The magnitude and characteristics of cognitive impairments following stroke show substantial individual differences across patients. While some patients show considerable deficits and subsequent increased risk of neurodegeneration and dementia, others show no observable impairments. This heterogeneity in cognitive deficits after stroke arise not only from differences in the localization and extent of focal lesions, but also from potential secondary cascade effects in terms of altered brain connectivity (Rehme & Grefkes, 2013), and alterations of the hierarchical brain network structure (Stam, 2014). Currently, common predictors of cognitive deficits and recovery include anatomical location, lesion severity, vascular risk factors, chronic brain pathology, and prestroke cognitive impairment (Bentley et al., 2014; Macciocchi et al., 1998; Munsch et al., 2016; Pendlebury, 2009). However, recent studies indicate added predictive value of connectivity-based measures, which capture perturbations of brain network connections or dynamics beyond focal lesion site (Ktena Sofia et al., 2019; Lopes et al., 2021b).
In the last decade, a large body of literature has characterized the brain as a complex network consisting of nodes and their connections, collectively termed the brain connectome. Building on functional imaging, a coarse parcellation of networks separate the brain in the cingulo-opercular, frontoparietal, ventral attention and default mode networks, aiding cognitive control. The cingulo-opercular network is associated with the ability to maintain focus, the ability to sustain top-down cognitive control across cognitive tasks (Cai et al., 2016; Hilger et al., 2017; Uddin et al., 2019; Wilk et al., 2012), whereas the frontoparietal network is assumed to guide moment to moment-attentional control (Fassbender et al., 2006; Majerus et al., 2018). The ventral attention network serves reorientation to relevant stimuli outside the scope of current attention (Corbetta & Shulman, 2011; Vossel et al., 2014), while default mode network is commonly associated with self-referential internal activity, and is commonly downregulated during task engagement (McKiernan et al., 2003; Vatansever et al., 2018). A central property of network nodes is how densely they are connected. Highly connected nodes, or hubs, are critical for efficient information flow between brain regions and are thought to play a crucial role in pivoting neural activity across distal brain regions, allowing for the integration of information required to support cognitive operations (Cole et al., 2013). Thus, focal damage to any part of a brain network might cause disruptions of distal but intact brain regions. Indeed, even small lesions in a densely connected hub may cause connectome-wide perturbations (Aben et al., 2019). Recent studies have therefore moved beyond traditional lesion-symptom mapping, to include measures of network dysfunction to explain and predict stroke sequelae (Lim & Kang, 2015; Ulrichsen et al., 2020). Recent findings using the lesion-network mapping approach suggest that patients with overlapping symptoms have lesions in regions that are functionally connected and that lesions to brain network hubs or their connecting white matter pathways are associated with more symptoms (Fox, 2018). This network-based concept has also been shown to apply to a wide range of brain disorders such as Alzheimer disease, schizophrenia and multiple sclerosis (Crossley et al., 2014; Stam, 2014; van den Heuvel & Sporns, 2019).
Whereas association between functional connectivity following stoke has been investigated in recent years (Klingbeil et al., 2019; Lopes et al., 2021a; Ptak et al., 2020), less is known regarding structural connectivity. The brain is connected by an intricate web of white matter (WM) pathways, consisting of bundles of myelinated axons responsible for conveying signals between brain regions, supporting functional networks. While measuring axonal disconnections in the living human brain has remained a challenging task, a recent implementation of diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)-based tractography for assessing full-brain connection probabilities has enabled opportunities for detailed estimation of the connections of one or several lesions in individual patients (Foulon et al., 2018). Based on normative data from healthy controls, voxel-wise disconnection probability maps for a particular patient can be derived based on a lesion map. The extent and distribution of these disconnectivity maps have been shown to correlate with a surrogate biomarker of neuronal damage in patients with MS (Rise et al., 2021) and shown promise in predicting deficits following stroke (Salvalaggio et al., 2020).
To test for associations between brain disconnection and cognitive performance, we used Partial Least Squares analysis (PLS) to map common variance between voxel-wise structural disconnection probability maps in 102 stroke survivors and their performance on the Montreal Cognitive assessment (MoCA; Nasreddine et al., 2005). PLS (Krishnan et al., 2011) is well-suited for investigating multivariate associations between neuroimaging features and non-Gaussian behavioral and clinical measures commonly obtained from stroke patients (Blackburn et al., 2013). Although PLS has been suggested to display less anatomical specificity, it has shown to produce higher stability in results (Ivanova et al., 2021). Importantly, MoCA have been shown to reliably identify cognitive impairment in clinical samples (Bernstein et al., 2011), across latent variables related to executive function, language, memory visuospatial skills, working memory, as well as orientation (Freitas et al., 2012). Based on the literature reviewed above, we hypothesized that patients with higher levels of stroke-induced brain disconnectivity would show poorer cognitive performance, and further anticipated that this association could not simply be explained by the size and location of the lesion itself. We further hypothesized that derived scores from the PLS would correlate with a broader cognitive battery derived for stroke patients (Willer et al., 2016). Since previous studies and existing models of the distributed nature of the neuroanatomical basis of cognitive functions are sparse, we remained agnostic about the anatomical distribution of the associations with the disconnectome maps and performed an unbiased full brain analysis with appropriate corrections for multiple comparisons.
METHODSThe present cross-sectional study included participants previously described in detail (Dørum et al., 2020; Kolskår et al., 2020; Richard et al., 2020; Ulrichsen et al., 2020). Briefly, the sample comprised 102 stroke survivors from sub-acute (>24 h poststroke and in clinical stable condition) to chronic stage. Inclusion criteria were radiologically documented ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke, and exclusion criteria were psychiatric conditions (bipolar disorder or schizophrenia), other neurological conditions including known cognitive impairment prestroke, substance abuse, and contraindications for MRI compatibility. All participants gave their written consent before participating, and the study was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics South-East Norway (2014/694 and 2015/1282).
Table 1 displays sample demographics as well as time between stroke and MRI and cognitive assessment.
TABLE 1 Summary of clinical and demographic variables
Mean | SD | Min | Max | |
Age | 66.3 | 12.3 | 24 | 87 |
Sex | 74.5% males | |||
Education (self-report, years) | 14.6 | 3.3 | 7 | 30 |
Interval between stroke onset and MRI/MoCA assessment (days) | 515 | 444 | 1 | 1399 |
Lesion size (2 mm resolution) | 7693 | 24,723 | 5 | 30,031 |
Number of lesions | 3 | 2 | 1 | 12 |
Interval between stroke onset and CabPad assessment (days) | 634 | 379 | 89 | 1428 |
NIHSS (score) | 1.02 | 1.35 | 0 | 7 |
Hemispheric distribution left: 39/ right: 41/ bilateral: 19/ subcortical: 2 | ||||
Time since stroke: acute: 20/ subacute: 25/ chronic: 57 |
NIHSS, National Institute of Stroke Scale; CapPad, Cognitive Assessment at Bedside for iPad; Acute, less than seven days since stroke; Subacute: between 7 and 180 days since stroke; Chronic, more than 180 days since stroke.
Cognitive assessmentPatients were assessed with MoCA (Nasreddine et al., 2005) at time of MRI scanning. At follow-up (Table 1), a subsample (n = 82) were also assessed using CabPad (Willer et al., 2016), a computerized test battery assessing a range of functions, including motor speed (finger tapping) for dominant and nondominant hand, verbal fluency (phonetic and semantic word generation), attention span (symbol sequencing), working memory (reversed symbol sequencing), a spatial stroop/flanker task, spatial short-term memory, and psychomotor speed (symbol-digit coding task).
MRI acquisitionPatients were scanned at Oslo University Hospital on a 3T GE 750 Discovery MRI scanner with a 32-channel head coil. T2-FLAIR images were acquired with the following parameters: TR: 8000 ms; TE: 127 ms, TI: 2240 ms; flip angle (FA): 90°; voxel size: 1 × 1 × 1 mm. T1-weighted scans were collected using a 3D IR-prepared FSPGR (BRAVO) sequence (TR: 8.16 ms; TE: 3.18 ms; TI: 450 ms; FA: 12°; voxel size: 1 × 1 × 1 mm; FOV: 256 × 256, 188 sagittal slices) for coregistration.
Lesion delineationLesion delineation was initially guided on each participant's FLAIR image, using the semiautomated Clusterize-Toolbox, implemented in MATLAB (de Haan et al., 2015), before finalized by manual demarcation. The demarcation was guided by radiological descriptions and evaluated by a medical doctor before finalization. Normalization parameters were estimated by linear registration of the structural T1-image to the MNI152-template and applied to the lesion masks using Flirt (Jenkinson & Smith, 2001). Figure 1 displays overlap in lesion location across the sample.
FIGURE 1. Lesion overlap across 102 stroke patients. 12 transversal slices, with 5-mm thickness. Color scale indicates number of participants overlapping. Z coordinates denotes transversal slices in the MNI152-coordinate system
To asess goup differences in MoCA-score between the patients in acute, subacute and chronic phase, a linear model was estimated with MoCA-score as dependent variable, and group as independent variable. To assess impact of lesion load, a linear model was estimated with MoCA-score as dependent variable and lesion size and number of lesions as independent variables.
Estimation of structural disconnectome mapsTo estimate the extent of the structural disconnection for each patient, we employed an automated tractography-based procedure (Foulon et al., 2018). Briefly, full-brain tractography data of 170 healthy controls from the 7T Human Connectome Project was used as a normative training set to identify fibers passing through each lesion (Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2020). Using affine and diffeomorphic deformations (Avants et al., 2011; Klein et al., 2009), individual patient's lesion maps were registered to each control's native space and used as seeds for the probabilistic tractography in Trackvis (Wang et al., 2007). The resulting tractograms were transformed to visitation maps, binarized, and registered to MNI152 space before a percentage overlap map was produced by summarizing each point in the normalized healthy subject visitation maps. The resulting disconnectome maps are whole-brain voxel-wise probability maps indicating for each patient and for each voxel in the brain the probability that the voxels were disconnected. Next, these individual-level disconnection maps were included in group-level analysis.
Statistical analysisTo assess associations between MoCA scores and disconnectome maps, we used nonrotated task-based PLS using PLS Application (Krishnan et al., 2010) for MATLAB (MathWorks, 2018), entering female, age, and MoCA score as behavioral variables, with contrasts for mean effects for each (1 0 0; 0 1 0; 0 0 1). We performed permutations (n = 1000) to assess the significance of the estimated latent variables, while precision was estimated using bootstrapping (n = 1000) and used to calculate pseudo-z or bootstrap ratio brain maps (McIntosh & Lobaugh, 2004).
To assess associations with cognitive performance, disconnectivity within each significant PLS cluster associated with MoCA was extracted and correlated with CabPad performance.
RESULTSFigure 2 displays patient distribution of MoCA-scores. Thirty-five percent of the patients fulfilled criteria for mild cognitive impairment, based on a suggested cutoff at 26 (Nasreddine et al., 2005).
Investigation of association between group and MoCA performance revealed no significant relationship (F = 1.8, p = .17, Figure 3). All participants were therefore pooled for further analysis. Investigation of association between MoCA score and lesion load identified a significant association between total lesion volume and MoCA score (t = −2.96, p = .004), but not with number of lesions (t = −1.502, p = .13). However, after removal of one participant with an extreme lesion size, the association between MoCA and lesion volume did not remain significant (t = −1.7, p = .09). See also Table S1 and Figures S1 and S2 for corresponding statistics and plots.
Figure 4 and Table 2 summarize the results from the PLS. Briefly, permutation testing revealed three significant clusters (pseudo-z > 3) showing common variance between MoCA performance and structural disconnectivity, including (1) right frontal operculum/insular cortex, (2) right superior temporal gyrus, and (3) the right putamen. Of note, association between lesion size and individual PLS brain scores remained highly similar after removal of an individual with an extreme lesion size (included: t = −9.1, p = 7.6e-15, excluded: t = −7.4 p = 5.13e-11), indicating that our main results within the PLS was not strongly driven by the outlier.
FIGURE 4. Voxel-wise disconnectome bootstrap ratio maps for the MoCA association, thresholded at pseudo-z > 3
TABLE 2 Disconnectome clusters significantly associated with MoCA performance, identified by PLS analysis
Cluster # | Location | Size (mm3) | Max pseudo-z | X, Y, Z (vox) |
1 | Right frontal operculum/insula | 1063 | 4.09 | [25, 75, 33] |
2 | Right superior temporal gyrus, posterior division | 273 | 3.65 | [15, 48, 41] |
3 | Right putamen | 37 | 3.61 | [34, 68, 32] |
Figure 5 illustrates correlations between disconnectivity within each of the significant clusters identified by MoCa-PLS and CabPad performance. Disconnectivity was negatively associated with performance on all tests, with strongest correlations for semantic and phonetic fluency.
FIGURE 5. Correlation between MoCA, PLS-weights, disconnectivity within the significant clusters, and CabPad performance
In the current study, we investigated associations between individual structural disconnectome maps and poststroke cognitive performance as measured with MoCA in 102 stroke survivors. We also performed follow-up investigation assessing cognitive associations with a broader cognitive battery on a subsample. Our analysis revealed that structural disconnections implicating the right insula and frontal operculum, along with right superior temporal gyrus and right putamen were associated with poorer general cognitive performance as measured with MoCA. In a subsample comprising 82 of the patients, we demonstrated that the association generalized to a range of cognitive domains, including word generation, attention span, and speed. Together, our results support the relevance of altered structural connectivity on cognitive impairments following stroke.
Stroke frequently causes cognitive impairment, and investigations of the underlying network dynamics may add to our current understanding and prognostics. Structural disconnections caused by stroke have been shown to influence functional connectivity both directly and indirectly (Griffis et al., 2019, 2020). This suggests a mechanism of cognitive sequelae in which altered functional connectivity due to structural disruptions cause aberrant function in distal nodes, where lesions perturbating any part of a network supporting cognitive functions may cause cognitive and behavioral impairment (Alstott et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2019; Lim & Kang, 2015).
A challenge in current healthcare is assessment of cognitive function with a sensitivity and specificity able to detect and differentiate cognitive difficulties (Dong et al., 2010). MoCA (Nasreddine et al., 2005) has been suggested as a feasible screening tool for detecting cognitive impairment following stroke (Horstmann et al., 2014; Julayanont & Nasreddine, 2017; Munthe-Kaas et al., 2021), where key advantages are short administration time and multiple cognitive domains of assessment (Burton & Tyson, 2015; Stolwyk Renerus et al., 2014). Importantly, MoCA is sensitive to specific cognitive domains such as attention, executive, and visuospatial function when utilized in lesion-mapping studies (Shi et al., 2018; Zhao et al., 2017). Of note, poststroke MoCA score has been demonstrated to differentiate cerebral blood flow in anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex (Nakaoku et al., 2018). Anterior cingulate is strongly connected to insula and frontal operculum both structurally (Ghaziri et al., 2017) and functionally (Horn et al., 2010), and is suggested to be among the core nodes of the cingulo-opercular network. Importantly, these network nodes are suggested to mediate regulation and differentiation between the frontoparietal, ventral attention, and default mode networks (Goulden et al., 2014; Menon & Uddin, 2010). The frontoparietal and default mode network are furthermore viewed as anticorrelated networks reflecting cognitive state, where degree of this differentiation have been shown to predict cognitive function in stroke survivors at the group (Geranmayeh et al., 2016) and individual level (Lorenz et al., 2021). Indeed, lesions causing altered connectivity in cingulo-opercular nodes have been linked to cognitive impairment across multiple domains (Siegel et al., 2016; Warren et al., 2014), including general impairments as measured using MoCA (Vicentini et al., 2021).
Our analysis revealed significant associations between MoCA and disconnectivity in the right superior temporal gyrus (STG) and a small cluster in the putamen. STG is commonly associated with language production and interpretation (Brugge et al., 2003) and has also been found to be activated in in conjunction with insula during evaluation of responses in decision-making tasks (Megías et al., 2018; Paulus et al., 2005), where information needs to be integrated and evaluated over longer periods. STG has also been found to coactivate with visual attention, frontoparietal, and cingulo-opercular nodes during reorientation of visual attention (Vossel et al., 2014), suggesting a more general role in attentional processing.
Putamen is involved in various aspects of motor functioning and learning, including language functions and reward signaling. The putamen is connected to thalamic and motor cortices (Jung et al., 2014; Leh et al., 2007) and is strongly connected to the frontoparietal network where the integrity of the connecting pathway is correlated with executive functioning in healthy adults (Bennett & Madden, 2014; Ystad et al., 2011). These results together suggest that poststroke cognitive dysfunction may partly arise from lack of differentiation between large-scale brain networks. Our results align with this understanding, as structural disconnectivity of the insula and operculum was associated with impaired MoCA performance, potentially through disrupted network regulation.
Expanding our results, disconnectivity within the MoCA-associated clusters correlated with performance across several cognitive domains as measured by CabPad. While the apparent lack of cognitive specificity within and across clusters may reflect that the disconnectome approach captures a more overarching impairment, strongest correlations were seen with phonetic and semantic generation and visual attention span. Phonetic and semantic fluency reflect executive functioning beyond pure language generation (Delis et al., 2004), and greater functional connectivity within the cingulo-opercular and ventral attention network has been associated with reading fluency.
Our results revealed significant associations within the right hemisphere only. A recent investigation of structural connectivity in patients with ischemic leukoaraiosis found predominantly right side altered graph metrics when compared to healthy controls, and reported associations with cognitive performance (Lu et al., 2021). Comparably, right side functional abnormalities in cingulo-opercular nodes during resting state have been associated with impaired MoCA performance following a transient ischemic attack (Guo et al., 2014), and right side dominance in bilateral insula activation is robustly found during task engagement in healthy controls (Rottschy et al., 2012). Further, right hemisphere is to a larger degree linked to visual attention, reorientation, and bottom-up processing, and damage to right side pathways associated with impaired spatial attention, target detection and vigilance (Corbetta & Shulman, 2011). Albeit speculative, our results indicate a larger right-side vulnerability for abrupted structural connectivity augmenting poststroke cognitive difficulties.
The current study has limitations. First, our cross-sectional design does not allow for integrating premorbid cognitive function and chronic brain pathology, which is highly relevant as an outcome predictor (Sagnier & Sibon, 2019). Further, the current sample represents a heterogeneous group regarding stroke severity, we lack measures on vascular risk, and the sample size is moderate, factors that are relevant for the generalizability of the reported findings to the stroke population in general (Marek et al., 2020). Of note, our data did not allow for differentiation between stroke reported at hospital admittance and potential older strokes reported in the radiological description. We can therefore not rule out the possibility that the associations are partly driven by previous strokes in some of the patients. Further, jointly estimating all voxels in a single PLS model increases sensitivity, however, comes with a trade-off in spatial specificity as we can only make statements about significance for the spatial pattern as a whole. PLS has shown comparable performance compared to similar multivariate lesion symptom mapping approaches, it has shown to display less anatomical specificity, warranting caution when interpreting spatially sparse clusters (Ivanova et al., 2021). Still, the reliability of each voxel's contribution to the observed pattern was assessed through bootstrapping. While the follow-up association between brain scores and the broader CabPad battery may indicate whether the MoCa-driven PLS-decomposition is driven by general or more specific cognitive domains, it cannot capture novel associations not initially captured by the MoCA. Although MoCA has been shown to display adequate sensitivity and specificity to detect mild cognitive impairment, it does not offer a comprehensive assessment. Indeed, lack of specificity and variability in difficulty increases the likelihood to overlook minor cognitive difficulties. Indeed, MoCA only provides a crude proxy for cognitive impairment and does not allow for drawing interference on broad cognitive abilities. Of note, premorbid cognitive function is associated with poststroke outcome. In the current data collection, we did not obtain measures allowing for estimation of premorbid function. In conclusion, our study supports the relevance of investigating disrupted structural connectivity following stroke. Although our study does not allow differentiation between proximal and distal effects due to sample size and lesion homogeneity, our results highlight the relevance of altered structural connectivity when investigating cognitive sequalae after stroke. In line with previous studies, our results indicate lesions affecting the insula and the frontal operculum are associated with cognitive impairment and support the inclusion of measures of structural disconnection when evaluating cognitive and functional sequelae in stroke patients.
FUNDINGThe current study has been funded by European Research Council under the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (ERC StG, grant agreement No. 818521/ 802998); South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (Grant/Award Numbers: 2014097, 2015044, 2015073, 2019107, 2020086); Norges Forskningsråd (Grant/Award Numbers: 249795, 262372); and Norwegian ExtraFoundation for Health and Rehabilitation (Grant/Award Number: 2015/FO5146).
CONFLICT OF INTERESTWe declare no competing financial interests. We obtained appropriate ethical approval from the local ethics committee, and all procedures were in line with the declaration of Helsinki.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENTRelevant scripts and scrambled and anonymized data needed to reconstruct the reported analyses and results are available on OSF (link below). While the sensitive nature of the data and our current approvals do not allow for public sharing of real data, anonymized data may be available upon request to the corresponding author following appropriate data transfer agreements. OSF-repository:
The peer review history for this article is available at
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2022. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Background and purpose
Sequalae following stroke represents a significant challenge in current rehabilitation. The location and size of focal lesions are only moderately predictive of the diverse cognitive outcome after stroke. One explanation building on recent work on brain networks proposes that the cognitive consequences of focal lesions are caused by damages to anatomically distributed brain networks supporting cognition rather than specific lesion locations.
Methods
To investigate the association between poststroke structural disconnectivity and cognitive performance, we estimated individual level whole‐brain disconnectivity probability maps based on lesion maps from 102 stroke patients using normative data from healthy controls. Cognitive performance was assessed in the whole sample using Montreal Cognitive Assessment, and a more comprehensive computerized test protocol was performed on a subset (n = 82).
Results
Multivariate analysis using Partial Least Squares on the disconnectome maps revealed that higher disconnectivity in right insular and frontal operculum, superior temporal gyrus and putamen was associated with poorer MoCA performance, indicating that lesions in regions connected with these brain regions are more likely to cause cognitive impairment. Furthermore, our results indicated that disconnectivity within these clusters was associated with poorer performance across multiple cognitive domains.
Conclusions
These findings demonstrate that the extent and distribution of structural disconnectivity following stroke are sensitive to cognitive deficits and may provide important clinical information predicting poststroke cognitive sequalae.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details


1 NORMENT, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital & Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Sunnaas Rehabilitation Hospital HT, Nesodden, Norway
2 NORMENT, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital & Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
3 Brain Connectivity and Behaviour Laboratory, Sorbonne Universities, Paris, France; Groupe d'Imagerie Neurofonctionnelle, Institut des Maladies Neurodégénératives—UMR 5293, CNRS, CEA University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
4 NORMENT, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital & Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Centre of Research and Education in Forensic Psychiatry, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
5 NORMENT, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital & Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Faculty of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands; Department of Radiology and Nuclear Medicine, Maastricht University Medical Center, the Netherlands
6 Department of Nephrology, Oslo University Hospital, Ullevål, Norway; Department of Medicine, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway
7 Department of Neurology, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
8 CatoSenteret Rehabilitation Center, Son, Norway; Faculty of Health Sciences, Oslo Metropolitan University, Oslo, Norway
9 NORMENT, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital & Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; KG Jebsen Centre for Neurodevelopmental Disorders, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
10 NORMENT, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital & Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway; Bjørknes College, Oslo, Norway