Introduction
Animals experience stress – a real or perceived threat to homeostasis – throughout their lives. This stress has the potential to negatively impact their survival and reproduction (Chrousos, 2009). Stressors such as predator encounters, conflicts with social competitors, and harsh weather can threaten survival through a reduction in foraging success and a weakening of the immune system, which can potentially even result in death (Brown & Kotler, 2004; Lange & Leimar, 2004; Pravosudov et al., 2001; Cirule et al., 2012). The presence of stressors can also suppress reproduction and result in the abandonment of dependent offspring (Love et al., 2004). Because stress can significantly reduce fitness, mechanisms have evolved to physiologically and behaviorally cope with it.
When a stressor is perceived, it triggers the activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, beginning a hormonal cascade that ultimately releases glucocorticoids into the bloodstream (Sapolsky, Krey & McEwen, 1986). Elevated glucocorticoids, such as cortisol and corticosterone, disrupt normal functions and shifts the individual into an ‘emergency life history stage’ (Wingfield et al., 1998). This stage can temporarily suppress the immune system (Shini et al., 2010), mobilize energy stores, and modify behavior to prioritize survival. Behavioral modifications in this stage can include increasing anti-predator behaviors (Thaker, Lima & Hews, 2009), decreasing parental care, and increasing group coordination (Raulo & Dantzer, 2018).
Although physiological and behavioral stress responses are often beneficial during stress, they can become harmful when prolonged. This can occur when either the stressor is chronic or because the stress response persists after the stressor is gone. Prolonged activation of the stress response can cause physiological problems (e.g., metabolic dysfunction and impaired reproduction (Wingfield & Sapolsky, 2003; López et al., 2018)) as well as cognitive issues (e.g., impaired memory and decision making (Voellmy et al., 2014; Aisa et al., 2007)). The ability to efficiently extinguish the stress response is important for resilience against the negative physiological and behavioral effects of prolonged stress (Romero & Wikelski, 2010; Vitousek et al., 2019; Zimmer et al., 2019).
Growing evidence indicates that dopamine plays an important role in extinguishing the stress response (Cabib & Puglisi-Allegra, 2012; Sullivan & Dufresne, 2006). Dopamine concentration increased in the nucleus accumbens, striatum, and medial frontal cortex of rodents exposed to a physiological stressor (Abercrombie et al., 1989). Activation of one dopamine receptor in particular, the D2 receptor, has been shown to increase with stress and may mediate coping behaviors (Cabib & Puglisi-Allegra, 2012; Lattin et al., 2019), which are behavioral responses to adverse stimuli that reduce the harmful physiological effects of stress (Schouten & Wiepkema, 1991). Examples of coping behaviors in some mammals and birds include grooming, freezing, and biting at inanimate objects (Giorgi et al., 2003; Henson et al., 2012; Reis-Silva et al., 2019; Koolhaas et al., 1999; Hori, Yuyama & Tamura, 2004; Savory, Seawright & Watson, 1992). Coping behaviors in response to stress can be crucial to regulating arousal levels and maintaining homeostasis by regulating glucocorticoids following a disturbance such as a stressor. Some animals that exhibit coping behaviors have reduced glucocorticoids when they are exposed to a stressor (Sato et al., 2010; Kostal, Savory & Hughes, 1992). In addition to altering stress related hormones, coping behaviors also impact other markers of stress including reducing the occurrence of stomach lesions (Koolhaas et al., 1999). Some of these coping behaviors are dependent on dopamine in the mesostriatal dopamine region in the brain (Jones, Mittleman & Robbins, 1989) and blocking D2 dopamine receptors during stress results in elevated post-stress glucocorticoids and increased stress-related health problems (Sullivan & Dufresne, 2006; Puri et al., 1994; Sullivan & Szechtman, 1995).
Our current understanding of dopamine’s role in animals’ behavioral response to stress largely comes from studies on domesticated and laboratory animals (Baik, 2020). The effects of stress have been shown to vary between wild and domestic animals, even in the same species, therefore it may not be possible to generalize dopamine’s role in stress (Cabezas et al., 2013). There have so far been few studies investigating dopamine’s effect on behavioral responses to stress in wild species. A study in wild-caught house sparrows (Passer domesticus) (Lattin et al., 2019) however found that increasing D2 receptor activation increased preening, a possible stress-related coping behavior (Henson et al., 2012). House sparrows are invasive in many areas of the world and are urban specialists that experience a wide range of stressors as a result of their proximity to humans (Beaugeard et al., 2018). Understanding their stress responses may lead to better understanding of how animals cope with urban stressors as well as this species’ success in non-native urban environments.
The aim of this study was therefore to test the hypothesis that activation of the D2 dopamine receptor influences stress-related coping behaviors in a wild species. We tested this hypothesis using wild-caught house sparrows, an urban specialist songbird species that is a model system for studying bird behavior and physiology (Hanson et al., 2020). Similar to the impact of D2 activation on the coping behavior of laboratory and domesticated animals (Sullivan & Dufresne, 2006; Puri et al., 1994; Cheng, Singleton & Muir, 2003; Dennis, Muir & Cheng, 2006), we predicted that increasing D2 receptor activation would increase coping behavior and blocking D2 receptor activation would decrease coping behavior. To test these predictions, we peripherally administered selective D2 dopamine agonists and antagonists to manipulate the activation of dopamine receptors in the brains of house sparrows.
Materials and Methods Animals
Twenty adult male house sparrows were captured with baited traps between February and May of 2019 in College Station, TX. Due to logistical issues associated with catching female birds, we restricted this study to males only. Our sample size was similar to that used in a previous study (Balthazart, Castagna & Ball, 1997). The birds were housed in randomly-assigned pairs in cages (0.6 × 0.33 × 0.3 m) at Texas A&M University (30°36′N, 96°21′W) in an indoor room (‘housing room’: 5 × 6.3 m). They were kept on a 13 h:11 h light:dark cycle at 24.0 ± 0.5 °C (mean ± SD). They were individually marked with metal and colored leg bands. The pairs were given at least 7 days to acclimate to captivity and their cagemate before being tested. Water and food were available ad libitum. The study was approved by Texas A&M University’s Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC#2019-0219).
Experimental procedure
During each trial, a pair of birds (consisting of a focal bird and non-focal bird; the initial designation of birds as the focal or non-focal bird was randomly assigned within each pair) was transported within its cage from the ‘housing room’ to an adjacent room (2.2 × 2.6 m) that contained a sound attenuation chamber (1.12 × 0.67 × 0.57 m). The pair, within its cage, was placed within the middle of this chamber, which was visually and acoustically isolated from the other captive birds in the ‘housing room’. For 30 min, the birds’ behavior was recorded using two video cameras (VIXIA HF R70; 60 frames/s; Cannon Inc., Ota City, Tokyo, Japan) positioned on each side of the chamber. After the 30 min, the experimenter (MRF) briefly removed the focal bird from his cage and intramuscularly injected 0.05 mL of a drug treatment or control into his breast. An experimenter monitored the birds (through remote videos) throughout their time in the chamber to ensure there were no signs of pain or adverse reactions from injection. The drug treatment consisted of a D2 agonist (PPHT; 1 mg/kg; Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Dallas, TX, USA) or a D2 antagonist (raclopride; 10 mg/kg; Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Dallas, TX, USA); the control consisted of 0.9% saline. Both drugs were dissolved in 0.9% saline. The doses were chosen to be high enough to insure they produced a behavioral effect and were the same as those used in previous avian studies (Balthazart, Castagna & Ball, 1997; Zawilska, Derbiszewska & Nowak, 1996); similar to these previous studies, all of the birds also received the same amounts of the drug treatment or control (the doses were calculated using 28 g as the average weight of the birds). Immediately following the injection, the focal bird was returned to his cage for another 30 min and their behavior was recorded. Once injected, the drugs were likely taken up into the brain rapidly (within 2 min in Sprague-Dawley rats (Mukherjee et al., 2004)). The non-focal bird remained within the chamber throughout the trial and did not receive any injections.
Each focal bird was tested in three separate trials in which he was administered either the D2 agonist, D2 antagonist, or saline (the order of the drug treatments and control was randomized across birds using a random number generator). Two days after the focal bird’s trials were completed, the above experimental procedure was repeated except that the designation of the birds reversed: the previously non-focal bird in the pair was designated as the focal bird while the previously focal bird became the non-focal bird. Because the half-life of raclopride and PPHT are both estimated to be approximately 30 min (Mukherjee et al., 2004; Köhler et al., 1985), 2 days between trials ensured that the drugs were eliminated from the body between trials. All six trials of a given pair were conducted within 95 days of the birds’ capture from the wild (mean ± SD: 31 ± 25 days) and between the hours of 08:00 and 13:00. Birds were released to their capture site at the conclusion of the study. Due to the short half-life of the treatment drugs, we did not anticipate that the birds would have adverse effects after release.
Behavioral analysis
The behavior of the focal birds was scored from the video recordings. During a 10-min period preceding the injection (“pre-period”) as well as a 10-min period following the injection (“post-period”), we recorded the amount of time the focal birds spent biting inanimate non-food objects (including the cage, perches, or their own leg bands) with their bill (henceforth ‘biting’). In some species, biting is a coping behavior that can reduce stress associated with physical restraint, food restriction, or unfavorable environmental conditions (Cabezas et al., 2013; Fossat et al., 2015; Hanson et al., 2020). During the pre- and post-periods, we also scored the amount of time the focal birds engaged in intraspecific aggression (pecking or biting the non-focal bird), preening (cleaning feathers with the bill or feet), bill wiping (rubbing bill on perch), feather ruffling (fluffing up and shaking feathers) and general movement (hopping or flying around the cage). Behaviors were scored by recording the frames at which the behaviors began and ended. We calculated the total amount of time the birds engaged in each behavior during the pre- and post-period for each treatment or control. One experimenter (MRF) scored all of the videos frame-by-frame using QuickTime (version 7; 60 frames/s; Apple Inc., Los Altos, CA, USA) and was blinded to the treatment.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were performed in R, Version 3.6.2 (R Core Team, 2019) and all models were performed using the package “lme4” (version 1.1-27.1; Bates et al., 2015). For each trial, we calculated a behavioral change score by subtracting the amount of time the birds engaged in each behavior during the pre-period from the amount of time the birds engaged in each behavior during the post-period. Because there were many trials in which biting, preening, and aggression did not occur, we adopted a two-step approach to account for this zero-inflated data. In the first step (“binomial model”), we performed a binomial mixed effect model with the response variable as whether or not the birds performed any biting, preening, or aggression behavior after injection; the treatment/control, trial order, change in weight during captivity, and change in general movement behavior were fixed effects in this model. In the second step (“count model”), we performed a linear mixed effect model using only trials where the biting, preening, or aggression behavior occurred after injection; we used the treatment/control, trial order, change in weight during captivity, and change in general movement behavior as fixed effects in this model. Because there were only two trials in which the birds exhibited biting after the antagonist injection, it was not possible to perform a statistical analysis on this group and it was therefore removed from the biting model in the second step (“count model”) of this analysis. General movement was characterized by a single linear mixed effect model with change in general movement as the response variable and treatment/control, trial order, and change in weight during captivity as fixed effects. We included individual bird identity as a repeated measure in all models. For any model in which the treatment/control type was significant, we then performed pairwise comparisons to evaluate differences among the treatment/control using the R package ‘emmeans’ (version 1.7.0; Searle, Speed & Milliken, 1980) and used a Bonferroni correction to evaluate statistical significance. Feather ruffling and bill wiping were both rare behaviors, occurring on average less than 2% and 1% of the time in each trial respectively and therefore were not included in the analysis. Raw data is available in the Supplemental Information.
Results
The treatment/control was the only significant predictor of whether biting occurred or not (n = 20, F = 4.31, p = 0.001; Table 1; Fig. 1). Biting occurred in 19 out of 20 trials after administering the agonist, 2 out of 20 trials after the antagonist, and 4 out of 20 trials after saline. The treatment/control was also the only significant predictor of the amount of time spent biting (assuming time biting was above zero). The birds spent more time biting when administered the D2 agonist compared to the control (n = 19, t = 4.84, p = 0.002; Table 2). No other variable predicted the presence or amount of biting (Table 1).
Response variable | Independent variables | Numerator df, denominator df | F-value (p-value) |
---|---|---|---|
Biting (Binomial model) | |||
Treatment/control | 2, 53.0 | 4.31 (0.001)* | |
Trial Order | 2, 53.0 | 1.24 (0.21) | |
General movement | 1, 53.0 | 0.41 (0.77) | |
Weight change | 1, 53.0 | 2.04 (0.15) | |
Biting (Count model) | |||
Treatment/control | 1, 4.49 | 33.04 (0.003)* | |
Trial Order | 2, 4.83 | 1.76 (0.26) | |
General movement | 1, 9.81 | 3.03 (0.11) | |
Weight change | 1, 18.99 | 1.85 (0.18) | |
Preening (Binomial model) | |||
Treatment/control | 2, 53.0 | 0.48 (0.71) | |
General movement | 1, 53.0 | 1.06 (0.49) | |
Trial order | 2, 53.0 | 0.58 (0.35) | |
Weight change | 1, 53.0 | 2.73 (0.09) | |
Preening (Count model) | |||
Treatment/control | 2, 14.0 | 0.40 (0.67) | |
General movement | 1, 14.0 | 6.25 (0.02)* | |
Trial order | 2, 14.0 | 1.61 (0.23) | |
Weight change | 1, 14.0 | 3.10 (0.09) | |
Aggression (Binomial model) | |||
Treatment/control | 2, 53.0 | 2.06 (0.13) | |
General movement | 1, 53.0 | 0.07 (0.87) | |
Trial order | 2, 53.0 | 0.24 (0.85) | |
Weight change | 1, 53.0 | 0.73 (0.38) | |
Aggression (Count model) | |||
Treatment/control | 2, 2.91 | 1.32 (0.38) | |
General movement | 1, 11.36 | 1.77 (0.20) | |
Trial order | 2, 3.73 | 1.05 (0.43) | |
Weight change | 1, 14.23 | 0.02 (0.88) | |
General movement | |||
Treatment/control | 2, 36.0 | 1.53 (0.22) | |
Trial order | 2, 36.0 | 1.91 (0.16) | |
Weight change | 1, 18.0 | 2.21 (0.15) |
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.13520/table-1
Note:
Statistical significance is indicated with an asterisk.
Figure 1: The change in number of frames birds (n = 20) spent on biting (A) general movement (B), aggression (C), and preening (D). Data points representing the same individual are connected with lines. Asterisks indicate statistical significance. DOI: 10.7717/peerj.13520/fig-1
Numerator df, denominator df | t-ratio (p-value) | |
---|---|---|
Comparisons (Biting Binomial model) | ||
Control vs Agonist | 1, 1.62 | 3.21 (0.003)* |
Control vs Antagonist | 1, 1.07 | 1.19 (0.69) |
Agonist vs Antagonist | 1, 1.82 | 3.57 (0.001)* |
Comparisons (Biting Count model) | ||
Control vs Agonist | 1, 6.11 | 4.84 (0.002)* |
Control vs Antagonist | -* | -* |
Agonist vs Antagonist | -* | -* |
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.13520/table-2
Note:
Statistical significance is indicated with an asterisk.
*Because only two birds exhibited biting after the injection in the antagonist trials, it was not possible to perform a statistical analysis on comparisons involving the antagonist.
The treatment/control had no effect on either the presence or amount of preening, aggression, or general movement (n = 20, F = 0.40, p = 0.67; n = 20, F = 1.32, p = 0.38; n = 20, F = 153, p = 0.22; Table 1). The birds were less likely to preen when they spent more time moving (n = 20 F = 6.25, p = 0.02; Table 1). No other variable predicted presence or amount of preening, aggression, or general movement (Table 1).
Discussion
We found possible support for the hypothesis that dopamine influences stress-related coping behavior in a wild-caught species. In particular, the house sparrows spent more time biting at inanimate objects when they were administered the D2 agonist. Biting at inanimate objects may be a coping mechanism in some bird species (Savory & Kostal, 1993; Nicol, 1987; Zarrindast, Hajian-Heydari & Hoseini-Nia, 1992), but has not yet been demonstrated in passerines or any other wild species. The amount of time house sparrows spent biting did not decrease when they were administered the D2 antagonist, although this is not surprising as they rarely (only two birds) exhibited biting prior to the antagonist administration. Biting behavior also did not increase when administered a saline control, indicating that the behavior is unlikely to be a response to stress induced by handling and injection. We also did not find any associations between the biting behavior and weight loss in captivity; weight loss is often a sign of stress from captivity (Parker Fischer, Wright-Lichter & Romero, 2018) and this therefore suggests that stress associated with captivity did not influence the biting behavior. Furthermore, increases in dopamine receptor activation are often associated with increases in general movement (Beninger, 1983). However, even after controlling for general movement, we still found that the house sparrows spent more time biting at inanimate objects when administered the D2 agonist.
In rodent models as well as poultry, environmental stressors elevate dopamine levels in the mesolimbic system (Cabib & Puglisi-Allegra, 1996; Cheng, Singleton & Muir, 2003). We therefore speculate that the activation of D2 dopamine receptors by the agonist could induce neurochemical changes consistent with stress in the house sparrows and the birds behaviorally coped with this perceived stress by biting at inanimate objects. Coping behaviors are de-arousal mechanisms that work to restore homeostasis after a disturbance (Savory & Kostal, 2006). Domesticated chickens (Gallus gallus) bite at inanimate objects when they experience stressful conditions such as high densities and low temperatures (Spinu, Benveneste & Degen, 2003) and those that engage in more pecking at inanimate objects had lower corticosterone levels (Kostal, Savory & Hughes, 1992). Rodents that bite at inanimate objects during stressful situations such as restraint and introduction into a new environment also have lower levels of cortisol and corticosterone, respectively (Sato et al., 2010; Hennessy & Foy, 1987). The behavior observed in this experiment may also act as a coping behavior, however we did not measure physiological measures of stress such as corticosterone or heart rate. Therefore, future studies are needed to determine whether the observed biting behavior also impacts stress levels in wild songbirds.
While it is possible that the house sparrows exhibited biting behavior to cope with stress, it is also possible that the biting behavior serves no purpose or even has negative effects. Repeated biting at inanimate objects is not a behavior typically observed in wild sparrows, making it a possible stereotypy (defined as a repetitive movement that serves no apparent purpose; Powell et al., 1999). Stereotypic behaviors can have positive effects; for example, voles performing stereotypy in captivity have higher rates of survival and reproduction, although the mechanism is unknown (Schønecker, 2009). However, stereotypy can also have neutral or negative effects (Mason & Latham, 2004). For example, tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) that exhibited stereotypic head twirling behaviors exhibited a more negative affective state compared to conspecifics that did not exhibit these stereotypic behaviors (Pomerantz et al., 2012). Stereotypic feather plucking in African gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus) is also associated with higher levels of fecal corticosterone suggesting they are experiencing chronic stress that is not extinguished by the stereotypy (Costa et al., 2016; Owen & Lane, 2006). A study in quail that (Balthazart, Castagna & Ball, 1997) used the same D2 agonist at the same concentration as our study; the quail exhibited extremely high rates of pecking behavior that were interpreted as a stereotypy and their pecking behavior may be analogous to the biting observed in this study. Therefore, the dopamine agonist in this study may be producing abnormal conditions in the brain leading to stereotyped behaviors that do not aid in coping with stress.
It is unlikely that the house sparrows exhibited increased biting because they were hungry. Although the birds did not have access to food during any of the trials, they only increased their biting after administration of the D2 agonist but not the D2 antagonist or saline. Rodents and Japanese quail also increased their biting behavior after they were given a D2 agonist, but they did not increase their food consumption even though food was available (Nicol, 1987; Beninger, 1983). This suggests that biting induced by D2 receptor activation is not related to an increased motivation to feed.
We also did not find support for the possibility that the birds increased biting because they became more aggressive. There was no relationship between biting inanimate objects and intraspecific aggression. Furthermore, the house sparrows exhibited very low levels of aggression across all trials. Similarly, manipulating dopamine receptor activation in group-housed chickens did not influence aggression towards conspecifics (Dennis, Muir & Cheng, 2006). These results indicate that the D2 receptors are not involved in aggression among familiar conspecifics, but further experiments could examine if this is also the case among unfamiliar conspecifics that are more likely to engage in aggression (Hegner & Wingfield, 1987).
We found that the D2 antagonist did not influence biting or any other behaviors in house sparrows. Because the birds rarely exhibited biting before the D2 antagonist treatment, there was little opportunity for the behavior to become even less frequent. In fact, only two birds exhibited biting before or after the D2 antagonist injection. However, the treatment may have effects when the behavior is already present. When chickens are chronically stressed, D2 antagonists reduced the amount of time they spent biting at inanimate objects (Savory & Kostal, 1993). Furthermore, a different dopamine antagonist administered to rats only had an impact on behavior after multiple treatments (Ikemoto & Panksepp, 1996). Additional studies could examine whether D2 antagonists likewise alter coping behavior in songbirds when their stress levels are high.
Conclusions
We found that an increase in D2 dopamine receptor activation in house sparrows leads to increased biting of inanimate objects. Because biting behavior may be a coping behavior to reduce stress (Savory & Kostal, 1993; Nicol, 1987), our results could indicate that dopamine is involved in songbirds’ stress response. However, the observed behavior may instead be an expression of stereotypic behavior. D2 dopamine receptor activation alters biting behavior in domesticated birds (Dennis, Muir & Cheng, 2006; Savory & Kostal, 1993; Zarrindast, Hajian-Heydari & Hoseini-Nia, 1992), indicating that the dopamine pathways between these domestic species and the wild songbird in this study are likely similar. While many studies have examined the influence of hormones on the stress response (Schoech, Rensel & Heiss, 2011; Creel et al., 2013), our understanding of how neurotransmitters impact stress in wild species is still poor (Lattin et al., 2019; Trainor, 2011). To further understand this question, research on the effect of dopaminergic drugs on birds that were socially or physiologically stressed compared to unstressed controls would be valuable. It would also be interesting to look at the effects of different dosages of these drugs, to see if the behavioral effects were specific to high dosages such as reported by Balthazart, Castagna & Ball (1997). Finally, we only used male birds in this experiment and further work is needed to determine if sex has an influence on dopamine’s effect on the stress response. Wild species are increasingly exposed to stress due to human disturbances (Birnie-Gauvin et al., 2016). Our current understanding of how animals perceive and cope with stress mostly comes from domestic animals, which may respond to stress in different ways than their wild counterparts. Therefore a greater understanding of how wild organisms respond to stress will be crucial to evaluate how these organisms will manage stressors in their environments.
Supplemental Information
Raw Experimental Data.
The count data for each behavior as well as the bird’s ID, trial order number, number of days each bird was in captivity, and treatment administered.
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.13520/supp-1
Download
ARRIVE 2.0 Checklist.
DOI: 10.7717/peerj.13520/supp-2
Download
Additional Information and Declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Author Contributions
Melanie R. Florkowski conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, prepared figures and/or tables, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, and approved the final draft.
Jessica L. Yorzinski conceived and designed the experiments, authored or reviewed drafts of the article, and approved the final draft.
Animal Ethics
The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):
The Texas A&M University’s Animal Care and Use Committee approved the study (IACUC# 2019-0219).
Data Availability
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
The raw data is available in the Supplemental File.
Funding
Jessica L. Yorzinski was supported by the National Science Foundation (BCS #1926327), the College of Agriculture and Life Sciences at Texas A&M University and Texas A&M AgriLife Research. Melanie R. Florkowski was supported by the College of Agriculture and Life Sciences and the College of Science at Texas A&M University as well as Texas A&M AgriLife Research and Texas A&M University’s Office of Graduate and Professional Students. Texas A&M University’s Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology provided funding for open access costs. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Abercrombie DE, Keefe KA, DiFrischia DS, Zigmond MJ. 1989. Differential effect of stress on in vivo dopamine release in striatum, nucleus accumbens, and medial frontal cortex. Journal of Neurochemistry 52(5):1655-1658
Aisa B, Tordera R, Lasheras B, Del Río J, Ramírez MJ. 2007. Cognitive impairment associated to HPA axis hyperactivity after maternal separation in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32(3):256-266
Baik J. 2020. Stress and the dopaminergic reward system. Experimental & Molecular Medicine 52(12):1879-1890
Balthazart J, Castagna C, Ball GF. 1997. Differential effects of D1 and D2 dopamine-receptor agonists and antagonists on appetitive and consummatory aspects of male sexual behavior in Japanese Quail. Physiology & Behavior 62(3):571-580
Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1):1-48
Beaugeard E, Brischoux F, Henry PY, Parenteau C, Trouvé C, Angelier F. 2018. Does urbanization cause stress in wild birds during development? Insights from feather corticosterone levels in juvenile house sparrows (Passer domesticus) Ecology and Evolution 9(1):640-652
Beninger RJ. 1983. The role of dopamine in locomotor activity and learning. Brain Research Reviews 6(2):173-196
Birnie-Gauvin K, Peiman KS, Gaallagher AJ, de Bruijn R, Cooke SJ. 2016. Sublethal consequences of urban life for wild vertebrates. Environmental Reviews 24(4):416-425
Brown JS, Kotler BP. 2004. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecology Letters 7(10):999-1014
Cabezas S, Carrete M, Tella JL, Marchant TA, Bortolotti GR. 2013. Differences in acute stress responses between wild-caught and captive-bred birds: a physiological mechanism contributing to current avian invasions? Biological Invasions 15(3):521-527
Cabib S, Puglisi-Allegra S. 1996. Stress, depression and the mesolimbic dopamine system. Psychopharmacology 128(4):331-342
Cabib S, Puglisi-Allegra S. 2012. The mesoaccumbens dopamine in coping with stress. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 36(1):79-89
Cheng HW, Singleton P, Muir W. 2003. Social stress in laying hens: differential effect of stress on plasma dopamine concentrations and adrenal function in genetically selected chickens. Poultry Science 82(2):192-198
Chrousos GP. 2009. Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nature Reviews Endocrinology 5(7):374-381
Cirule D, Krama T, Vrublevska J, Rantala MJ, Krams I. 2012. A rapid effect of handling on counts of white blood cells in a wintering passerine bird: a more practical measure of stress? Journal of Ornithology 153(1):161-166
Costa P, Macchi E, Valle E, De Marco M, Nucera DM, Gasco L, Schiavone A. 2016. An association between feather damaging behavior and corticosterone metabolite excretion in captive African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) PeerJ 4(1):e2462
Creel S, Dantzer B, Goymann W, Rubenstein DR. 2013. The ecology of stress: effects of the social environment. Functional Ecology 27(1):66-80
Dennis RL, Muir WM, Cheng HW. 2006. Effects of raclopride on aggression and stress in diversely selected chicken lines. Behavioural Brain Research 175(1):104-111
Fossat P, Bacqué-Cazenave J, De Deurwaerdère P, Cattaert D, Delbecque JP. 2015. Serotonin, but not dopamine, controls the stress response and anxiety-like behavior in the crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Journal of Experimental Biology 218(Suppl. 3):2745-2752
Giorgi O, Lecca D, Piras G, Driscoll P, Corda MG. 2003. Dissociation between mesocortical dopamine release and fear-related behaviours in two psychogenetically selected lines of rats that differ in coping strategies to aversive conditions. European Journal of Neuroscience 17(12):2716-2726
Hanson HE, Mathews NS, Hauber ME, Martin LB. 2020. The house sparrow in the service of basic and applied biology. eLife 9:4542
Hegner RE, Wingfield JC. 1987. Effects of experimental manipulation of testosterone levels on parental investment and breeding success in male house sparrows. The Auk 104(3):462-469
Hennessy HB, Foy T. 1987. Nonedible material elicits chewing and reduces the plasma corticosterone response during novelty exposure in mice. Behavioral Neuroscience 101(2):237-245
Henson SM, Weldon LM, Hayward JL, Greene DJ, Megna LC, Serem MC. 2012. Coping behaviour as an adaptation to stress: post-disturbance preening in colonial seabirds. Journal of Biological Dynamics 6(1):17-37
Hori N, Yuyama N, Tamura K. 2004. Biting suppresses stress-induced expression of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) in the rat hypothalamus. Journal of Dental Research 83(2):124-128
Ikemoto S, Panksepp J. 1996. Dissociations between appetitive and consummatory responses by pharmacological manipulations of reward-relevant brain regions. Behavioral Neuroscience 110(2):331-345
Jones GH, Mittleman G, Robbins TW. 1989. Attenuation of amphetamine-stereotypy by mesostriatal dopamine depletion enhances plasma corticosterone: implications for stereotypy as a coping response. Behavioral and Neural Biology 51(1):80-91
Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, Van Der Vegt BJ, Van Reenen CG, Hopster H, De Jong IC, Ruis MAW, Blokhuis HJ. 1999. Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 23(7):925-935
Kostal L, Savory CJ, Hughes BO. 1992. Diurnal and individual variation in behaviour of restricted-fed broiler breeders. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 32(4):361-374
Köhler C, Hall H, Ögren SO, Gawell L. 1985. Specific in vitro and in vivo binding of 3H-raclopride a potent substituted benzamide drug with high affinity for dopamine D-2 receptors in the rat brain. Biochemical Pharmacology 34(13):2251-2259
Lange H, Leimar O. 2004. Social stability and daily body mass gain in great tits. Behavioral Ecology 15(4):549-554
Lattin CR, Merullo DP, Riters LV, Carson RE. 2019. In vivo imaging of D2 receptors and corticosteroids predict behavioural responses to captivity stress in a wild bird. Scientific Reports 9(1):1-13
Love OP, Breuner CW, Vézina F, Williams TD. 2004. Mediation of a corticosterone-induced reproductive conflict. Hormones and Behavior 46(1):59-65
López AL, Villanueva ME, Padilla MB, Jaime HB, Aguilar FA. 2018. Chronic unpredictable mild stress progressively disturbs glucose metabolism and appetite hormones in rats. Acta Endocrinologica (Bucharest) 14(1):16-23
Mason GJ, Latham NR. 2004. Can’t stop, won’t stop: is stereotypy a reliable animal welfare indicator? Animal Welfare 13:S57-S69
Mukherjee J, Narayanan TK, Christian BT, Shi B, Yang ZY. 2004. Binding characteristics of high-affinity dopamine D2/D3 receptor agonists, 11C-PPHT and 11C-ZYY-339 in rodents and imaging in non-human primates by PET. Synapse 54(2):83-91
Nicol CJ. 1987. Behavioural responses of laying hens following a period of spatial restriction. Animal Behavior 35:1709-1719
Owen DJ, Lane JM. 2006. High levels of corticosterone in feather-plucking parrots (Psittacus erithacus) Veterinary Record 158(23):804-805
Parker Fischer C, Wright-Lichter J, Romero LM. 2018. Chronic stress and the introduction to captivity: how wild house sparrows (Passer domesticus) adjust to laboratory condition. General and Comparative Endocrinology 259(1):85-92
Pomerantz O, Terkel J, Suomi SJ, Paukner A. 2012. Stereotypic head twirls, but not pacing, are related to a ‘pessimistic’-like judgment bias among captive tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) Animal Cognition 15(4):689-698
Powell SB, Newman HA, Pendergast JF, Lewis MH. 1999. A rodent model of spontaneous stereotypy: initial characteriszation of developmental, environmental, and neurobiological factors. Physiology & Behavior 66(2):355-363
Pravosudov VV, Kitaysky AS, Wingfield JC, Clayton NS. 2001. Long-term unpredictable foraging conditions and physiological stress response in mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli) General and Comparative Endocrinology 123(3):324-331
Puri S, Ray A, Chakravarti A, Sen P. 1994. Role of dopaminergic mechanisms in the regulation of stress responses in experimental animals. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 48(1):53-56
R Core Team. 2019. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Version 3.6.2. Vienna: The R Foundation for Statistical Computing
Raulo A, Dantzer B. 2018. Associations between glucocorticoids and sociality across a continuum of vertebrate social behavior. Ecology and Evolution 8(15):7697-7716
Reis-Silva TM, Sandini TM, Calefi AS, Orlando BCG, Moreira N, Lima APN, Florio JC, Queiroz-Hazarbassanov NGT, Bernardi MM. 2019. Stress resilience evidenced by grooming behaviour and dopamine levels in male mice selected for high and low immobility using the tail suspension test. European Journal of Neuroscience 50(6):2942-2954
Romero LM, Wikelski M. 2010. Stress physiology as a predictor of survival in Galapagos marine iguanas. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277(1697):3157-3162
Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEwen BS. 1986. The neuroendocrinology of stress and aging: the glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis. Endocrine Reviews 7(3):284-301
Sato C, Sato S, Takashina H, Ishii H, Onozuka M, Sasaguri K. 2010. Bruxism affects stress responses in stressed rats. Clinical Oral Investigations 14(2):153-160
Savory C, Kostal L. 1993. Behavioural responses to reserpine treatment in restricted-fed broiler breeder fowls. Medical Science Research 21:351-352
Savory CJ, Kostal L. 2006. Is expression of some behaviours associated with de-arousal in restricted-fed chickens? Physiology & Behavior 88(4–5):473-478
Savory CJ, Seawright E, Watson A. 1992. Stereotyped behaviour in broiler breeders in relation to husbandry and opioid receptor blockade. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 32(4):349-360
Schoech SJ, Rensel MA, Heiss RS. 2011. Short- and long-term effects of developmental corticosterone exposure on avian physiology, behavioral phenotype, cognition, and fitness: a review. Current Zoology 57(4):514-530
Schouten WGP, Wiepkema PR. 1991. Coping styles of tethered sows. Behavioural Processes 25:125-132
Schønecker B. 2009. Increased survival and reproductive success associated with stereotypical behaviours in laboratory-bred bank voles (Clethrionomys glareolus) Applied Animal Behaviour Science 121(1):55-62
Searle SR, Speed FM, Milliken GA. 1980. Population marginal meaans in the linear model: an alternative to least squares means. The American Statistician 34(4):216-221
Shini S, Huff GR, Shini A, Kaiser P. 2010. Understanding stress-induced immunosuppression: exploration of cytokine and chemokine gene profiles in chicken peripheral leukocytes. Poultry Science 89(4):841-851
Spinu M, Benveneste S, Degen A. 2003. Effect of density and season on stress and behaviour in broiler breeder hens. British Poultry Science 44(2):170-174
Sullivan RM, Dufresne MM. 2006. Mesocortical dopamine and HPA axis regulation: role of laterality and early environment. Brain Research 1076(1):49-59
Sullivan R, Szechtman H. 1995. Asymmetrical influence of mesocortical dopamine depletion on stress ulcer development and subcortical dopamine systems in rats: implications for psychopathology. Neuroscience 65(3):757-766
Thaker M, Lima SL, Hews DK. 2009. Acute corticosterone elevation enhances antipredator behaviors in male tree lizard morphs. Hormones and Behavior 56(1):51-57
Trainor BC. 2011. Stress responses and the mesolimbic dopamine system: social contexts and sex differences. Hormones and Behavior 60(5):457-469
Vitousek MN, Johnson MA, Downs CJ, Miller ET, Martin LB, Francis CD, Donald JW, Fuxjager MJ, Goymann W, Hau M. 2019. Macroevolutionary patterning in glucocorticoids suggests different selective pressures shape baseline and stress-induced levels. The American Naturalist 193(6):866-880
Voellmy IK, Purser J, Flynn D, Kennedy P, Simpson SD, Radford AN. 2014. Acoustic noise reduces foraging success in two sympatric fish species via different mechanisms. Animal Behavior 89(1257):191-198
Wingfield JC, Maney DL, Breuner CW, Jacobs JD, Lynn S, Ramenofsky M, Richardson RD. 1998. Ecological bases of hormone—behavior interactions: the emergency life history stage. American Zoologist 38(1):191-206
Wingfield J, Sapolsky R. 2003. Reproduction and resistance to stress: when and how. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 15(8):711-724
Zarrindast M, Hajian-Heydari A, Hoseini-Nia T. 1992. Characterization of dopamine receptors involved in apomorphine-induced pecking in pigeons. General Pharmacology: The Vascular System 23(3):427-430
Zawilska J, Derbiszewska T, Nowak J. 1996. Pharmacological modifications in dopaminergic neurotransmission affect the quinpirole-evoked suppression of serotonin N-acetyltransferase activity in chick retina: an impact on dopamine D4-like receptors. Journal of Neural Transmission 103(12):1405-1414
Zimmer C, Taff CC, Ardia DR, Ryan TA, Winkler DW, Vitousek MN. 2019. On again, off again: acute stress response and negative feedback together predict resilience to experimental challenges. Functional Ecology 33(4):619-628
Melanie R. Florkowski1, Jessica L. Yorzinski1,2
1 Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Program, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States
2 Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2022 Florkowski and Yorzinski. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Animals experience stress throughout their lives and exhibit both physiological and behavioral responses to cope with it. The stress response can become harmful when prolonged and increasing evidence suggests that dopamine plays a critical role in extinguishing the stress response. In particular, activation of the D2 dopamine receptor reduces glucocorticoids and increases coping behavior, i.e., behavioral responses to adverse stimuli that reduce the harmful effects of stress. However, few studies have examined the effects of dopamine on the stress responses of wild species. We therefore tested the hypothesis that activation of the D2 dopamine receptor influences coping-like behavior in a wild-caught species. We recorded behavior of house sparrows (Passer domesticus) before and after they received injections of D2 dopamine agonists, D2 dopamine antagonists, or saline. House sparrows are common in urban environments and understanding how they cope with stress may help us better understand how animals cope with urban stressors. We found that the birds significantly increased biting of inanimate objects after the agonist but there was no change following the antagonist or saline. The biting of inanimate objects may be a mechanism of behavioral coping. This change in biting behavior was not correlated with general movement. This study supports the hypothesis that D2 dopamine receptor activation is involved in the regulation of the stress response in a wild bird.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer