid images (e.g.: fables when a main character is portrayed; colors selected by an emotional intention (pink-red for love or black for sadness); and/or talking about significative social activities like Mother’s Day; Child’s day; Christmas; and so forth. ECEs’ communicational interactions are also strongly focused on social engagements and proper behavior to later be generalized to a rule held by everyone. |
Speech connoting the expression, distinguishing, sharing and/or inhibition of emotions |
Affective-speech encourages children through questions, instructions, and/or insinuations to express and share emotions, mainly in situations where a learning dynamic requires such verbal manifestations, as for example in: playing; morning welcoming; and/or singing. |
Speech connoting emotions before, during, and after behavior |
Affective-speech connotes or predicts children’s emotions, functionally linking an antecedent and/or a consequence of their behaviors for punctuating its relational and emotional implications (e.g.: hitting another child or being hit; crying for a non-apparent reason and asking about possible causations and outcomes of such behavior; or estimating an emotion as being exaggerated). |
Speech connoting calls for attention, warnings, reprehension, and praise |
Affective-speech focused on redirecting unwilling behavior into a more desirable educative goal (e.g.: warning; connoting rules-breaking; calling for order; reminding of previous rule; and/or just explaining a new task to do). ECEs’ speech reprehends or approves children’s behavior as a directional pedagogical tool to teach a more appropriate behavior, reinforce children’s behavior, delay rewards, and then redirect their attention to a contingent new goal (e.g.: admonitions associated to transgressing a rule or exposing an individual achievement). |
Speech connoting touching and the body |
Affective-speech focused on the body, bodily functions, and sensorial-perceptual experiences around at least three dimensions: promoting affective interactions through touching (e.g.: showing care to others, hugging, and/or solacing); facilitating behaviors associated to the body (e.g.: hygiene, proper sitting, and clothing); and teaching about and/or through body parts. |
Speech connoting increasing, decreasing and normalizing language |
Affective-speech intended to modulate children’s experiences after a situation happened, usually emotionally charged to them, as in: diminishing children’s anxiety in front of incidents like strikes, falls or injuries, suggesting - for example - an injury is not so bad or an animal is not so fierce; normalizing complaints, shame or urgencies (e.g.: explaining an emotion is normal under certain circumstances); or making bigger verbal utterances to praise a success or avoid a danger (e.g.: praising small successes). |
ECEs own emotions |
Lack of sentences that explicitly refer to ECEs personal affective issues with a few exemptions (e.g.: disliking a children disruptive behavior, expressing tiredness or a critical view of weekly administration). |
ECEs affective-speech’s degree of responsiveness
We were interested in exploring the pedagogical expertise that is implied in such language use and phrasings by checking ECEs progressive ability to verbally “tune” with children learning demands in their educative interactions with a recognizable affective implication. This was conducted through a blind inter-raters’ procedure, three experts, and the five abilities from teacher language responsiveness’ framework (Prediger, 2019a). Each node has a constitutive number of groups (table 1) and a large number of in-built sentences, that were pinpointed by the raters according to their affective connotation, and without limits. The node “ECEs own emotions” was not included due to a possible theoretical contradiction with the model, and its orientation to address children learning demands, but not to describe adults’ auto-referencing linguistic behavior.
Spearman coefficient for rater 1 and rater 2 shows the strongest correlation (r =.94 at p≤.00); while left associations were less significative. Krippendorff ‘s alpha was applied to check overall agreement, having in mind it demands a score of α=.6 to make tentative conclusions (Hayes & Krippendorff, 2007). Nonetheless, medium to very low scores were observed, suggesting the necessity of checking once again raters’ punctuations through another well-known agreement’s coefficient (Fleiss-kappa), but this time separated by skills. Table 4 exposes the results for both coefficients.
Table 4: Reliability coefficients’ scores
Krippendorff ‘s α * |
Fleiss-kappa (k) | ||||||||
Overall k ** |
k by dimensions |
Conditional probability |
Lower 95% CI |
Upper 95% CI | |||||
Noticing |
.56 |
.54 |
.8 |
.9 |
.00 |
.38 |
1.23 | ||
Demanding |
.53 |
.66 |
.01 |
.1 |
.96 | ||||
Supporting |
.22 |
.33 |
.3 |
-.2 |
.65 | ||||
Developing |
-.05 |
.00 |
.81 |
-.47 |
.37 | ||||
Identifying |
-.00 |
.00 |
1 |
-.00 |
-.00 |
*p. ≤ .05. **p. ≤ .00.
Above-results highlight Noticing as the single ability above the minimum requirement for significance in assessing the reliability of agreement between raters.
Discussion
We detected a large number of cross-semantic regularities in ECEs speech that allowed us to positively answer the first research question. This leads to the final recognition of 8 general nodes of affective-related speech, through associations of convergence and divergence within a certain degree of feasibility that the a priori affective semantic categories made possible. These nodes can be also understood as a network of communicational paths that could serve as “anchor points” for the further exploration of ECEs’ affective-speech. Although in some cases sentences were scattered all across the transcriptions, making difficult to pinpoint them out into a hierarchy of predominance, a few characteristics can be marked out by their relevance for ECEs pedagogical work.
The highest number of sentences was clearly related to positive nuances, which mirrors a well-known ECEs communicational style that may be understood as a desirable professional ability, that purposely enriches children’s learning environment mostly drenched into positive experiences, stimulating their cognitive exploration, group cohesion, feelings of safety, and – generally speaking – well-being. This can be contextualized under the suggestions given by Decker-Woodrow (2018), which claims that greater emotional support, classroom management abilities, and quality interactions are characteristics of efficacious facilitators in prekindergarten environments. Another relevant observation highlights the characteristic ECES temporal use of emotion-related words before or after a meaningful educative situation occurs, as for example in a quick verbal intervention after a child’s fall, which suggests some ideas about the effect of this kind of linguistic punctuation on children socio-affective learning. More specifically, phoneme awareness is the strongest predictor of reading development (Melby-Lervåg, 2012), confirmed also by newer studies and meta-analytic reviews (e.g.: D’Alessio et al., 2019; Duncan, 2018). Thus, ECEs use of affective close-related morphemes or simple affective-connoting words may provide to children access to a wider semantic and syntactic information by means of expanding children vocabulary and conscious accessibility to their use. This also follows the observations presented by Ren and coresearchers (2016), on how learning of grammatical morphemes might be associated with emotion regulation among bilingual preschoolers, and the role of child language to express their needs with words rather than with emotions. In this vein, studies shown that toddlers’ and preschoolers’ language skills are positively associated with self-regulation of emotions such as anger and frustration (Roben et al., 2013). Furthermore, the relatively high use of words regarding the body aligns with the well-accepted importance of embodied-words in ECEs pedagogical work, as argued by Cekaite and Bergnehr (2018). Also, the node addressing the specific use of language when increasing, decreasing and normalizing a child’s verbal utterancemay give some insights on ECEs coping skills when dealing with children situational and pressing demands, which may likewise inspire new research following the line proposed by King and La Paro’s work (2018). Additionally, we were particularly interested in higher order emotion-referencing such as love, compassion, empathy, and teaching values, thus, such sentences were traced back to the source-transcriptions to get any hint for further exploration. Two of the ECEs who participated in this study belong to a Cristian school, and most referred words belong to them by expressing spiritual topics that connote verbal references in third person plural over first and second person singular. This could give an exemplary hint on ECEs’ own emotions and personal experiences (node 8), when sharing a common socio-cultural framework with children and the significance of working with them. Besides that, compassion and forgiveness are emotions of a higher order that imply more complex cognitive processes, and little is known on ECEs compassionate behavior and their ability to teach it to children, as recently argued by Vuorinen and co-researches (2020).
Despite these confirmatory findings, in our judgment the second research question was only partially accepted due to the low observed agreement between experts. We consider these identified values may simultaneously respond to the multi-layered nature of gathered information and low case number, but also to the quality of expert judgments. However, we decided not to reject the experts that reached the lowest observed nominal correlations. Nonetheless, we recommend future studies include at least five expert-raters to ensure a good reliability score in case of such possible rejections. Moving ahead into this discussion, the relative predominance of Noticing and – up to a certain degree – Demanding, may suggest an alignment that responds to the progressive complexity that underlies what Prediger’s theoretical model proposes, and may be strongly related to other theoretical proposals that argue in favor of progressive and escalated models of competencies development, such as is - for example - proposed by Marzano and Kendall (2008), widely addressed in competence-based education. Our findings suggest that most ECEs affective-talk relies on noticing or spotting children’s affective issues and classroom contingencies alongside the educative daily flow. Although, other verbal abilities are weakly recognized by our experts, this may be likewise aligned again to a developmental progressive factor to be considered when working with preschool children (Copple & Bredekamp, 2009), but also to the spectrum of competencies that ECEs who participated in this study may have, and could develop. Due to it exploratory nature, the future exploration of this specific finding demands an expanded methodological approach that could otherwise recall specific characteristics of the instructional language, that may give ECEs’ the ability to develop each dimension of the five core’s abilities of the language-responsiveness framework taken into consideration in our study.
Some limitations can be highlighted to improve future studies within this research line. ECEs’ speech was short, fast spoken, and many times expressed in a “volatile” manner, depending on very changeable class contingencies, which could be addressed in future studies. In our concern, this is an attractive research line to follow due to the role of quality instructions in pedagogical work, as recently emphasized by Brown and coresearchers (2020). Also, future studies necessarily must incorporate children’s speech, giving these exploratory findings a more overall scope, and similarly adding the correlation with teachers’ affective talk in primary school. Moreover, words that were used for detecting the qualitative nodes were approached from a general linguistic background provided by the software, which gave us the opportunity to situate our findings in a wider and linguistic standardized framework, but also alerts us to further consider the specific disciplinary, socio-cultural and idiosyncratic linguistic context in which our ECEs work.
Conclusion
This study was designed to empirically identify ECEs common phrasings that refer upon different degrees, to an affective content or meaning from which was possible to address the extent of their professional competence when answering to children learning demands that require or imply and affective connotation. We were able to tentatively acknowledge many distinguishable nodes of ECEs’ affective-speech, and identify one predominant linguistic approximation or verbal strategy to children learning demands. Despite of the fact that by the methodological scope of this study it is not possible to characterize these findings as a generalizable use of ECEs affective-speech, they give some preliminary insights into an attractive direction worth following. Thus, this study’ contribute to moving forward the study of the gradation of ECEs’ verbal competencies or coping-verbal strategies with children and specially suggest some contents to stress when follow affective-related contents in their communications interaction with children. This may also help to guide curricular policies in higher education on pivotal dimensions for ECEs’ professional development, especially regarding the relevance of the affective factors in ECE’s professional speech. Further studies in this regard could expand their research topics to close-related lines such as, for example, ECEs’ technical vocabulary growth; appropriate lexical choices; suitable meanings usage; and/or developmentally proper sense-making to teach associated to the affective domain.
We hope these findings will invigorate this research line in order to foster ECEs’ language proficiency and strengthen their formative experience and professional efficacy.
Transference
The findings provided contribute to a program for socio-affective competences acquisition, aimed to students of early childhood education from the University of Atacama, northern Chile (Lería Dulčić et al., 2019).
1. Lería-Dulčić, F J, Acosta-Peña, R N & Sasso-Orellana, P E . Socio-affective words production by early childhood educators: lexical density, clusters, and predictors. Revista Electrónica de Investigación Educativa 23:In press.
2. Pluye, P & Hong, Q N . 2014. Combining the Power of Stories and the Power of Numbers: Mixed Methods Research and Mixed Studies Reviews. Annual Review of Public Health 35(1):29–45.
3. Farrow, J M, Wasik, B A & Hindman, A H . 2020. Exploring the unique contributions of teachers’ syntax to preschoolers’ and kindergarteners’ vocabulary learning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 51(2):178–190.
4. Ciesielski, E J M & Creaghead, N A . 2020. The Effectiveness of Professional Development on the Phonological Awareness Outcomes of Preschool Children: A Systematic Review. Literacy Research and Instruction 59(2):121–147.
5. Muhinyi, A & Rowe, M L . 2019. Shared reading with preverbal infants and later language development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 64(101053):1–11.
6. Markussen-Brown, J, Juhl, C B, Piasta, S B, Bleses, D, Højen, A & Justice, L M . 2017. The effects of language- and literacy-focused professional development on early educators and children: A best-evidence meta-analysis. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 38(1):97–115.
7. Lería-Dulčić, F J, Sasso-Orellana, P E & Salgado-Roa, J A . 2019. Strengthening initial professional education in Preschool teachers: Contemplative program "The rite of silence“. Aula De Encuentro 21(1):160–181.
8. Cuellar, M & Farkas, K Ch . 2018. Sensibilidad y mentalización de las educadoras de párvulos. Predictores del lenguaje infantil a los 30 meses. Perfiles educativos 40(160):64–82.
9. Prediger, S & Zindel, C . 2017. School academic language demands for understanding functional relationships: A design research project on the role of language in reading and learning. Science and Technology Education 13(7b):4157–4188.
10. Dickinson, D K, Hofer, K G, Barnes, E M & Grifenhagen, J F . 2014. Examining teachers’ language in Head Start classrooms from a systemic linguistics approach. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 29(3):231–244.
11. Hu, J, Torr, J, Degotardi, S & Han, F . 2017. Educators’ production of commanding language to direct infants’ behaviour: relationship to educators’ qualifications and implications for language learning oppor-tunities. Early Years 39(2):190–204.
12. Marzano, R J & Kendall, J S . 2008. Designing and assessing educational objectives: Applying the new taxonomy. California, EE.UU: Corwnin Press
13. D’alessio, M J, Jaichenco, V & Wilson, M A . 2019. The relationship between morphological awareness and reading comprehension in Spanish-speaking children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 1–12.
14. Hajer, M & Norén, E . 2017. Teachers’ knowledge about language in mathematics professional development courses: From an intended curriculum to a curriculum in action. Science and Technology Education 13(7b):4087–4114.
15. Hayes, A F & Krippendorff, K . 2007. Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods and Measures 1(1):77–89.
16. Pennebaker, J W, Boyd, R L, Jordan, K & Blackburn, K . 2015. The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2015. University of Texas at Austin
17. Melby-Lervåg, M . 2012. The Relative Predictive Contribution and Causal Role of Phoneme Awareness, Rhyme Awareness, and Verbal Short-Term Memory in Reading Skills: A Review. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research 56(1):101–118.
18. Roben, C K P, Cole, P M & Armstrong, L M . 2013. Longitudinal relations among language skills, anger expression, and regulatory strategies in early childhood. Child Development 84(3):891–905.
19. Hinojosa, J A, Moreno, E M & Ferré, P . 2019. Affective neurolinguistics: towards a framework for reconciling language and emotion. Language. Cognition and Neuroscience 35(7):813–839.
20. Guelmes, E L & Nieto, L E . 2015. Algunas reflexiones sobre el enfoque mixto de la investigación pedagógica en el contexto cubano. Revista Universidad y Sociedad Cienfuegos 7(2):23–29.
21. Strasser, K, Barra, G, Darricades, M & Mendive, S . 2018. Instructional activities and the quality of language in Chilean preschool classrooms. Early Education and Development 29(3):357–378.
22. Lemerise, E A & Arsenio, W F . 2000. An Integrated Model of Emotion Processes and Cognition in Social Information Processing. Child Development 71(1):107–118.
23. Pizarro, P, Peralta, N, Audisio, C, Mareovich, F, Alam, F & Rosemberg, C . 2019. El lenguaje de las educadoras y de los niños y niñas. Pensamiento Educativo: Revista de Investigación Educacional Latinoamericana 56:1–18.
24. Brown, Ch P, Ku, Da. H. & Barry, D P . 2020. Making sense of instruction within the changed kinder-garten: perspectives from preservice early childhood educators and teacher educators. Journal of Early Childhood Teacher Education 1–33.
25. Ren, Y, Rattanasone, N Xu, Demuth, K, Andronos, F & Wyver, S . 2016. Relationships between proficiency with grammatical morphemes and emotion regulation: a study of Mandarin-English preschool-ers. Early Child Development and Care 188(8):1055–1062.
26. Subsecretaria-de-educación-parvularia . 2019. Lenguaje Verbal Orientaciones Técnico-Pedagógicas para el Nivel de Educación Parvularia. Ministerio de Educación
27. Brownell, C A, Svetlova, M, Anderson, R, Nichols, S R & Drummond, J . 2012. Socialization of Early Prosocial Behavior: Parents’ Talk About Emotions is Associated With Sharing and Helping in Toddlers. Infancy 18(1):91–119.
28. Ruggeri, A, Swaboda, N, Sim, Z L & Gopnik, A . 2019. Shake it baby, but only when needed: Pre-schoolers adapt their exploratory strategies to the information structure of the task. Cognition 193(104013):1–7.
29. Justice, L M, Jiang, H & Strasser, K . 2018. Linguistic environment of preschool classrooms: What dimensions support children’s language growth? Early Childhood Research Quarterly 42(1):79–92.
30. Duncan, L G . 2018. Language and Reading: the Role of Morpheme and Phoneme Awareness. Current Developmental Disorders Reports 5(4):226–234.
31. Fontaine, J J J, Scherer, K R & Soriano, C . 2013. Components of emotional meaning: a sourcebook. (pp. 672) Oxford: Oxford University Press
32. Cekaite, A & Bergnehr, D . 2018. Affectionate touch and care: embodied intimacy, compassion and control in early childhood education. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal 26(6):940–955.
33. Reisenzein, R . 2019. Cognition and emotion: a plea for theory. Cognition and Emotion 1–10.
34. Prediger, S . 2019. Design-Research in der gegenstandsspezifischen Professionalisierungsforschung. In: T. Leuders, E. Ch., Hemmer, M., Korneck, F., Labudde & P. , eds. Fachdidaktische Forschung zur Lehrerbildung. (pp. 11-34)
35. Shirvan, M E, Khajavy, G H, Macintyre, P D & Taherian, T . 2019. A Meta-analysis of L2 Willingness to Communicate and Its Three High-Evidence Correlates. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 48:1241–1267.
36. Kalinowski, E, Egert, F, Gronostaj, A & Vock, M . 2020. Professional development on fostering students’ academic language proficiency across the curriculum-A meta-analysis of its impact on teachers’ cognition and teaching practices. Teaching and Teacher Education 88(102971):1–15.
37. Prediger, S . 2019. Investigating and promoting teachers’ expertise for language-responsive mathematics teaching. Mathematics Education Research Journal 31:367–392.
38. SEP, . 2018. Bases curriculares de la educación parvularia. Ministerio de Educación
39. Copple, C & Bredekamp, S . 2009. Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs: Serving Children from Birth through Age 8. (Vol. 8) NAEYC
40. Cunningham, J E, Zimmerman, K N, Ledford, J R & Kaiser, A P . 2019. Comparison of measurement systems for collecting teacher language data in early childhood settings. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 49:164–174.
41. King, E & La-Paro, K M . 2018. Teachers’ Emotion Minimizing Language and Toddlers’ Social Emotional Competence. Early Education and Development 29(8):989–1003.
42. Piasta, S B, Park, S, Farley, K S, Justice, L M & O'connell, A A . 2020. Early childhood educators' knowledge about language and literacy: Associations with practice and children's learning. Dyslexia 26(2):137–152.
43. Schleppegrell, M J . 2004. The language of schooling: A functional linguistics perspective. Mahwah: New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers
44. Decker-Woodrow, L . 2018. The Relationship Between Internal Teacher Profiles and the Quality of Teacher-Child Interactions in Prekindergarten. Journal of Research in Childhood Education 32(1):32–51.
Francisco José Leria Dulcic
Universidad de Atacama
Patricia Ester Sasso Orellana
Universidad de Atacama
Roxana Nora Acosta Peña
Universidad de Antofagasta
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2021. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Increasing attention is being given to early childhood educators’ verbal abilities due to its well-recognized role in teaching and learning by means of a plurality of meanings that are transmitted through them to children. Nevertheless, moderate interest has been devoted to the study of these abilities when addressing the affective domain in early childhood educators’ interactions with children. Therefore, this study identifies qualitative nodes of characteristic uses, meanings, and phrasings produced by these professionals and associated to the affective domain, to then access the degree of expertise in such communicational interactions with preschool children. Through a mixed cross-sectional design, twenty early childhood educators provided a large number of continuous audio-recordings while working in regular educational environments, to identify sentences that, by their phrasings, recall an affective connotation. The findings suggest 8 differential nodes of affective connotations: Positive emotions v/s Negative emotions; Teaching v/s Learning processes; Expression, distinguish, sharing and/or inhibition of emotions; Emotions before, during, and after behavior; Calls for attention, warnings, reprehension and praise; Touching and the body; Increasing, decreasing and normalizing language; and, educators’ own emotions. The degree of linguistic expertise was identified by experts and a blind sequential procedure, showing early childhood educators’ affective-speech is mostly limited to noticing children’s emotional experiences. The scope of these findings is briefly discussed taking into consideration future studies as well as its contributions to early childhood educators’ formative processes.