1. Introduction
An urban forest refers to the natural forest area within or near a city [1]. Their geographical advantages and natural environments make them attractive places for recreation. A forest has a unique aesthetic value compared to a park. Forest bathing, as a kind of natural therapy, has been shown to promote physical and mental health [2]. This activity is persuading more and more visitors to undertake forest recreation and promoting the development of forest tourism in regional economic growth. Further, compared to landscape parks, forests, as a wilder natural area, are often considered to be more dangerous. For this reason, some tourists restrict their travel choices, and few select forest environments, especially for outdoor parent–child activities [3]. Therefore, it is particularly important to eliminate tourists’ worries and prejudices about the urban forest environment.
To make the forest a popular recreational green space for tourists, it is essential to balance the perceived conflict between the recreational appeal and the threat of wild nature. Improving tourists’ recreation experience while convincing tourists that these environments are safe has become a key research issue in urban forest recreation development. Visitors enjoy recreation in the planned activity area in urban forest green space. Therefore, improving the service quality in the activity space can enable tourists to have a better recreational experience. According to the broken window theory [4], a well-managed and orderly green space can make tourists feel safe. Improving the management of an urban forest can be achieved by enhancing the green space’s social attribute, which is the convenience of recreational activities in the environment [5]. The service quality that reflects this trait may affect tourists’ safety perception in the urban forest. To test this hypothesis, it is necessary to have a deeper understanding of the internal relationship between safety perception and urban forest service quality.
Perception can be defined as the experience process obtained through mental understanding and processing after acquiring external information through the senses [5]. It is collectively influenced by external environmental information, emotion, and social factors. This theoretical use is also used to explain people’s perception of safety in the environment. Safety perception is a process by which people selectively filter information related to safety in the environment and pre-judge whether they are in a safe state. Factors such as objective environment, instinct, individual experience, knowledge, and social propaganda can all influence people’s recognition of safety clues in the environment [6]. Fisher and Nasar [7] proposed in their study of campus crime that in addition to prospect and shelter environmental characteristics, the assessment of escape possibility in the environment is also an important factor affecting safety assessment. Leon Van Rijswijk [8] further summarized the factors influencing safety assessment into three parts: the predictability of the environment, individuals’ behavioral control over potentially dangerous situations, and the influence of personal anxiety traits on environmental safety judgment. Although previous studies on environmental safety assessment have included some theories on safety perception, few studies have summarized them. The safety assessment of built parks tends to be divided into personal, social, and physical environments from the macro-level perspective [9]. However, people’s forest environmental safety is strongly correlated with concerns about personal safety, which is also a focus in the literature of safety perception. From the perspective of self-assessment, our research selects three main bases for personal safety judgments, environment, control, and emotion, to collect and organize the literature.
Based on the existing theoretical research results, we further clarify the three dimensions of personal safety perception and put forward the theoretical concepts of each dimension, which are the main theoretical basis for the subsequent compilation of the safety perception scale. We named these three dimensions safety environment perception, control perception, and safety emotion.
Safety environment perception is a predictive judgment that the environment is non-threatening. From the perspective of space and time, the prediction of environmental safety needs to include the judgment of both the certainty and uncertainty of environmental conditions [10]. The basic condition to judge the safety of an environment is to ensure that the space is not suitable for concealment and entrapment [11]. Concealment refers to criminals or dangerous people hiding in the space. A space with open views and no conditions for concealment is considered to be safe. Entrapment occurs in a space in which escape is obstructed. Even though there is no hazard in the environment, a physical obstacle on a path makes people feel unsafe. The uncertainty of a safe environment comes from potential threats that cannot be immediately observed when entering the environment and are assumed based on people’s own experience [9]. The forest environment is different from parks, as in addition to the uncivilized behavior associated with crime, such as empty or dilapidated infrastructure construction, wandering youth, and homeless people [12,13], there are more potential threats from the wild environment itself, including dangerous animals and insects, deep water, extreme weather, getting lost, etc. [14].
Control perception is an assessment of behavior feasibility that ensures one’s safety under an imaginary dangerous situation, which can enhance one’s confidence in safety [15]. In the natural environment, risk management is very important. Regardless of whether the main body controlling safety is oneself or others, it is an active preparation against potential dangers [16]. Approaching positive stimuli and avoiding negative stimuli are people’s instinctive behavioral tendencies [17] and are basic strategies for individuals to deal with threats. Therefore, the most basic behavioral tendencies of self-control perception can be divided into two types: approaching shelters and staying away from danger sources. The control perception from others can be regarded as a positive feedback of the “eyes on the street” effect. Individuals’ judgment on the possibility of assistance from others in the natural environment is mainly based on two points: informal social control carried out by other green space users and supervision by green space managers [18,19]. The natural control of many users over the green space makes them more likely to notice abnormal behaviors or dangers and provide help or call the management of the green space for rescue, which is also a benefit brought about by sufficient user visits [20]. Therefore, control perception can be divided into four aspects: escape, shelter, help, and rescue.
Emotional factors are rarely mentioned in studies on safety perception, but many studies have confirmed that negative emotions can greatly affect the perceived results [21,22]. People’s negative emotions towards safety are mainly expressed as anxiety and fear, which make them psychologically reject the strange natural environment [23]. In addition, loneliness is also an important factor affecting people’s safety perception in the forest environment; companionship can effectively solve this problem, even if the companionship is only in the form of a human voice [24,25]. Positive emotions are rarely mentioned in studies on safety perception, which may be because, as a basic survival need, safety does not cause rich psychological and emotional changes. The neutral position of safety psychology is often determined by negating negative emotions. However, some studies have proposed that positive emotions can also affect safety perception. As a positive emotional state, comfort can improve people’s affinity to the natural environment and inhibit the tendency to perceive threats [26].
Visual quality, accessibility, and facility completeness were selected as specific indicators of the quality of urban forest recreation services in this study. The recreation service of an urban forest closely relates to the needs of tourists. As one of the most basic needs of people, safety is the focus of research on recreation services. In several nature recreational studies, accessibility, facility completeness, and visual quality have been proven to be needs that tourists expect to be met, as well as external manifestations of the management level of natural green space [27,28,29]. Visual quality is the charm of natural green space and viewing angle presented to tourists after aesthetic selection. It fulfills the needs of tourists to get close to nature. Accessibility refers to the convenience of transportation from the urban residential area to the urban forest recreation area and reflects the closeness of service connection between nature and society. Facility completeness reflects the completeness of facilities in the man-made environment of an urban forest and is the basic guarantee for tourists to conduct social activities and outdoor sports. Facilities, transportation, and landscape are the most frequently mentioned environmental elements in environmental safety assessment research [3,30]. Existing research results have proven that the lighting quality of space can affect the perceived safety of the environment at night and further influence people’s route choices [6]. The planning and designing of public spaces such as parks with existing facilities like public telephones, fences, landscaping, and monitoring are believed to reduce the incidence of crimes [13]. In urban natural areas, the optimization of vegetation features, such as by vegetation density adjustment and vegetation space design, has been proven to have a positive impact on personal safety perception [31], which can reduce the degree of threat from wild vegetation. Maintenance is one of the important principles of crime prevention through environmental design (CPTED) theory. The maintenance trace of vegetation can indicate that the environment is under supervision and protection and thus improve tourists’ evaluation of environmental safety [32]. At the same time, the accessibility, scale, naturalness, and other qualities of urban natural areas are also believed to affect the social and psychological benefits perceived by tourists in the natural environment [33]. These research results show that there is a connection between visual quality, accessibility, and facility completeness and safety perceptions, but few studies have clarified the specific relationship between these three service qualities and safety perceptions and explained why these three services can affect tourists’ assessment of environmental safety.
In this empirical study, our main research purpose is to test the hypothesis that facility completeness, accessibility, and visual quality can affect tourists’ perception of safety in urban forests. To have a deeper understanding of the relationship between the three service qualities of urban forests and safety perception, this study is divided into two stages. In the first stage, we divide the internal dimensions of safety perception, which will help to obtain more comprehensive and specific safety perception assessment results. Based on collating and summarizing the existing research results, we construct a theoretical framework of safety perception and conduct field application tests to verify the scientific nature of the division of the internal dimensions of safety perception. In the second phase, we explore the impact of facility completeness, accessibility, and visual quality on the three safety perception dimensions. In the specific research process, we also explore specific paths through which three service quality measures affect environmental safety assessment and build the internal mechanism model between them. This helps us to have a deeper understanding of the internal reasons why these three service qualities affect safety perception and analyze the differences and commonalities between them. This can provide a reference for formulating effective measures to improve the perception of urban forest safety, and also has important enlightening significance for the design and management of urban forests.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Overview of the Study Area
The research was carried out in Fuzhou from 15 September 2020 to 16 April 2021. Fuzhou, as the capital of Fujian Province, is located on the coast of the East China Sea. The landscape of Fuzhou is a typical estuary basin surrounded by mountains. The altitude of its mountains is mostly between 600~1000 m, and it has rich forest resources. In the first stage of the study, five urban forest recreational lands in Fuzhou were selected to verify the safety perception scale (Figure 1): Jinjishan Park, Fuzhou Urban Forest Trail, Fuzhou Forest Park, Fushan Country Park, and Niugangshan Park. These are the five green spaces with the highest proportion of natural forests around the main city of Fuzhou, including two geographical elements of an urban interior and urban edge. Fuzhou Forest Park (F1) is a comprehensive park that integrates scientific research and tourism. Fushan Country Park (F2) has effectively highlighted the beauty of the wilderness landscape through planning and transformation. Fuzhou Urban Forest Trail (F3) is completely built in the forest, retaining the original appearance of the natural mountains and forests. Jinjishan Park (F4) is located at the foot of Jinji Mountain and has many scenic spots and historical sites. Niugangshan Park (F5) is a small woodland park that is easy to explore.
To further study the relationship between urban forest service quality and safety perception, Fuzhou Urban Forest Trail was selected to be representative of an urban forest green space in the second stage, to reduce the influence of other interference factors on urban forest service quality and safety perception. Fuzhou Urban Forest Trail is located in the city’s Gulou district. The main part of the trail is built along the ridge line of Jinniu Mountain. The total length of its axis is 6.3 km, and the total length of the loop is about 19 km. It is the first urban forest walkway in China to adopt a steel frame hollow-out design. All along the trail is a natural forest landscape with a unique canopy and a wide variety of species, providing a natural and novel experience for visitors. Fuzhou Urban Forest Trail has now become the first choice for forest strolling in Fuzhou. It has a relatively large number of tourists, which makes it a feasible and convenient location for this study.
2.2. Methods of Research Phase 1
The internal dimensions of security perception are mainly divided based on the results of previous studies, so a structural validity test was required. This was the prerequisite for the follow-up research. To obtain information about tourists’ safety perception quickly and effectively, it was important to develop a set of feasible safety perception measurement tools. This was also an important way to test the rationality of the safety perception dimension division.
2.2.1. Questionnaire
The theoretical framework of personal safety perception that we collated compiles four measurement items for each of the three dimensions of safety perception. To make the project description simpler and more user-friendly, without jargon, we referred to some topics and item description methods of Han’s self-rating restoration scale (SRRS) and state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI) [34,35]. After a collective discussion between the questionnaire designers, a list of scale items (12 items) was formed. Later, the researcher conducted interviews at the research site to ensure the reliability of the questionnaire. The researcher compiled the safety perception scale (SPS) after making a semantic adjustment and supplementary annotation to the items’ descriptions that were difficult to understand or were ambiguous and then conducted a preliminary survey. In addition to the 5-point semantic difference scale used in the measurement of emotional description, a 5-point Likert scale was used for the other items. The final questionnaire was formed after further adjustment according to the results of the preliminary survey.
2.2.2. Participants and Procedure
The study was conducted from 15 September to 20 October in 2020. To ensure the scientific nature of the dimension division of the safety perception scale, the collected test data were provided by users of several urban forests and green spaces in Fuzhou according to their experience, reducing the interference of the environment on safety perception. The questionnaires were randomly given out in five urban green forest areas during legal holidays, weekends, and working days. The items in the questionnaire were randomly sorted. Before the survey, the purpose of the study and the matters needing attention were explained in detail to those who were willing to complete the questionnaire to improve the efficiency of questionnaire collection. Ninety questionnaires were sent out in each of the five urban forest green spaces. In total, 450 questionnaires were sent out, and 411 valid questionnaires were collected, for an effective questionnaire rate of 91.3%. Among the respondents, 187 were male and 224 were female; 17 participants were under 18 years old; 177 people were between 18 and 25 years old; 112 people were between 26 and 35 years old; 56 people were between 36 and 45 years old; 32 people were between 46 and 55 years old; and 13 people were 56 years old and above.
2.3. Methods of the Research Phase 2
Based on the research results of phase 1, we proposed the basic theoretical conception of safety perception. In phase 2, we further verified this theory and applied it to research on the relationship between service quality and safety perception. The intrinsic process of safety perception can be understood from the perspective of human–environment interaction. According to the human environmental interaction (HEI) model [36], human feelings towards social and physical environments can be influenced by individual reaction tendencies. Control perception can be seen as a person’s tendency to react to threats and as an intermediary variable between safety emotions and safety environment perception. The purpose of phase 2 was to explore the impact of facility completeness, accessibility, and visual quality on environmental safety assessment. To achieve the purpose of this study, we proposed the structural equation model (SEM) relationship between variables as shown in Figure 2 and made the following hypotheses.
The three internal dimensions of safety perception are all affected by the visual quality of the landscape.
Visual quality has a significantly positive effect on safety environment perception.
Visual quality has a significantly positive effect on control perception.
Visual quality has a significantly positive effect on safety emotion.
The three internal dimensions of safety perception are all affected by accessibility.
Accessibility has a significantly positive effect on safety environment perception.
Accessibility has a significantly positive effect on control perception.
Accessibility has a significantly positive effect on safety emotion.
The three internal dimensions of safety perception are all affected by facility completeness.
Facility completeness has a significantly positive effect on safety environment perception.
Facility completeness has a significantly positive effect on control perception.
Facility completeness has a significantly positive effect on safety emotion.
Safety emotion has a significantly positive effect on safety environment perception.
Control perception has a significantly positive impact on safety environment perception.
Safety emotion has a significantly positive influence on control perception.
2.3.1. Questionnaire
In phase 2, the questionnaire was divided into two parts: service quality and safety perception. In the study on the service quality of the Fuzhou Urban Forest Trail, some researchers have constructed evaluation indices for landscape visual quality, facility completeness, and accessibility [37,38], which have been verified to be effective in the field investigations. The service quality survey in the questionnaire was modified and compiled according to the existing research results, and the scoring standard was also a 5-point Likert scale. The scale and scoring criteria in phase 1 were used to investigate safety perception.
2.3.2. Participants and Procedure
This research was conducted from 2 November 2020 to 28 November 2020 and from 20 March 2021 to 16 April 2021, and included working days with few tourists and days off on which more tourists visited the area. One hundred volunteers were invited to participate in a walking tour of a specific distance at five regions with different landscape characteristics on the Fuzhou Urban Forest Trail (Figure 3, Table 1). They later completed the questionnaire about the service quality and safety perception of the forest trail after they were familiar with the surrounding environment. In total, 500 people were surveyed and 480 questionnaires were collected, for an effective questionnaire rate of 96%. Of the participants, 233 were males and 247 were females; there were 33 people under 18 years old; 123 people between 18 and 25 years old; 158 people between 26 and 35 years old; 92 people between 36 and 45 years old; 51 people between 46 and 55 years old; and 23 people 56 years old and above.
2.4. Statistical Analysis
SPSS 24.0 and AMOS 24.0 were used for data processing and statistical analysis. This study followed the basic procedures and principles of scale development when designing the questionnaire. Construct validity was mainly embodied by convergent validity and discriminant validity. The Cronbach’s α coefficient of the questionnaire was greater than 0.7, indicating that the measurement has good stability. The SEM was evaluated according to the theoretical perspective and according to the fit quality criteria. The maximum likelihood method was used to estimate the parameters of the structural model. The bias-corrected percentile bootstrap method was used to test the mediating effect.
3. Results
3.1. Reliability and Validity Test of the Safety Perception Scale
3.1.1. Project Analysis and Reliability Analysis
The results showed that the critical ratio (CR) values of all items were between 18.965 and 28.039, or greater than the minimum reference value of 3.000, and the significance test probability p-value was less than 0.001. The score differences of all items were statistically significant. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the safety perception scale was 0.911; this coefficient did not increase when any question was removed, indicating that the scale had good internal reliability.
3.1.2. Exploratory Factor Analysis and Testing
The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy was 0.907 for the safety perception scale, the approximate chi-square distribution of Bartlett’s sphericity test was 3043.224, and the significance probability value was p < 0.001, reaching the significant level. The above findings represent that the sample data were suitable for factor analysis. Based on principal component analysis of the data sample (Table 2), the cumulative variance contribution rate of the three common factors of the safety perception scale was 73.909%. The percentage of the explanatory variance of factor 1 was 25.227%, that of factor 2 was 24.912% and that of factor 3 was 23.769%. The classification results of the principal component factors were consistent with the envisaged theoretical framework and expectations. The three factors were respectively named control perception, safety emotion, and safety environment perception.
3.2. Structural Equation Model Testing
3.2.1. Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Test
Among the overall fit indices, the chi-square degree of freedom ratio (CMIN/DF) was 2.660. The root means square error of the approximation (RMSEA) was 0.059, which is less than the reference value of 0.08. The goodness-of-fit index (GFI) was 0.903, the normed fit index (NFI) was 0.910, the incremental fit index (IFI) was 0.942, the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) was 0.931, and the comparative fit index (CFI) was 0.942; all of these figures were greater than the reference value of 0.9. The results showed that the overall model fits well. The phase 2 study questionnaire used Cronbach’s alpha to test the internal reliability of each category (Table 3). The results were all greater than the reference value of 0.7, indicating that the questionnaire has good reliability.
Construct reliability (CR) and average variance extracted (AVE) were used to detect the testing variables’ convergent validity (Table 4). The results showed that the various factors in the questionnaire of AVE values were greater than the reference value of 0.5, and CR values were greater than the reference value of 0.7. All observed variables had good internal consistency and good convergence in each latent variable.
Discriminant validity of the test results showed that all six latent variables had a significant correlation with each other (p < 0.001, Table 5). The correlation coefficients were less than the square root of AVE, suggesting that all latent variables had both a certain correlation and differentiation between each other.
3.2.2. Structural Model
SEM was used to test the hypothesis of the relationship between service quality and safety perception (Table 6). The best fitting model was shown in Figure 4. The fitting index of the model was CMIN/DF = 2.447, RMSEA = 0.055, GFI = 0.912, NFI = 0.918, IFI = 0.950, TLI = 0.940, and CFI = 0.949. The theoretical model had a good degree of fit.
The first hypothesis test results (Table 6) supported that visual quality could positively affect safety emotion (p < 0.001), but the hypothesis that visual quality could positively affect safety environment perception and control perception was rejected. The second hypothesis test results supported that traffic accessibility positively affected control perception (p < 0.001), but the hypothesis that traffic accessibility positively affected safety environment perception and safety emotion was rejected. The third hypothesis test results rejected that facility completeness could positively affect safety environment perception, but the hypothesis that facility completeness could positively affect control perception and safety emotion was accepted. The fourth hypothesis explored the internal mechanism of safety perception, and all hypotheses were supported. Both safety emotion and control perception had a significantly positive impact on safety environment perception, and safety emotion also had a significantly positive impact on control perception.
3.2.3. Mediation Inspection
Intermediary inspection results (Table 7) found that safety emotion played a mediating role between landscape visual quality and safety environment perception (p = 0.009), while control perception did not (p = 0.145). Both safety emotion and control perception were mediating variables between facility completeness and safety environment perception (p = 0.001). Control perception played a mediating role between traffic accessibility and safety environment perception (p = 0.007), while safety emotion did not (p = 0.587). In the internal mechanism of safety perception, control perception played a moderately mediating role. Control perception was a mediating variable between safety emotion and safety environment perception (p = 0.001).
4. Discussion
4.1. Internal Components of Safety Perception
Used alongside the results of exploratory factor analysis and confirmatory factor analysis in the two research stages (Table 2, Table 4 and Table 5), the compiled safety perception scale had good construct validity, convergent validity, and discriminant validity. The three dimensions of safety perception in an urban forest environment (safety environmental perception, control perception, and safety emotion) could be properly distinguished in people’s cognition and were consistent with the theoretical conception of personal safety perception.
Among the predictors of safety environment perception, in addition to the two physical characteristics of the environment (concealment and entrapment), the empirical prediction of potential threats is also an important basis for judging environmental safety. According to the calculation results (Table 2), the item of worries about dangerous people (0.776) had close factor loading values to the item of worries about animals and insects (0.797). It was common for tourists to worry about animals and insects in the forest. However, we encountered very few dangerous people during our investigation, but dangerous people were still generally perceived by tourists as a potential threat. News reports or past experiences still made them worry about encountering dangerous people in a small space separated by dense trees [39]. Even if the actual management was improved, this insecurity may not be eliminated. In some studies, the experience of danger is often viewed only as a personal characteristic of the tourist [40,41], but when certain threats are widely experienced or feared enough to become widely understood features of the particular environment, they will become an inherent cognitive feature of the environment, even if the feature is not visible. This finding further explains and confirms the correlation between safety perception and threat concern proposed by Farbod [13].
Safety control has always been the focus of safety research. The analysis results of the control perception dimension showed that the control expectation for others had a higher factor load value than the self-judgment of the individual’s control ability, which was the main factor in the control perception dimension. This may be because living creatures have the instinct of self-preservation. When facing danger, people are used to putting themselves in a weak or safe position, hoping that others can face or deal with the danger for them. This subconscious psychological dependence may also explain the findings of Jorgensen et al. [42] that visitors feel safer when they see other users who are engaged in the expected and acceptable use of space because these people are potential objects who may help them. In addition, the research results also validate the findings of Thomas [43] based on a large number of studies on the forest environments and emphasize the important role of movement ease in the perception of danger.
There are few studies at present on safety perception related to positive emotions, and there is only one item on positive emotions (“I feel uncomfortable or comfortable”) in the dimension of safety emotion. However, it can still be proven that the main information reflected by comfort (positive emotion) is consistent with fear, anxiety, and loneliness of the negative emotions in safety cognition. Although positive emotion is more significant in other psychological benefits [44], its influence on safety perception cannot be ignored and requires qualitative study.
4.2. Relationship between Urban Forest Service Quality and Safety Perception
Phase 2 of the study confirmed that the internal relationship of safety perception that control perception played a partial mediating role between safety emotion and safety environment perception. Studies on behavioral science suggest that stimulation from threats triggers a physical defense response in the human body, accompanied by the emotion of fear [45]. The results have shown that people can reversely adjust this instinctive response. Tourists with high psychological safety are more inclined to think that their safety has been effectively guaranteed, and this optimistic expectation of safety control can improve their evaluation of environmental safety.
The urban forest service quality measures of visual quality and facility completeness has a direct positive impact on safety emotion and indirectly affects safety environment perception through safety emotion. Good visual quality can counteract the negative emotions of the visitors and bring them peace of mind. In one existing study, a charming natural landscape was also shown to influence tourists’ positive emotions [46]. As a result, safety emotions can also be regarded as the premise that the natural landscape further produces other psychological benefits. Perfect facilities can meet the various recreational needs of tourists in the natural green space, while unsatisfied needs directly trigger their negative emotions, which affects their state of psychological safety. The same interpretation applies to visitors whose primary need is to enjoy the natural landscape of an urban forest. This finding is consistent with the conclusion of other empirical studies that satisfaction of basic needs negatively correlates with negative psychological states (such as anxiety and depression) [47,48]. According to the model analysis findings (Table 6, Figure 4), facility completeness (0.359) has a greater impact on safety emotion than visual quality (0.243). It is normal for tourists to admire the natural scenery while visiting the forest green places. This is their primary objective for visiting the forest. When people enjoy their touring experience, they often overlook facilities. However, when tourists need to rest or find toilets, inadequate or non-existent facilities might leave tourists’ physiological needs unsatisfied. This impact further amplifies insecurity.
Although visual quality and facility completeness of an urban forest cannot be used as the direct predictor of environmental safety, they can enhance the psychological tolerance of tourists to environmental threats by stabilizing their psychological safety state, thus indirectly affecting their environmental safety assessment. This is consistent with the conclusion of empirical psychological research. Indeed, individuals with low anxiety will show higher tolerance to threats when they are rewarded [49]. In other words, when tourists are immersed in the recreation services of an urban forest, they are more likely to ignore the potential threat of the wild forest itself.
The urban forest service quality measures of accessibility and facility completeness had a direct impact on control perception and indirectly affected safety environment perception through control perception. We noticed that appealing and well-developed sites, such as multi-purpose plazas and rest stops, were often accessed by visitors throughout our survey. The results (Figure 4) showed that in addition to safety facilities and maintenance, the completeness of other facilities is also very important for urban forests, which affects tourists’ judgment of control perception. This is because when faced with danger, people rely on the facilities and tools available around them to fight threats. High-quality facilities can help tourists deal with different emergencies and reduce the probability of danger. The reason that accessibility has a positive impact on tourists’ control perception is that control is a continuous process that lasts until their safety is guaranteed. When tourists are in danger, they will not only control and judge the environment within their visual range but also plan a subsequent evacuation route to ensure that they can quickly return to safe accommodation from the green space or more easily access continuous support and rescue. Tourists’ ambivalence towards nature is largely derived from survival anxiety, and individuals are more likely to realize their vulnerability in the wilderness environment [50]. From the results of the model analysis (Table 6, Figure 4), facility completeness (0.304) can affect control perception more than accessibility (0.138). Tourists tended to pay more attention to the presence and quality of facilities than to accessibility, with signage and monitoring often seen during tours, constantly reminding visitors that they were being protected. The two aspects of accessibility and facility completeness can improve tourists’ subconscious survival confidence by emphasizing social support and lowering tourists’ assessment level of environmental threats. This is consistent with the results of empirical studies, in which the availability of avoidance can keep threats at a low level [51]. When tourists realize that most potential threats can be avoided or solved by the built environment, they will judge wild forests to be non-threatening.
Fieldwork is an extremely effective way to understand tourists’ safety perceptions of urban forests. However, this survey also has limitations. The data collected in this survey do not accurately represent the opinions of people during all months of the year or times of day. Forests vary in their hazard and attractiveness depending on the season. This survey was conducted during the daylight hours of winter and spring when there were numerous tourists. In future, an independent survey is required to investigate periods of low tourism or at night, when the presence of fewer people might exacerbate feelings of insecurity. The study findings’ dependability and utility will be evaluated further following a comparison of various periods.
The findings of the study also highlight several new research directions. First, the survey of landscape visual quality in this study mainly comes from the evaluation of tourists after viewing the distant wild forest landscape. The aesthetic appreciation will be affected by the sense of distance. Whether the wild landscape preserved at different viewing distances can generate tourists’ aesthetic appreciation and promote their safety emotions is a matter for future research. Second, facility completeness mainly relies on the visual judgment of tourists. It is also meant to explore whether the facilities experience of other sensory forms will affect the perception of safety, such as the comfort of paving and seats and the use of familiar materials. It may subconsciously make tourists feel safe and at ease.
5. Conclusions
This study has discussed the relationship between service quality and safety perception in an urban forest, constructed the theoretical framework of safety perception by evaluating the previous research results on green space safety perception, and compiled a simple safety perception measurement scale. This study evaluated the role of visual quality, facility completeness, and accessibility of urban forest space service quality in the process of safety perception and tested the availability of safety perception scale. The results showed that good service quality in an urban forest green space can improve people’s safety perception, specifically through directly improving tourists’ safety emotion or control perception and then improving their evaluation of environmental safety. It was also found that different service qualities affect the safety perception of an intrinsic mechanism path differently. This reflects the finding that safety perception is a mutual and complicated internal process and also proves that the urban forest environment can improve the safety perception of tourists through targeted service quality optimization.
Conceptualization, H.W. and J.Y.; Data curation, M.W.K.T. and Y.L.; Formal analysis, H.W.; Funding acquisition, Y.Z.; Methodology, H.W. and J.Y.; Project administration, H.W.; Software, H.W.; Writing—original draft, H.W.; Writing—review & editing, M.W.K.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
This research was funded by the Major Science and Technology Project of Fujian Province, China, grant number 2013NZ0001.
Not applicable.
Not applicable.
The data used to support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Figure 1. Locations of 5 Urban Forest Recreation Areas in the main city of Fuzhou: S1, the location of the main city of Fuzhou; F1, Fuzhou Forest Park; F2, Fushan Country Park; F3, Fuzhou Urban Forest Trail; F4, Jinjishan Park; F5, Niugangshan Park.
Figure 3. Location of five walking tour areas in Fuzhou Urban Forest Trail. (W1~W5, walking tour area numbers).
Figure 4. Structural equation modelling results. (Dotted line, hypothesis rejected; solid line, hypothesis accepted).
Study site descriptions of Fuzhou Urban Forest Trail.
Research Positions | Location Description | Pavement Material | Common Animals | Distances from Entrances (m) | Number of Monitors |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
W1 | Observation deck with wide view | Glass; wood | Birds, squirrels | 1100 | 1 |
W2 | Wooded horizontal walking trails | Steel frame hollow-out | Snakes, spiders, mosquitoes | 800 | 1 |
W3 | Rest point at the intersection | Steel frame hollow-out | Birds, squirrels, mosquitoes | 600 | 0 |
W4 | High-altitude continuous uphill walking path | Steel frame hollow-out | Birds, snakes | 1500 | 2 |
W5 | Multifunctional event service plaza | Permeable brick | Birds, squirrels, stray dogs | 500 | 3 |
Information derived from the author’s field records.
Results of exploratory analysis.
Items | Factor 1 | Factor 2 | Factor 3 |
---|---|---|---|
If I am in danger, I can quickly seek help from the managers of the scenic spot | 0.854 | ||
If I am in danger, I can easily seek help from others | 0.834 | ||
If I am in danger, I can quickly find a shelter to hide or protect myself | 0.817 | ||
If I am in danger, I can quickly determine the direction of travel and escape | 0.717 | ||
Here, I feel anxious or not anxious | 0.875 | ||
Here, I feel alone or not alone | 0.822 | ||
Here, I feel uncomfortable or comfortable | 0.767 | ||
Here, I feel scared or not scared | 0.725 | ||
Here, I am not worried about being infested by annoying or scary animals or insects | 0.797 | ||
There is no dark or obstructed space around | 0.788 | ||
Here, I will not worry about encountering dangerous people | 0.776 | ||
There are no obstacles on the road that prevent me from escaping from danger | 0.717 | ||
Eigenvalue | 6.191 | 1.536 | 1.143 |
Eigenvalue variance explained (%) | 25.227 | 24.912 | 23.769 |
Cumulative variance explained (%) | 25.227 | 50.14 | 73.909 |
Factor 1, 2, and 3: commonalities of items.
Reliabilities of categories.
Category | Items | Var. | Mean | S.D. | Alpha |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Safety environment perception | There is no dark or obstructed space around | ENV1 | 4.120 | 1.061 | 0.874 |
Here, I am not worried about being infested by annoying or scary animals or insects | ENV2 | 4.160 | 1.023 | ||
Here, I will not worry about encountering dangerous people | ENV3 | 4.260 | 1.043 | ||
There are no obstacles on the road that prevent me from escaping from danger | ENV4 | 4.210 | 0.992 | ||
Safety emotion | Here, I feel uncomfortable or comfortable | EMO1 | 4.200 | 1.055 | 0.849 |
Here, I feel alone or not alone | EMO2 | 4.440 | 0.868 | ||
Here, I feel anxious or not anxious | EMO3 | 4.500 | 0.820 | ||
Here, I feel scared or not scared | EMO4 | 4.320 | 0.882 | ||
Control perception | If I am in danger, I can quickly seek help from the managers of the scenic spot | CON1 | 4.260 | 0.924 | 0.854 |
If I am in danger, I can easily seek help from others | CON2 | 3.790 | 1.126 | ||
If I am in danger, I can quickly find a shelter to hide or protect myself | CON3 | 3.800 | 1.127 | ||
If I am in danger, I can quickly determine the direction of travel and escape | CON4 | 3.680 | 1.108 | ||
Visual quality | The ecological woodland here is very ornamental | VIS1 | 4.280 | 0.871 | 0.886 |
The landscape here is very colorful | VIS2 | 4.150 | 0.945 | ||
Here undulating terrain of the mountain is very beautiful | VIS3 | 4.350 | 0.825 | ||
The design of the trails here is very beautiful | VIS4 | 4.410 | 0.828 | ||
Accessibility | It is convenient to get here by public transportation in the city | ACC1 | 3.680 | 0.840 | 0.749 |
The time it took to get here was in line with my expectations | ACC2 | 3.440 | 0.898 | ||
The number and location of entrances and exits are convenient for me to reach | ACC3 | 3.560 | 0.957 | ||
Facility completeness | The identification systems here are very complete and numerous | FAC1 | 4.220 | 0.885 | 0.811 |
The monitoring facilities and protective fences here are very complete | FAC2 | 4.250 | 0.856 | ||
The sanitation facilities here are very complete | FAC3 | 4.370 | 0.832 | ||
The rest facilities here are very complete | FAC4 | 3.990 | 1.028 |
S.D., Standard deviation.
Convergent validity.
Factor | Variables | Std. Coefficient | AVE | CR |
---|---|---|---|---|
Safety environment perception | ESP1 | 0.721 | 0.640 | 0.876 |
ESP2 | 0.795 | |||
ESP3 | 0.88 | |||
ESP4 | 0.797 | |||
Safety emotion | EMO1 | 0.748 | 0.604 | 0.858 |
EMO2 | 0.81 | |||
EMO3 | 0.866 | |||
EMO4 | 0.671 | |||
Control perception | BEH1 | 0.866 | 0.599 | 0.854 |
BEH2 | 0.857 | |||
BEH3 | 0.754 | |||
BEH4 | 0.583 | |||
Visual quality | AES1 | 0.87 | 0.668 | 0.889 |
AES2 | 0.868 | |||
AES3 | 0.804 | |||
AES4 | 0.717 | |||
Accessibility | ACC1 | 0.761 | 0.507 | 0.755 |
ACC2 | 0.715 | |||
ACC3 | 0.656 | |||
Facility completeness | FAC1 | 0.837 | 0.552 | 0.829 |
FAC2 | 0.817 | |||
FAC3 | 0.725 | |||
FAC4 | 0.561 |
Notes: CR, construct reliability; AVE, average variance extracted.
Discriminant validity and the correlations of variables.
Variable | Safety Environment Perception | Safety |
Control Perception | Visual Quality | Traffic |
Facility |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Safety environment perception | 0.640 | |||||
Safety emotion | 0.762 *** | 0.604 | ||||
Control perception | 0.560 *** | 0.521 *** | 0.599 | |||
Visual quality | 0.472 *** | 0.518 *** | 0.498 *** | 0.668 | ||
Accessibility | 0.192 *** | 0.191 *** | 0.312 *** | 0.197 *** | 0.507 | |
Facility completeness | 0.499 *** | 0.532 *** | 0.574 *** | 0.697 *** | 0.328 *** | 0.552 |
Square root of AVE | 0.800 | 0.777 | 0.774 | 0.817 | 0.712 | 0.743 |
*** indicates that the p value is less than 0.001, and the diagonal line is the amount of AVE evaluation variance variation extraction.
Summary of hypotheses’ results.
Hypothesis | Estimate | S.E. | C.R. | P | Result |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
H1-1. Visual quality → safety environment perception | 0.018 | 0.077 | 0.329 | 0.742 | Not accepted |
H1-2. Visual quality → control perception | 0.114 | 0.063 | 1.773 | 0.076 | Not accepted |
H1-3. Visual quality → safety emotion | 0.243 | 0.084 | 3.517 | 0.000 | Accepted |
H2-1. Accessibility → safety environment perception | −0.008 | 0.054 | −0.181 | 0.857 | Not accepted |
H2-2. Accessibility → control perception | 0.138 | 0.044 | 2.724 | 0.006 | Accepted |
H2-3. Accessibility → safety emotion | 0.028 | 0.056 | 0.523 | 0.601 | Not accepted |
H3-1. Facility completeness → safety environment perception | 0.038 | 0.086 | 0.589 | 0.556 | Not accepted |
H3-2. Facility completeness → control perception | 0.304 | 0.072 | 3.966 | 0.000 | Accepted |
H3-3. Facility completeness → safety emotion | 0.359 | 0.091 | 4.53 | 0.000 | Accepted |
H4-1. Safety emotion → safety environment perception | 0.627 | 0.069 | 10.427 | 0.000 | Accepted |
H4-2. Control perception → safety environment perception | 0.204 | 0.076 | 3.792 | 0.000 | Accepted |
H4-3. Safety emotion → control perception | 0.277 | 0.047 | 4.76 | 0.000 | Accepted |
S.E.: Approximate standard error; C.R.: Critical ratio.
Test of bootstrap indirect effect.
Parameter | Estimate | SE | Bias-Corrected 95%CI | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower | Upper | P | |||
VIS → CON → ENV | 0.032 | 0.027 | −0.014 | 0.095 | 0.146 |
VIS → EMO → ENV | 0.212 | 0.09 | 0.058 | 0.407 | 0.009 |
FAC → CON → ENV | 0.082 | 0.038 | 0.025 | 0.18 | 0.001 |
FAC → EMO → ENV | 0.299 | 0.09 | 0.144 | 0.494 | 0.001 |
ACC → CON → ENV | 0.034 | 0.018 | 0.007 | 0.084 | 0.007 |
ACC → EMO → ENV | 0.021 | 0.047 | −0.069 | 0.118 | 0.583 |
EMO → CON → ENV | 0.065 | 0.029 | 0.022 | 0.133 | 0.001 |
Note: SE, bootstrap standard error; CI, confidence interval; VIS, visual quality; ACC, accessibility; FAC, facility completeness; ENV, safety environment perception; EMO, safety emotion; CON, control perception.
References
1. Konijnendijk, C.C. New perspectives for urban forests: Introducing wild woodlands. Wild Urban Woodlands; Kowarik, I.; Körner, S. Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2005; pp. 33-45.
2. Marc, R.F.; Kyle, W. A Review of Field Experiments on the Effect of Forest Bathing on Anxiety and Heart Rate Variability. Glob. Adv. Health Med.; 2019; 8, 2164956119848654. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2164956119848654]
3. Sonti, N.F.; Campbell, L.K.; Svendsen, E.S.; Johnson, M.L.; Auyeung, D.S.N. Fear and fascination: Use and perceptions of New York City’s forests, wetlands, and landscaped park areas. Urban For. Urban Green.; 2020; 49, 126601.
4. Kuo, F.E.; Bacaicoa, M.; Sullivan, W.C. Transforming inner-city landscapes: Trees. Environ. Behav.; 1998; 30, 28. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013916598301002]
5. Patrik, G.; Ulrika, K.S. The relation between perceived sensory dimensions of urban green space and stress restoration. Landsc. Urban Plan.; 2009; 94, pp. 264-275.
6. Rahm, J.; Sternudd, C.; Johansson, M. “In the evening, I don’t walk in the park”: The interplay between street lighting and greenery in perceived safety. Urban Des. Int.; 2021; 26, pp. 42-52. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1057/s41289-020-00134-6]
7. Fisher, B.S.; Nasar, J.L. Fear of Crime in Relation to Three Exterior Site Features: Prospect, refuge, and escape. Environ. Behav.; 1992; 24, pp. 35-65. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013916592241002]
8. Leon, V.R.; Gerrit, R.; Antal, H. Safety in the eye of the beholder: Individual susceptibility to safety-related characteristics of nocturnal urban scenes. J. Environ. Psychol.; 2016; 45, pp. 103-115.
9. Maruthaveeran, S.; van den Bosch, C.C.K. A socio-ecological exploration of fear of crime in urban green spaces—A systematic review. Urban For. Urban Green.; 2014; 13, pp. 1-18.
10. Robert, K.C. Predictability, Constancy, and Contingency of Periodic Phenomena. Ecology; 1974; 55, pp. 1148-1153.
11. Boomsma, C.; Steg, L. Feeling Safe in the Dark: Examining the Effect of Entrapment, Lighting Levels, and Gender on Feelings of Safety and Lighting Policy Acceptability. Environ. Behav.; 2014; 46, pp. 193-212. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013916512453838]
12. Townsand, P.-S.; Wayne, A.S. Neighborhood disorder and individual community capacity: How incivilities inform three domains of psychosocial assessment. Sociol. Spectr.; 2011; 31, pp. 579-605.
13. Sara, F.; Mustafa, K.; Suhardi, M. Safety perception and concerns in naturalistic landscapes of urban parks in Malaysia. Secur. J.; 2017; 30, pp. 106-122.
14. Agnes, E.V.D.B.; Marlien, T.H. Fear versus fascination: An exploration of emotional responses to natural threats. J. Environ. Psychol.; 2005; 25, pp. 261-272.
15. Bandura, A. Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. Self-Effic. Beliefs Adolesc.; 2006; 5, pp. 307-337.
16. Gstaettner, A.M.; Lee, D.; Weiler, B. Responsibility and preparedness for risk in national parks: Results of a visitor survey. Tour. Recreat. Res.; 2020; 45, pp. 485-499. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02508281.2020.1745474]
17. Susanne, V.; Lars, S. Stress, aggression, and the balance of approach and avoidance. Psychoneuroendocrinology; 2019; 103, pp. 137-146.
18. Youngs, Y. Danger Beyond This Point: Visual Representation, Cultural Landscapes, and the Geography of Environmental Hazards in U.S. National Parks. GeoHumanities; 2020; 6, pp. 314-346. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/2373566X.2020.1784017]
19. Lis, A.; Iwankowski, P. Where do we want to see other people while relaxing in a city park? Visual relationships with park users and their impact on preferences, safety and privacy. J. Environ. Psychol.; 2021; 73, 101532. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101532]
20. Jacobs, J. The Death and Life of Great American Cities: The Failure of Town Planning; Random House, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1961.
21. Jianxun, Y.; Qi, Z.; Xingyu, L.; Miaomiao, L.; Shen, Q.; Jun, B. Biased perception misguided by affect: How does emotional experience lead to incorrect judgments about environmental quality?. Glob. Environ. Chang.; 2018; 53, pp. 104-113.
22. Paul, V.S.; Karen, R.; Christopher, W.; Jurjen, J.; Joseph, O. Risk as affect: The affect heuristic in cybersecurity. Comput. Secur.; 2020; 90, 101651.
23. Whitzman, C. Feminist activism for safer social space in High Park, Toronto: How women got lost in the woods. Can. J. Urban Res.; 2002; 11, pp. 299-321.
24. Henk, S.; Terry, H. Alone or with a friend: A social context for psychological restoration and environmental preferences. J. Environ. Psychol.; 2003; 24, pp. 199-211.
25. Yiping, L.; Mengjun, H.; Bing, Z. Audio-visual interactive evaluation of the forest landscape based on eye-tracking experiments. Urban For. Urban Green.; 2019; 46, 126476.
26. Feagan, E.R. Perceptions of safety on the North Cconee River Greenway. Ph.D. Thesis; University of Georgia: Athens, GA, USA, 2011.
27. Lorena, P.; Izaskun, C.-A.; Miren, O. Mapping recreation supply and demand using an ecological and a social evaluation approach. Ecosyst. Serv.; 2015; 13, pp. 108-118.
28. Heagney, E.C.; Rose, J.M.; Ardeshiri, A.; Kovač, M. Optimising recreation services from protected areas—Understanding the role of natural values, built infrastructure and contextual factors. Ecosyst. Serv.; 2018; 31, pp. 358-370. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2017.10.007]
29. Rosa, M.G.; Ángel, S.M.; Manuel, N.-I. Tourists’ travel time values using discrete choice models: The recreational value of the Teide National Park. J. Sustain. Tour.; 2018; 26, pp. 2021-2042.
30. Lis, A.; Iwankowski, P. Why is dense vegetation in city parks unpopular? The mediative role of sense of privacy and safety. Urban For. Urban Green.; 2021; 59, 126988. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ufug.2021.126988]
31. Märit, J.; Hanna, F.; Therese, L.; Björn, W. Perceived personal safety in relation to urban woodland vegetation—A review. Urban For. Urban Green.; 2013; 12, pp. 127-133.
32. Sharifah, K.S.O.T.; Nor, H.M.H.; Wan, H.W.I. Surveillance by Design: Assessment Using Principles of Crime Prevention through Environmental Design (CPTED) in Urban Parks. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci.; 2016; 234, pp. 506-514.
33. Åsa, O.S.; Igor, K.; Bengt, G.; Marcus, H. The effects of naturalness, gender, and age on how urban green space is perceived and used. Urban For. Urban Green.; 2016; 18, pp. 268-276.
34. Ke-Tsung, H. A reliable and valid self-rating measure of the restorative quality of natural environments. Landsc. Urban Plan.; 2003; 64, pp. 209-232.
35. Vagg, P.R.; Spielberger, C.D.; O, X.; Hearn, T.P. Is the state-trait anxiety inventory multidimensional?. Pergamon; 1980; 1, pp. 207-214. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(80)90052-5]
36. Kuller, R. Environmental assessment from a neuropsychological perspective. Environment, Cognition and Action: An Integrated Approach; Gärling, T.; Evans, G.W. Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1991; pp. 111-147.
37. Xu, X.L.; Zhu, Z.P.; Chen, Z.R.; Zheng, X.; Dong, J.W. Comprehensive evaluation of urban forest trail—Taking Fudao as an example. J. Northwest Norm. Univ. Sci.; 2018; 54, pp. 109-115.
38. Yuebin, L.; Jian, L.; Kunyong, Y.; Yan, K. Research on Perceived Evaluation for Landscape Environment of Treetop Walk—A Case Study of “Fu Forest Trail” in Fuzhou. Chinese Landsc. Archit.; 2019; 35, pp. 72-77.
39. Paul, H.G.; Lynne, M.W. The human dimensions of urban greenways: Planning for recreation and related experiences. Landsc. Urban Plan.; 2004; 68, pp. 147-165.
40. Brownlow, A. An archaeology of fear and environmental change in Philadelphia. Geoforum; 2006; 37, pp. 227-245. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.geoforum.2005.02.009]
41. Castonguay, G.; Jutras, S. Children’s appreciation of outdoor places in a poor neighborhood. J. Environ. Psychol.; 2009; 29, pp. 101-109. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.05.002]
42. Lisa, J.J.; Gary, D.E.; Edward, R. Fear perceptions in public parks: Interactions of environmental concealment, the presence of people recreating, and gender. Environ. Behav.; 2013; 45, pp. 803-820.
43. Thomas, R.H. Preference and Perceived Danger in Field/Forest Settings. Environ. Behav.; 2002; 34, 819.
44. Patrick, M.E.; Joshua, J.C.; Edward, R.H. Revisiting the restorative effects of positive mood: An expectancy-based approach to self-control restoration. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.; 2015; 57, pp. 87-99.
45. Alfons, O.H. Fear, anxiety, and their disorders from the perspective of psychophysiology. Psychophysiology; 2020; 57, e13474.
46. Ulrich, R.S. Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment. Behavior and the Natural Environment; Altman, I.; Wohlwill, J.F. Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1983; Volume 6, pp. 85-125.
47. Constantinos, K.; Georgia, K.; Fotios, A. Attachment Anxiety, Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction and Depressive Symptoms in University Students: A Mediation Analysis Approach. Int. J. Psychol. Stud.; 2014; 6, pp. 1-10.
48. Joanne, D. Refining The Association Between Excessive Reassurance Seeking And Depressive Symptoms: The Role Of Related Interpersonal Constructs. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol.; 2001; 20, pp. 538-559.
49. Pittig, A.; Scherbaum, S. Costly avoidance in anxious individuals: Elevated threat avoidance in anxious individuals under high, but not low competing rewards. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry; 2020; 66, 101524. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.101524] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31707292]
50. Van den Berg, A.E.; Konijnendijk, C.C. Ambivalence towards nature and natural landscapes. Environmental Psychology: An Introduction; 2nd ed. John Wiley & Sons Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012; pp. 67-76.
51. Weike, X.; Eric, E.; Keith, L.; Bram, V.; Simon, D. Living in fear: Low-cost avoidance maintains low-level threat. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry; 2018; 62, pp. 57-64.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
The service quality and safety perception of urban forests are important factors that influence tourists to choose them as recreation destinations. This study aims to propose a theoretical model of multivariate relationships to explore the relationship between service quality (including visual quality, facility completeness, and accessibility) and safety perception to examine whether visual quality, facility completeness, and accessibility on tourists’ safety perception in the urban forest and to explain the specific reasons for the impact. We collected sample data from many urban forest green spaces in Fuzhou through a two-stage field survey (N = 891), and controlling for potential confounders, a structural equation model was used to estimate relationships. Safety perception was divided into safety environment perception, control perception, and safety emotion. Visual quality of an urban forest positively affected safety emotion. Traffic accessibility positively affected control perception. Facility completeness had a positive impact on safety emotion and control perception. Both safety emotion and control perception played an important intermediary role in improving the perception of a safe environment in the multivariate model. Visual quality, facility completeness, and accessibility all had a positive impact on tourists’ safety perception of urban forests. The findings suggest that improving the service quality of a green space can effectively improve tourists’ evaluation of the safety of the urban forest environment. Specifically, tourists’ psychological tolerance to threats and their self-confidence in survival can be enhanced by improving the service quality of a green space.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer