This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
1. Introduction
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a complicated mental illness [1]. PTSD individuals often encounter a variety of psychiatric symptoms that include feeling fear, helplessness, horror, increased arousal, autonomic hyperarousal activities, fear sensitization, serious startle responses, hypervigilance, insomnia, irritability, and impaired concentration [2–4]. Concerning the numerous symptoms of PTSD, the fear symptom through classical conditioning to form fear memory in the animal model of PTSD effectively simulated the PTSD’s human symptoms [5]. Moreover, PTSD’s fear symptom is crucial to the etiology of PTSD, and it is highly preserved in the process of evolution cross-species [5].
In light of the previous studies, environmental enrichment (EE) exposures can effectively reduce the PTSD symptoms in the animal model [6–11]. For example, EE reduced anxiety due to differentially activating the serotonin system and neuropeptide system in the PTSD animal model [11]. Chronic EE exposures reduced neonatal isolation-induced contextual freezing; however, chronic EE exposures did not affect single prolonged stress-related to anxiety behaviors or analgesia [9]. Acute EE exposures facilitated fear extinction through neuropeptide Y-Y1 receptor modulation in the hippocampus [10]. EE enhanced anxiolytic effect induced by deep-brain stimulation in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in the PTSD animal model [6]. Traumatic stress caused the reduction in the volume of the hippocampus and central amygdala, and EE was able to ameliorate trauma-induced volume decreases of the hippocampus resulted in behavioral, morphological, and molecular changes [8]. EE facilitated the negative feedback of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal gland (HPA) axis-related stress system and then improved abnormal behaviors through decreasing the glucocorticoid receptors in the hypothalamus and hippocampus [7]. Therefore, EE is a nonpharmacological treatment in reducing PTSD symptoms at behavioral, neuronal, and molecular levels.
The EE manipulation combined exposure of sensory, physical, cognitive, and social stimuli in the contextual surroundings for individuals, and the EE simulated a contextual environment. Animals exposed the entire components of sensory, physical, social, and cognitive stimuli [12–14]. In contrast to the EE stimuli, the cue is a single component or stimulus of the contextual stimuli [15]. Based on the previous evidence, the different category of fear conditioning is controlled by the different neural substrates: the hippocampus is contributable to the context-induced fear conditioning [15, 16], whereas the amygdala modulates the cue-induced fear conditioning [17, 18]. The hippocampus plays a function in configural memory and the encoding process of contextual stimulus [19]. The amygdala is essential to regulate the negative valence of emotion and negative emotional processes [20]. Recently, accumulated studies have clarified the roles of the mPFC, amygdala, and hippocampus in the fear conditioning of the PTSD symptoms after encountering a traumatic stress event [21–24]. The individual who experienced traumatic events was revealed to decrease the volumes of the hippocampus and anterior cingulate cortex; however, it increased the hyperactivity of the amygdala; moreover, the mPFC showed dysfunction that cannot inhibit the activity of the amygdala [21]. The hippocampus contributed to spatial learning and declarative memory; moreover, the hippocampus was projected from the amygdala, and thereby, the hippocampus-related functions, including spatial learning and memory, were regulated by the amygdala’s activity [25]. Alternatively, little research has demonstrated that the nucleus accumbens (NAc) might be involved in the PTSD symptoms associated with stress behaviors [26, 27]. Therefore, the study examined whether the mPFC, amygdala, hippocampus, and NAc mediated fear behavior of PTSD symptoms.
Altogether, some critical issues should be concerned: (a) Whether EE decreased fear symptoms of PTSD. (b) Whether a stimulus cue can reduce PTSD fear symptoms. (c) Using the c-Fos labeling and the assessment of c-Fos mRNA expression with the qRT-PCR methods, whether the subareas of mPFC (e.g., the Cg1, PrL, and IL), the subregions of the hippocampus (e.g., the CA1, CA3, and DG), the NAc, and BLA were involved in the amelioration of EE, cue, or the EE and cue combination in footshock-induced fear behavior of the PTSD symptom.
2. Methods and Materials
2.1. Animals
Fifty-six male wild-type C57BL/6 mice (approximate 25~35 g at the beginning of the experiments) were bought from National Laboratory for Animal Breeding and Research Center, Taipei, Taiwan. All mice were randomly assigned into no environmental enrichment (no EE; i.e., standard housing) and EE cages. The mice of no EE and EE groups were raised with another two mice in a colony room with constant temperature (approximately
2.2. Apparatus
2.2.1. Inescapable Footshock
The inescapable footshock apparatus is a box composed of a plastic surrounding shell measuring
2.3. Behavioral Procedure
All mice received an adaptation regimen for ad libitum food and water for seven days in the adaptation phase. After that, all mice were dependent on their assigned groups, and they were, respectively, raised in a mixed environment, including any combinations of no EE, EE, no cue, and cue until the end of the experiment (day 0-day 20). On day 17, all mice were given a single footshock (2 mA, 10 seconds) in the footshock apparatus. On days 18-20, all mice received the situational reminder procedure. Note, the current rate of footshock (2 mA, 10 seconds) and three freezing-times in situational reminders were determined by our previous studies [22, 29]. The mice were placed in the footshock apparatus for 2 min without footshock. One hundred twenty minutes after completing the last behavioral test, the immunohistochemical staining and the qRT-PCR method were, respectively, performed to measure c-Fos protein and c-Fos mRNA expression in the selected brain areas (see Figure 1).
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]
All of the mice were assigned into EE or cue environments into four groups: no EE/no Cue, no EE/cue, EE/no cue, and EE/cue groups (
2.4. Immunohistochemical Staining
Mice were sacrificed by sodium pentobarbital overdose after completing behavioral tests for 120 min (i.e., labeling IHC c-Fos for the best expression). When completely unresponsive, mice were perfused with 0.9% NaCl followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M sodium phosphate-buffered saline (PBS). The brain tissues were dissected, blocked, postfixed for 3 days, and transferred to 30% sucrose for cryoprotection for 2 days until the specimens sink to the bottom of the solution. Forty-micron coronal sections were cut on freezing using a sliding microtome. All brain slices were processed by c-Fos immunoreactivity staining. Free-floating brain slices were washed once for 10 mins in 0.1 M PBS, permeabilized in 3% H2O2 for 1 hour, washed three times in PBS for 10mins, and then soaked in normal goat serum for 1 hour. After washing with PBS once for 10 mins, the slices were incubated overnight with the specific first antibody (i.e., sheep anti-c-Fos primary antibody at a dilution of 1 : 500). This antibody was raised against a peptide sequence to enable crossreaction with other proteins for detecting c-Fos immunoreactivity. The slices were then washed once with PBS for 10 mins, and then, these slices were incubated in a second antibody (i.e., biotinylated anti-sheep secondary antibody at a dilution of 1 : 500) for 2 hours. Ten minutes with PBS washing later, the bound secondary antibody was amplified using the Vector Elite ABC kit.
The positive expression nucleus of neurons was quantified for the selected brain areas. Counting was performed visually at ×20 magnification by a researcher blinded to the condition of each mouse. Every third slice of the brain tissue was selected into an available counted section. The software ImageJ was used to count the amounts of c-Fos-positive neurons [31].
2.5. Real-Time Quantitative PCR of c-Fos
Based on the manufacturer’s instructions, total RNA was extracted with TRIzol Reagent (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA). For cDNA synthesis, total RNA was applied with random hexamers. Later, the reverse-transcription PCR was the amplification of c-fos. For the amplification, it was initiated with a pair of c-fos primers (forward: 5
2.6. Statistical Analysis
A three-way mixed (environmental enrichment vs. cue vs. session) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed for the freezing time. One-way ANOVA was conducted for the total freezing time in the main effect of environmental enrichment, cue, and situational reminders. Furthermore, two-way mixed (group vs. session) ANOVA was performed for the freezing time over sessions 1-3 in situational reminders and total freezing time among the no EE/no cue, no EE/cue, EE/no cue, and EE/Cue groups. When appropriate, the post hoc with Tukey’s Honest Significant Difference (HSD) test was performed.
Regarding the examination of the c-Fos protein expression and c-Fos mRNA expression, two-way ANOVA analysis was conducted, and one-way ANOVA analysis was performed. For c-Fos protein expression analysis, the selected brain areas were determined, including Cg1, PrL, IL, NAc, BLA, CA1, CA2, and DG. For c-Fos mRNA expression analysis, the mPFC, NAc, amygdala, and hippocampus were determined. When appropriate, the post hoc with Tukey’s HSD test was performed.
3. Results
3.1. PTSD Behavioral Tests
To examine the effect of environmental enrichment and cue in freezing behavior of the PTSD animal model, a
Table 1
Mean (± SEM) freezing time (sec.) was analyzed by a
Environmental enrichment | |
Cue | |
Session | F2,40 =27.95, |
Environmental enrichment × cue | |
Environmental enrichment × session | |
Cue × session | |
Environmental enrichment × cue × session |
Alternatively, a
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]
To analyze mean (± SEM) total freezing time that was merged with the behavioral data overall sessions 1-3, two-way ANOVA was conducted to show that significant differences occurred in the factor of EE (
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]
In the NAc c-Fos expression measurements, a significant difference occurred in the factor of group (
[figures omitted; refer to PDF]
In the BLA c-Fos expression measurements, a significant difference occurred in the factor of the group (
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]
In the CA1 c-Fos expression assessments, a significant difference occurred in the factor of group (
[figures omitted; refer to PDF]
In the CA3 c-Fos expression assessments, a significant difference occurred in the factor of group (
In the DG c-Fos expression assessments, a significant difference occurred in the factor of group (
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]
Therefore, the Cg1, IL, and NAc were upregulated in the c-Fos expression, but the BLA was a downregulation in the c-Fos expression through the EE manipulations. The Cg1, PrL, IL, CA1, CA3, and DG were a downregulation in the c-Fos expression via the cue manipulations. Notably, the Cg1 and NAc were significant differences in the c-Fos expression under the interaction between EE and cue.
3.3. qRT-PCR Assessment for the c-Fos mRNA Expression
To test the c-Fos mRNA expression of EE, cue, and EE×cue in the mPFC, NAc, amygdala, and hippocampus, a
Table 3
Effects of environmental enrichment and cue for PTSD in the normalized c-Fos mRNA expression for selected brain areas in the situational reminder phase.
mPFC | NAc | Amygdala | Hippocampus | |
Environmental enrichment | ↑ | ↑ | ↓ | — |
Cue | ↓ | — | — | ↓ |
Interaction of Cue and environmental enrichment | — | — | — | — |
Note: (↑): increases; (↓): decreases; (-): nonsignificant difference; (+):
Furthermore, one-way ANOVA analysis was conducted to analyze the c-Fos mRNA expression in the mPFC, NAc, amygdala, and hippocampus among the no EE/no cue, no EE/cue, EE/no cue, and EE/cue groups. For the mPFC, the factor of group was significant differences (
[figures omitted; refer to PDF]
To test the c-Fos mRNA expression of the NAc, one-way ANOVA analysis showed that significant differences occurred in the factor of the group (
One-way ANOVA was performed to test the amygdala’s c-Fos mRNA expression in the EE and cue manipulations. The results showed that the factor of group had significant differences (
To test c-Fos mRNA of the hippocampus in the EE and cue manipulations, one-way ANOVA analysis was conducted to show that only the EE/cue group was significant decreases in the c-Fos mRNA expression compared to the no EE/no cue and the EE/no cue groups (
In conclusion, the results of the c-Fos mRNA expression were similar to the findings of the c-Fos protein expression in the mPFC, NAc, amygdala, and the hippocampus (see Tables 2 and 3).
4. Discussion
The EE manipulations can reduce footshock-induced fear behavior, and the cue manipulation also reduces fear behavior in PTSD symptoms. The present data showed that the EE is better than the cue manipulation to ameliorate fear behavior in PTSD. The combination of EE and cue induced the most significant reduction effect in fear behavior. Cue manipulation is likely a novel treatment to reduce PTSD symptoms, in particular, to fear behavior.
The c-Fos expression data showed that the Cg1, IL, and NAc upregulated the c-Fos expression in fear behavior; however, the BLA downregulated the c-Fos expression in fear behavior for PTSD symptoms through the EE manipulations, indicating the Cg1, IL, NAc, and BLA controlled the EE-induced fear reduction. In contrast, the Cg1, PrL, IL, CA1, CA3, and DG were downregulated in the c-Fos expression after the cue manipulations in fear behavior, indicating the Cg1, PrL, IL, CA1, CA3, and DG contributed to a cue-induced fear reduction in PTSD. The EE and cue had significant interactions in the Cg1 and NAc for the c-Fos expression. Accordingly, the Cg1 and NAc were involved in the interaction of EE and cue.
The data of the c-Fos mRNA expression were similar to the findings of the c-Fos protein expression. EE manipulations upregulated the c-Fos mRNA expression in the mPFC and the NAc; however, it downregulated the c-Fos mRNA expression in the amygdala. Cue manipulations downregulated the c-Fos mRNA expression in the mPFC and hippocampus. EE and cue interactions did not affect c-Fos mRNA expression in the mPFC, NAc, amygdala, and hippocampus.
4.1. Environmental Enrichment, Cue, and Combination of Environmental Enrichment and Cue to Ameliorate Footshock-Induced Fear Behavior in PTSD Symptoms
In the present data, the EE manipulations ameliorated footshock-induced fear behavior. Therefore, the present data of EE is consistent with the previous findings [33]. Dependent on the previous data, a growing body of evidence showed that the EE manipulations decreased fear- and stress-related behaviors in different levels [6–11, 34]. For example, the behavioral test of EE showed that long-term EE suppressed neonatal isolation-induced contextual freezing, but this type of EE did not change single prolonged stress related to anxiety behavior or analgesia. Moreover, EE exposures showed to reduce deep-brain stimulation-induced resistant depression and PTSD in the animal model [6]. Alternatively, the examinations of the brain system in EE ameliorations showed that EE could facilitate the negative feedback regulation of the HPA axis for stress rats, and it ameliorated the abnormal behaviors via downregulation of glucocorticoid receptor expression in the hippocampus and hypothalamus [7]. In another study, activations of serotonin and neuropeptide systems through EE manipulations were shown to reduce anxiety behaviors in the PTSD animal model [11]. Regarding the examination of EE manipulations in neuronal levels, animals with EE exposures increased hippocampal cell proliferation and recovered normal behaviors in PTSD symptoms [34]. Additionally, short-term EE exposures potentiated the extinction process of fear via regulating neuropeptide Y-Y1 receptors in the hippocampus [10]. A previous study has demonstrated that EE exposures ameliorated the extended volumes of the hippocampus and central amygdala caused by traumatic stress [8]. Therefore, the EE manipulations may be effectively nonpharmacological interventions in reducing footshock-induced stress or anxiety in behavior, brain systems, and neuronal levels.
On the other hand, manipulating the cue stimulus is likely a novel approach to examine the amelioration of footshock-induced fear behavior. The present results showed that the cue exposure showed a significant decrease in fear behavior induced by footshock; however, this suppressed effect by the cue stimulus was not higher than that of the EE manipulation. The single cue stimulus may cause the different effects between EE and cue, but the EE is composed of a variety of stimuli, including sensory, physical, social, and cognitive components [12, 13]. Therefore, the EE manipulation is similar to the real contextual stimulus.
4.2. Cue and Environmental Enrichment: An Occasion Setter or a CS Associated with the US?
These inconsistent data for the EE and cue manipulations should be explained by that the novelty and salient property of stimulus from the EE exposures might be the crucial factor to ameliorate footshock-induced fear behavior [35, 36]. However, the cue stimulus is short of comprehensive and complete stimuli; instead, the cue exposure only has a single stimulus dimension, causing a weaker novelty and salient property to reduce footshock-induced fear behavior. Interestingly, the combination of EE and cue revealed the highest reduction in footshock-induced fear behavior. What reason is the combination of EE and cue due to the highest reduction for footshock-induced fear behavior? It might be two possibilities. One possible reason is that the cue stimulus is served as a conditioned stimulus (CS) to interact with the unconditioned stimulus (US) to modulate the strengths of the conditioned response (CR) by the rule of classical conditioning [37]. Another possible reason is that the cue stimulus is an occasion setter to enhance the CR, fear behavior. Based on the occasion setting theory, the cue stimulus itself was not involved in the association with the US; however, the occasion setter stimulus only modulates the strength of the CR [38, 39]. According to the present study, the cue stimulus seemingly contributed to the association with the US. Therefore, whether the cue stimulus is an occasion setter or a CS remains scrutinized in further studies.
4.3. Involvements of the mPFC, Amygdala, Hippocampus, and NAc in Environmental Enrichment, Cue, or the Combination of Environmental Enrichment and Cue in PTSD Fear Symptoms
To date, no research comprehensively examined how the mPFC, amygdala, and hippocampus contributed to suppressing EE or cue in PTSD symptoms and behaviors. However, amount of research has demonstrated that the mPFC’s neuronal activity inhibited the amygdala’s negative emotional responses, and the activation of the amygdala conveyed negative feedback information with the negative value to transmit into the mPFC, which explained the valences of this information [40]. This neural pathway of the mPFC projecting to the amygdala has been shown to regulate PTSD symptoms [41, 42]; moreover, the present data showed that the mPFC-amygdala pathway was seemingly involved in the suppression of EE or cue to PTSD symptoms. On the other hand, the hippocampus was suggested to modulate spatial learning and configural memory [19, 43], and the hippocampus is essential to process memory contents in the process of consolidation from the short-term memory to the long-term memory [44, 45]. Accordingly, the hippocampus was acquired by the amygdala’s information to modulate the formation of the configural memory and spatial learning. This result is likely consistent with the recent findings that the amygdala interacted with the hippocampus to mediate emotional learning and memory and PTSD symptoms [46]. The amygdala’s pathway with the hippocampus might be another crucial neural pathway for PTSD symptoms.
In the reduced effect of the EE in PTSD fear behavior, it was shown that the Cg1 and IL (but not the PrL) of the mPFC were increased in the c-Fos expression, and it indicated that after chronic EE exposures, the mPFC’s executive and inhibitory functions interfered with the activity of the amygdala’s BLA, which presented decreases in the c-Fos expression. Decreased c-Fos expression of the BLA indicated that the BLA inhibited the negative emotional responses following the EE exposures.
Alternatively, the present data showed that the c-Fos expression of the NAc significantly increased, indicating that EE exposures might induce dopamine neurotransmitters secretions in the NAc due to the novelty or saliency effects following EE exposures [36, 47, 48]. This novelty and saliency of EE’s property may be the critical point that the EE manipulations could reduce the symptoms of psychiatric disorders (e.g., PTSD and depression) or neurological diseases (Parkinson’s disease or Alzheimer’s disease) [35]. How did the novelty and saliency of EE affect the symptoms of psychiatric and neurological diseases which remain to be investigated further?
In particular, the cue stimulus manipulations showed different results that the Cg1, PrL, and IL of the mPFC and the CA1, CA3, and DG of the hippocampus decreased the c-Fos expression compared to the EE manipulations. Accordingly, the cue manipulations may play a different role in the reduction of footshock-induced fear behavior when compared to the EE manipulations. The cue stimulus is not contextual, and it is likely an occasion setting to modulate fear behavior induced by footshock in the PTSD animal model [39, 49]. Thus, the occasion setter role of the cue fully decreased the c-Fos expression in the whole subregions of the mPFC and the hippocampus. The results indicated that the cue stimulus interfered with the executive and inhibitory functions resulted in the mPFC dysfunctions; moreover, the cue stimulus decreased the c-Fos expression in the CA1, CA3, and DG of the hippocampus, indicating the hippocampus revealed dysfunctions in spatial learning and configural memory. Accordingly, the c-Fos data of the cue stimulus manipulations revealed a worse reduction to footshock-induced fear behavior than those of the EE manipulation and the combination of the EE and cue manipulations.
In the examinations of the interaction between EE and cue, the results showed that significant differences occurred in the Cg1 of the mPFC and the NAc to reduce footshock-induced fear behavior. Why did the c-Fos expression of the Cg1 and NAc produce an interaction between the EE and cue manipulations? It is due to the property of the Cg1 governing the stress or anxiety behaviors and the property of the NAc involving the reward or reinforcement process. This emerged issue should be concerned with further studies.
5. Conclusion
The EE and cue suppressed footshock-induced fear behavior; however, the EE manipulation produced a more potent suppression in footshock-induced fear behavior compared to the cue manipulation; although, the cue stimulus also reduced footshock-induced fear behavior. These data supported the previous findings [6, 8, 11, 35, 36]. For example, EE exposures reduced traumatic stress and increased the volume of the hippocampus and central amygdala [8]; moreover, it ameliorated anxiety [11], depression [6], PTSD symptoms [6], and footshock-induced fear behaviors [35, 36] in the animal model. Notably, the combination of EE and cue manipulations produced the strongest reduction in footshock-induced fear behavior. Under the EE manipulations, c-Fos upregulation occurred in the Cg1, IL, and NAc, but c-Fos downregulation occurred in the BLA. Under cue manipulations, c-Fos downregulation occurred in the Cg1, PrL, IL, CA1, CA3, and DG. The data of the c-Fos mRNA expression were similar with the data of the c-Fos protein expression. The c-Fos mRNA expression was upregulation in the mPFC and amygdala in the EE manipulations; the amygdala was downregulated for the c-Fos mRNA expression in the EE manipulations. Cue manipulations were decreases in the c-Fos mRNA expression for the mPFC and hippocampus. The present findings are likely to offer contributions for novel and nonpharmacological treatments to PTSD symptoms. The present data might help understand the amelioration mechanisms of PTSD fear symptoms in the brain.
Authors’ Contributions
YHY contributed to the conceptualization, methodology, validation, investigation, data curation, and project administration. YSL contributed to the project administration. CYO contributed to the project administration. KCC contributed to the data curation. FCC contributed to the project administration. ACT contributed to the statistic analysis supports. ACWH contributed to the conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, investigation, writing-review and editing, and funding acquisition.
Acknowledgments
The present study was supported by funding granted from the Ministry of Science and Technology, Taiwan to Dr. Andrew Chih Wei Huang (MOST 110-2410-H-431-004) and Dr. Arthur C. Tasi (MOST 107-2118-M-001-006-MY2).
[1] H. Cohen, N. Kozlovsky, C. Alona, M. A. Matar, Z. Joseph, "Animal model for PTSD: from clinical concept to translational research," Neuropharmacology, vol. 62, pp. 715-724, 2012.
[2] C. Grillon, S. M. Southwick, D. S. Charney, "The psychobiological basis of posttraumatic stress disorder," Molecular Psychiatry, vol. 1, pp. 278-297, 1996.
[3] D. Kozaric-Kovacic, "Psychopharmacotherapy of posttraumatic stress disorder," Croatian Medical Journal, vol. 49, pp. 459-475, 2008.
[4] A. Siegmund, C. T. Wotjak, "Hyperarousal does not depend on trauma-related contextual memory in an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder," Physiology & Behavior, vol. 90, pp. 103-107, 2007.
[5] E. I. Flandreau, M. Toth, "Animal models of PTSD: a critical review," Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, vol. 38, pp. 47-68, DOI: 10.1007/7854_2016_65, 2018.
[6] Y. Bhaskar, L. W. Lim, R. Mitra, "Enriched environment facilitates anxiolytic efficacy driven by deep-brain stimulation of medial prefrontal cortex," Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 12, 2018.
[7] Z. Fan, J. Chen, L. Li, H. Wang, X. Gong, H. Xu, L. Wu, C. Yan, "Environmental enrichment modulates HPA axis reprogramming in adult male rats exposed to early adolescent stress," Neuroscience Research, vol. 172, pp. 63-72, DOI: 10.1016/j.neures.2021.04.007, 2021.
[8] Y. Golub, S. F. Kaltwasser, C. P. Mauch, L. Herrmann, U. Schmidt, F. Holsboer, M. Czisch, C. T. Wotjak, "Reduced hippocampus volume in the mouse model of posttraumatic stress disorder," Journal of Psychiatric Research, vol. 45, pp. 650-659, DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.10.014, 2011.
[9] A. Imanaka, S. Morinobu, S. Toki, S. Yamawaki, "Importance of early environment in the development of post-traumatic stress disorder-like behaviors," Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 173, pp. 129-137, 2006.
[10] G. Lach, M. A. Bicca, A. A. Hoeller, E. C. Santos, A. P. Costa, T. C. de Lima, "Short-term enriched environment exposure facilitates fear extinction in adult rats: the NPY-Y1 receptor modulation," Neuropeptides, vol. 55, pp. 73-78, DOI: 10.1016/j.npep.2015.10.001, 2016.
[11] D. Ragu Varman, K. E. Rajan, "Environmental enrichment reduces anxiety by differentially activating serotonergic and neuropeptide Y (NPY)-ergic system in Indian field mouse (Mus booduga): an animal model of post-traumatic stress disorder," PLoS One, vol. 10, article e0127945, 2015.
[12] J. C. Brenes, M. Lackinger, G. U. Hoglinger, G. Schratt, R. K. Schwarting, M. Wohr, "Differential effects of social and physical environmental enrichment on brain plasticity, cognition, and ultrasonic communication in rats," The Journal of Comparative Neurology, vol. 524 no. 8, pp. 1586-1607, DOI: 10.1002/cne.23842, 2016.
[13] J. Nithianantharajah, A. J. Hannan, "Enriched environments, experience-dependent plasticity and disorders of the nervous system," Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, vol. 7, pp. 697-709, 2006.
[14] P. Sampedro-Piquero, A. Begega, "Environmental enrichment as a positive behavioral intervention across the lifespan," Current Neuropharmacology, vol. 15, pp. 459-470, 2017.
[15] D. T. Acheson, J. E. Gresack, V. B. Risbrough, "Hippocampal dysfunction effects on context memory: possible etiology for posttraumatic stress disorder," Neuropharmacology, vol. 62, pp. 674-685, 2012.
[16] N. Kaouane, E. G. Ducourneau, A. Marighetto, M. Segal, A. Desmedt, "False opposing fear memories are produced as a function of the hippocampal sector where glucocorticoid receptors are activated," Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 14, 2020.
[17] F. Steiger, F. Nees, M. Wicking, S. Lang, H. Flor, "Behavioral and central correlates of contextual fear learning and contextual modulation of cued fear in posttraumatic stress disorder," International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol. 98, pp. 584-593, 2015.
[18] N. C. Huff, J. W. Rudy, "The amygdala modulates hippocampus-dependent context memory formation and stores cue-shock associations," Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 118, pp. 53-62, 2004.
[19] L. E. Jarrard, "On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat," Behavioral and Neural Biology, vol. 60, 1993.
[20] S. Tanimoto, T. Nakagawa, Y. Yamauchi, M. Minami, M. Satoh, "Differential contributions of the basolateral and central nuclei of the amygdala in the negative affective component of chemical somatic and visceral pains in rats," The European Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 18, pp. 2343-2350, 2003.
[21] J. D. Bremner, "Traumatic stress: effects on the brain," Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, vol. 8 no. 4, pp. 445-461, DOI: 10.31887/DCNS.2006.8.4/jbremner, 2006.
[22] S. H. Chang, Y. H. Yu, A. He, C. Y. Ou, B. C. Shyu, A. C. W. Huang, "BDNF protein and BDNF mRNA expression of the medial prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus during situational reminder in the PTSD animal model," Behavioural Neurology, vol. 2021,DOI: 10.1155/2021/6657716, 2021.
[23] B. M. Elzinga, J. D. Bremner, "Are the neural substrates of memory the final common pathway in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)?," Journal of Affective Disorders, vol. 70, 2002.
[24] L. M. Shin, S. L. Rauch, R. K. Pitman, "Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampal function in PTSD," Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 1071, pp. 67-79, 2006.
[25] M. M. Miller, B. S. McEwen, "Establishing an agenda for translational research on PTSD," Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 1071 no. 1, pp. 294-312, DOI: 10.1196/annals.1364.023, 2006.
[26] N. Korem, R. Lange, C. J. Hillard, I. Akirav, "Role of beta-catenin and endocannabinoids in the nucleus accumbens in extinction in rats exposed to shock and reminders," Neuroscience, vol. 357, pp. 285-294, 2017.
[27] C. Piao, X. Deng, X. Wang, Y. Yuan, Z. Liu, J. Liang, "Altered function in medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens links to stress-induced behavioral inflexibility," Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 317, pp. 16-26, 2017.
[28] J. C. Kung, T. C. Chen, B. C. Shyu, S. Hsiao, A. C. Huang, "Anxiety- and depressive-like responses and c-fos activity in preproenkephalin knockout mice: oversensitivity hypothesis of enkephalin deficit-induced posttraumatic stress disorder," Journal of Biomedical Science, vol. 17 no. 1,DOI: 10.1186/1423-0127-17-29, 2010.
[29] Y. H. Yu, C. Y. Ou, B. C. Shyu, A. C. W. Huang, "Basolateral amygdala but not medial prefrontal cortex contributes to chronic fluoxetine treatments for PTSD symptoms in mice," Behavioural Neurology, vol. 2020,DOI: 10.1155/2020/8875087, 2020.
[30] T. Takahashi, K. Shimizu, K. Shimazaki, H. Toda, M. Nibuya, "Environmental enrichment enhances autophagy signaling in the rat hippocampus," Brain Research, vol. 1592, pp. 113-123, 2014.
[31] C. A. Schneider, W. S. Rasband, K. W. Eliceiri, "NIH image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis," Nature Methods, vol. 9, pp. 671-675, 2012.
[32] T. D. Schmittgen, "Real-time quantitative PCR," Methods, vol. 25, pp. 383-385, 2001.
[33] H. Hendriksen, B. Olivier, R. S. Oosting, "From non-pharmacological treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder to novel therapeutic targets," European Journal of Pharmacology, vol. 732, pp. 139-158, 2014.
[34] H. Hendriksen, J. Prins, B. Olivier, R. S. Oosting, "Environmental enrichment induces behavioral recovery and enhanced hippocampal cell proliferation in an antidepressant-resistant animal model for PTSD," PLoS One, vol. 5, article e11943, 2010.
[35] A. J. Hannan, "Environmental enrichment and brain repair: harnessing the therapeutic effects of cognitive stimulation and physical activity to enhance experience-dependent plasticity," Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology, vol. 40 no. 1, pp. 13-25, DOI: 10.1111/nan.12102, 2014.
[36] C. Girbovan, H. Plamondon, "Environmental enrichment in female rodents: considerations in the effects on behavior and biochemical markers," Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 253, pp. 178-190, 2013.
[37] I. P. Pavlov, Conditioned reflexes, 1927.
[38] K. M. Fraser, P. C. Holland, "Occasion setting," Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 133, pp. 145-175, 2019.
[39] N. A. Schmajuk, J. A. Lamoureux, P. C. Holland, "Occasion setting: a neural network approach," Psychological Review, vol. 105, 1998.
[40] M. J. Kim, R. A. Loucks, A. L. Palmer, A. C. Brown, K. M. Solomon, A. N. Marchante, P. J. Whalen, "The structural and functional connectivity of the amygdala: from normal emotion to pathological anxiety," Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 223, pp. 403-410, 2011.
[41] T. F. Giustino, S. Maren, "The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the conditioning and extinction of fear," Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 9,DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00298, 2015.
[42] M. Koenigs, J. Grafman, "Posttraumatic stress disorder: the role of medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala," The Neuroscientist, vol. 15, pp. 540-548, 2009.
[43] J. W. Rudy, R. J. Sutherland, "Configural association theory and the hippocampal formation: an appraisal and reconfiguration," Hippocampus, vol. 5, pp. 375-389, 1995.
[44] I. Izquierdo, C. R. Furini, J. C. Myskiw, "Fear memory," Physiological Reviews, vol. 96 no. 2, pp. 695-750, DOI: 10.1152/physrev.00018.2015, 2016.
[45] S. Languille, S. Davis, P. Richer, C. Alcacer, S. Laroche, B. Hars, "Extracellular signal-regulated kinase activation is required for consolidation and reconsolidation of memory at an early stage of ontogenesis," The European Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 30 no. 10, pp. 1923-1930, DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06971.x, 2009.
[46] A. J. McDonald, D. D. Mott, "Functional neuroanatomy of amygdalohippocampal interconnections and their role in learning and memory," Journal of Neuroscience Research, vol. 95, pp. 797-820, 2017.
[47] A. Badiani, M. M. Oates, H. E. W. Day, S. J. Watson, H. Akil, T. E. Robinson, "Amphetamine-induced behavior, dopamine release, and c-fos mRNA expression: modulation by environmental novelty," The Journal of Neuroscienc., vol. 18 no. 24, pp. 10579-10593, DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-24-10579.1998, 1998.
[48] A. Badiani, M. M. Oates, H. E. W. Day, S. J. Watson, H. Akil, T. E. Robinson, "Environmental modulation of amphetamine-induced c-fos expression in D1 versus D2 striatal neurons," Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 103, pp. 203-209, 1999.
[49] P. Holland, "Ocassion setting in Pavlovian conditioning," The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, pp. 69-125, 1992.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright © 2022 Ying Hao Yu et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Abstract
A growing body of evidence showed that environmental enrichment (EE) ameliorated footshock-induced fear behavior of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). However, no research comprehensively tested the effect of EE, cue, and the combination of EE and cue in footshock-induced fear behavior of PTSD symptoms. The present study addressed this issue and examined whether the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC, including the cingulate cortex 1 (Cg1), prelimbic cortex (PrL), and infralimbic cortex (IL)), the nucleus accumbens (NAc), the basolateral amygdala (BLA), and the hippocampus (e.g., CA1, CA3, and dentate gyrus (DG)) regulated the amelioration of the EE, cue, or the combination of EE and cue. The results showed that EE or cue could reduce fear behavior. The combination of EE and cue revealed a stronger decrease in fear behavior. The cue stimulus may play an occasion setting or a conditioned stimulus to modulate the reduction in fear behavior induced by footshock. Regarding the reduction of the EE in fear behavior, the Cg1 and IL of the mPFC and the NAc upregulated the c-Fos expression; however, the BLA downregulated the c-Fos expression. The mPFC (i.e., the Cg1, PrL, and IL) and the hippocampus (i.e., the CA1, CA3, and DG) downregulated the c-Fos expression in the suppression of the cue in fear behavior. The interaction of EE and cue in reduction of fear behavior occurred in the Cg1 and NAc for the c-Fos expression. The data of c-Fos mRNA were similar to the findings of the c-Fos protein expression. These findings related to the EE and cue modulations in fear behavior may develop a novel nonpharmacological treatment in PTSD.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details

1 Department of Psychology, Fo Guang University, Yilan County 26247, Taiwan; Department of Biotechnology and Animal Science, National Ilan University, Yilan County 26247, Taiwan
2 Department of Psychology, Fo Guang University, Yilan County 26247, Taiwan
3 Institute of Statistical Science, Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan