Introduction
Effort-based decision-making, or choosing when to exert effort to gain rewards, is important for effective behavioral functioning, and is impaired in many psychopathologies including substance use disorders, schizophrenia, posttraumatic stress disorder, and depression [1–5]. Thus, it is important to understand how both abused and medicinal drugs may affect effort-based decision-making in humans. Preclinical studies show effort-based decision-making is most prominently regulated by dopamine [6], and consistent with this, dopaminergic drugs alter effort-based decision-making in humans [7–10]. However, preclinical studies show that opioidergic and cholinergic systems also play a significant role in effort-based decision-making [11, 12]. However, less is known about how opioidergic and cholinergic drugs affect effort-based decision-making in humans.
Opioidergic manipulations might be expected to affect effort because opioidergic “hedonic hotspots” in the nucleus accumbens appear to regulate pleasure or "liking" for rewards [13–16]. Opioidergic agonists, which increase "liking" of reward, might thus increase effort to obtain rewards, while antagonists would be expected to decrease effort. This might be particularly evident over repeated trials as organisms experience altered hedonic “payoffs” for their effort. Consistent with this theory, opioid agonists in rats increase the value of rewards and the effort exerted to obtain rewards [11, 17, 18], while antagonists, such as naltrexone, reduce liking and consumption of rewards [13] as well as willingness to exert effort for rewards [19–22]. The literature in humans is more limited. Studies in healthy humans show that acute administration of the μ-opioid receptor antagonists naloxone and naltrexone reduce preference for high-value food rewards [15, 23] and reduce both self-reported pleasure and activity in the anterior cingulate cortex (an area associated with valuation) to rewarding gambling outcomes [24]. These findings are consistent with reduced pleasure, but do not directly address effort. In healthy humans, acute naltrexone also reduces two indirect measures of effort: it slows reward learning in a decision-making task, which the authors attribute to decreased effort exerted to attend to reward cues [15, 25], and it decreases attention to the information-rich eye region of photos, suggestive of reduced motivation for valuable social cues [26]. Finally, acute naltrexone administration disrupts computations of reward value in a Pavlovian instrumental transfer task in healthy humans [27]. However, all these studies used indirect measures of effort, rather than directly studying effort/reward tradeoffs. Thus, additional research is needed to understand whether opioidergic manipulations directly affect effort-based decision making in humans.
Drugs affecting the nicotinic acetylcholine system would also be expected to affect effort. Here we base our predictions on the "dual reinforcement model" of nicotine addiction. This model proposes that nicotine both serves as a primary reinforcer and enhances the rewarding properties of concurrent non-drug rewards, in part through interactions with the mesolimbic dopamine systems [28, 29]. If nicotine increases the rewarding properties of non-drug rewards, we would expect nicotine to enhance effort for these rewards as well. However, here the rodent literature presents mixed findings. Some studies find that nicotine increases operant responding with visual rewards [30], whereas others find nicotine either has no effect or even decreases effort for food rewards [31]. In humans, the question of whether nicotine enhances effort for rewards has often been studied in dependent smokers [32–34], but these results are complicated by receptor adaptations, tolerance and the reward-blunting effects of nicotine withdrawal. However, there is evidence that in non-nicotine-dependent individuals acute nicotine administration increases responding for sensory rewards (i.e., audio, olfactory, taste, visual stimuli), but not for non-sensory rewards (i.e., money) [29, 35]. The neuropharmacological mechanism behind this sensory/non-sensory distinction is unclear, and the question of whether nicotine specifically affects effort to obtain rewards has not been addressed using a standardized effort-based decision-making task.
Here, we tested the acute effects of naltrexone and nicotine on effort expenditure in two separate studies. We used the Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task (EEfRT), which has previously been sensitive to dopamine manipulations in humans [7, 36, 37]. In Study 1, we compared naltrexone (25mg and 50mg) to placebo. We hypothesized naltrexone would reduce willingness to exert effort for rewards. We also hypothesized this effect would be more evident in later trials, after participants had repeatedly experienced lower pleasure from rewards. In Study 2, a pilot study, we compared nicotine (7mg) to placebo. We hypothesized nicotine would increase willingness to exert effort for rewards. We also hypothesized nicotine would have a greater effect on willingness to exert effort for rewards in trials with lower reward probabilities, because, in previous studies, willingness to exert effort has been particularly pronounced for trials when reward probability was lower [7, 37].
Materials and methods
Manipulation checks
We assessed mood and physiological effects of the drugs to confirm that they produced their expected effects. In both studies, we measured subjective effects of naltrexone and nicotine with the Drug Effects Questionnaire (DEQ) [38], which contains items asking the extent to which participants felt the drug, liked the drug, and disliked the drug. We assessed cardiovascular effects on heart rate and blood pressure (diastolic and systolic) [39]. In Study 1, we also measured subjective effects with the Profile of Mood States (POMS) [40]. Naltrexone manipulation checks were conducted at baseline, 30 min. post-administration, 75 min. post-administration, 180 min. post-administration, and 210 min. post-administration. Nicotine manipulation checks were measured at baseline, 60 min. post-administration, 100 min. post-administration, and 160 min. post-administration.
Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task (EEfRT)
The EEfRT measures effort-based decision-making [36] via a computerized game consisting of 50 trials. In each trial, participants are prompted to choose either an easy task or a hard task to earn monetary rewards of varying amounts. During the choice period, individuals are presented with the probability of the trial being a “win” trial (12, 50, or 88%) and the amount of money they could earn for the hard task ($1.24-$4.20). Easy tasks are always worth $1. Participants can earn money if they successfully complete the trial, and the trial is a “win” trial. “No win” trials result in $0. Participants then choose which task they will attempt and begin the task. To successfully complete the easy task, participants must press a key with their dominant index finger 30 times in 7 seconds. To successfully complete the hard task, participants must press a key with their non-dominant hand’s little finger 100 times in 21 seconds. After the attempt is complete, participants receive feedback informing them how much money they won during the trial. At the end of the task, participants received monetary rewards from two, randomly selected, “win” trials that they successfully completed. The primary outcome variable modeled is whether the participants selected the high cost/high reward option over the low cost/low reward option on a given trial.
For both studies, subjects were excluded if they failed to complete 40 trials in one session, if they always chose the easy or hard task, only completed “high value” trials (this is a strategy that can artificially increase payouts on this task) or if they did not consistently choose the hard task more as probability of reward increased (indicating non-systematic responding). This exclusion criteria is similar to that of previous studies which used the EEfRT [37]. One participant was excluded in Study 1, and 3 participants were excluded in Study 2.
Study 1 naltrexone
Overall design.
These data were obtained as part of a larger study that was examining the effect of naltrexone on processing of social and emotional stimuli [41]. The study used a three-session, within-subjects design in which healthy volunteers received oral placebo, 25mg or 50mg naltrexone in a counterbalanced order under double-blind conditions. Sessions were separated by at least 1 week, and the EEfRT was completed 90 minutes after drug administration, during the expected peak effect for naltrexone. During the interval between administration and EEfRT, subjects were permitted to relax and watch a movie or read a book, but they were not allowed to engage in work. The EEfRT was completed as part of a 1.5hr battery of social/emotional tasks, which were administered in randomized order. Results of the other tasks are reported elsewhere [41]. Subjects completed rating scales and cardiovascular measures were obtained at regular intervals during the session.
Participants.
Thirty-three healthy adults (18–35 years old) participated (Table 1). They were screened with a physical examination, electrocardiogram, modified structured clinical interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV) [42], and self-reported drug and health history. Criteria for inclusion were a high school education with English fluency and body mass index (BMI) of 19–30. Exclusion criteria included regular use of prescription medications (except oral contraceptives), medical contraindication to naltrexone, prior negative reactions to naltrexone, regular use of opioid drugs, past-year DSM-IV Axis I diagnosis (except Nicotine Dependence), and women who were pregnant or planning to become pregnant. Participants abstained from alcohol and medications (except hormonal contraceptives) for 24 hours before sessions and abstained from other drugs (except caffeine and nicotine) for 48 hours before sessions. They were instructed to fast for 2 hours. Compliance was verified using self-report, plus breath (Alcosensor III, Intoximeters Inc., St. Louis, MO) and urine tests (ToxCup, Branan Medical Corporation, Irvine, CA) for alcohol and drugs. Women not on hormonal contraceptives were scheduled during the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle [43]. To reduce expectancy effects, individuals were told they might receive a stimulant, a sedative, a cannabinoid, an opioid antagonist, or a placebo.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Participants provided written informed consent, and procedures were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the University of Chicago Institutional Review Board.
Analytic approach.
All analyses were mixed models performed in R (Version 3.5.2; [44]) using the lme4 package (Version 1.1–21; [45]). We mean centered all continuous independent variables and contrast coded categorical independent variables. To establish the random effects models, we created a maximal model and iteratively reduced it according to Bates, Kliegl, Vasishth, and Baayen using the RePsychLing package [46, 47]. We used the emmeans package to conduct follow-up tests on significant main effects and interactions (Version 1.5.2–1; [48])
For naltrexone, the “expected effects” used as manipulation checks were subjective fatigue, subjective feeling of a drug effect, and drug disliking. These effects are typical of naltrexone and were all observed in the primary analysis of this data, with only one additional participant excluded here [41]. Subjective (POMS: Fatigue, DEQ: Feel Drug, and DEQ: Dislike Drug) effects were modeled using a linear mixed effects model (LMM), with fixed effects for Drug (0, 25, 50mg), Time (pre-capsule, 30 min. post-capsule, 75 min. post-capsule, 180 min. post-capsule, 210 min. post-capsule), and their interactions. We conducted parallel analyses of heart rate and blood pressure effects for both drugs but did not expect strong effects of naltrexone on these outcomes. These were modeled using a linear mixed effects model (LMM), with fixed effects for Drug, Time (pre-capsule, 30 min. post-capsule, 75 min. post-capsule, 180 min. post-capsule, 210 min. post-capsule), and their interactions. Omnibus tests were conducted on the models using analysis of variance (ANOVA) Type III based on Satterthwaite’s method.
To test the effect of naltrexone on choice in the EEfRT task, we used a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with a logit link function for the binomial outcome (choice of easy vs. hard task). Fixed effects were linear and quadratic effects of Naltrexone Dose (0, 25, 50mg), Reward Amount ($1.24-$4.20), linear and quadratic effects of Probability (12, 50, 88%), and their interactions. Fixed effects also included linear and quadratic effects of Session (1, 2, 3), Trial Number (1–50) and its interaction with Naltrexone dose (linear and quadratic), to test the effect of naltrexone on willingness to exert effort across the trials. Omnibus tests were conducted on the model using Type III Wald chi-square tests. Continuous variables of interest were grand-mean centered and converted to Z-scores for all analyses.
Study 2 nicotine
Overall design.
To study the effects of nicotine on willingness to exert effort, we conducted a pilot study and used a two-session, within-subjects design. At each session, healthy, light-smoking volunteers received a transdermal patch containing inactive placebo or 7 mg nicotine (Nicoderm CQ, Glaxo Smith Kline) under double-blind conditions. Sessions were separated by at least 24 hours, and the EEfRT was completed 60 minutes after patch administration. At this time, participants’ plasma nicotine concentrations are expected to be approximately 5ng/ml [49]. We collected heart rate and blood pressure (diastolic and systolic) over the course of each session (Baseline, 60 min. [EEfRT], 100 min., 160 min.) to check that the nicotine manipulation was effective.
Participants.
We used the same participant recruitment strategies and assessment procedures as described in Study 1. Participants were 15 healthy adults (19–30 years old), who reported smoking fewer than five cigarettes per day to minimize effects of tolerance. Exclusion criteria were the same as Study 1. Participants attended a 1-hour orientation session and two 3-hour study sessions. Participants were asked to fast the night before each study session and to abstain from alcohol and substance use (except for their typical caffeine intake) from 24 hours prior to the first study session until after the second study session was completed. At each study session, participants provided urine and breath samples to confirm that they had complied with the study’s guidelines. Similar to study 1, all participants provided written informed consent and all procedures were conducted in accordance with the University of Chicago Institutional Review Board.
Analytic approach.
All analyses were again mixed models performed in R, using the same packages, coding and random effects models approaches outlined for Study 1.
We conducted parallel analyses of subjective effects for both drugs, but for nicotine we did not expect strong subjective effects in our population and dose. Subjective (DEQ: Feel Drug, and DEQ: Dislike Drug) effects were modeled using a linear mixed effects model (LMM), with fixed effects for Drug, Time (Baseline, 60 min. [EEfRT], 100 min., 160 min.), and their interactions. Instead, for nicotine, our “expected effects” used as manipulation checks were increases in heart rate and blood pressure. These were modeled using a linear mixed effects model (LMM), with fixed effects for Drug, Time (Baseline, 60 min. [EEfRT], 100 min., 160 min.) and their interactions. Omnibus tests were conducted on the models using analysis of variance (ANOVA) Type III based on Satterthwaite’s method.
To test the effect of nicotine on participants’ willingness to exert effort for monetary rewards, we used generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) with a logit link function for the binomial outcome (choice of easy or hard task). Fixed effects were Nicotine Dose (0, 7mg), Reward Amount for the hard task ($1.24-$4.20), linear and quadratic effects of Probability of receiving a reward (12, 50, 88%), and their interactions. The Probability x Amount interaction represents the expected value of a reward. Additionally, we included fixed effects Trial Number (0–50) and Session (1, 2) to account for fatigue and practice effects over the course of the task or sessions based on previous studies [50]. Omnibus tests were conducted on the model using Type III Wald chi-square tests. Continuous variables of interest were grand-mean centered and converted to Z-scores for all analyses.
Results
Study 1 naltrexone
Manipulation checks.
Subjective effects of naltrexone. Compared to placebo, naltrexone significantly increased reports of feeling the drug before the task (at 75 min. post capsule), after the task (at 180 min.), and at the final time point (210 min.); Drug x Time interaction, F(8, 312.67) = 3.82, p = < 0.001. Compared to placebo, both 25mg and 50mg doses of naltrexone also increased reports of disliking a drug effect immediately after the task (at 180 min.) and at the final time point (210 min.); Drug x Time interaction, F(8, 318.19) = 3.79, p < 0.001. Finally, compared to placebo, both doses of naltrexone also increased reports of fatigue immediately after the task (at 180 min.) and at the final time point (210 min.), with 50mg also elevating reports of fatigue immediately before the task (75 min.); Drug x Time interaction, F(8, 318.42) = 3.65, p < 0.001. This suggests the typical effects of naltrexone were present and measurable in our sample at the time of the effort-based decision-making task.
Cardiovascular effects of naltrexone. Consistent with our expectations, there was no significant effect of naltrexone on systolic blood pressure F(2, 62.61) = 0.08, p = 0.92, diastolic blood pressure F(2, 31.92) = 1.12, p = 0.34, or heart rate F(2, 31.98) = 0.51, p = 0.60. See S1 Table for full results.
EEfRT.
Our two hypotheses for naltrexone were assessed using Type III Wald tests for our binomial mixed-effects regression model with choice of the hard task as the dependent variable. Full results are provided in Table 2.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
The analysis showed no significant effect of naltrexone on choice of the hard task (Wald X2(2, N = 34) = 0.98, p = 0.61). See Fig 1.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Estimated marginal means (EMM) are plotted with standard errors (SE) from mixed-effects model (0mg EMM = 0.19, SE = 0.07; naltrexone (25mg) EMM = 0.16, SE = 0.06; naltrexone (50mg) EMM = 0.15, SE = 0.06). No significant effect of naltrexone on choice of hard task (p = 0.61).
Furthermore, although all participants tended to choose the hard task less often in later trials (Wald X2(1, N = 34) = 38.69, p < 0.001), naltrexone did not accelerate this tendency, as there was also no significant interaction between trial number and naltrexone dose (Wald X2(2, N = 34) = 1.53, p = 0.46; Fig 2).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Estimated marginal means are plotted with standard errors shaded from mixed-effects model. No significant interaction between trial number and naltrexone dose (p = 0.46).
As expected, as the amount of money that individuals could win for successfully completing the hard task increased, participants chose the hard task more frequently (Wald X2(1, N = 34) = 156.61, p < 0.001. Likewise, participants chose the hard task more often when the probability of receiving the monetary reward increased (Wald X2(2, N = 34) = 106.31, p < 0.001). Finally, there was a significant interaction between probability and amount such that higher expected values were related to increased choices of the hard task (Wald X2(2, N = 34) = 61.37, p < 0.001). Together this all suggests participants attended to the task and their responses were typically sensitive to variations in standard parameters.
Study 2 nicotine
Manipulation checks.
Subjective effects of nicotine. Consistent with our expectations, nicotine did not significantly increase reports of feeling a drug effect F(1, 70) = 3.54, p = 0.06, or reports of disliking a drug effect F(1, 70) = 0.47, p = 0.50.
Cardiovascular effects of nicotine. Cardiovascular effects were consistent with our expectations. Compared to placebo, nicotine significantly increased participants’ systolic blood pressure F(1, 91) = 14.61, p < 0.001, and diastolic blood pressure F(3, 89.98) = 3.13, p < 0.05 before the task (at 60 min.) and after the task (at 100 min.). Nicotine also increased participants’ heart rate after the task (at 100 min.) and at the final time point (at 160 min.); F(3, 89.03) = 4.28, p < 0.01. See S2 Table for full results. Again, this suggests that typical effects of the drug were present and measurable in our sample at the time of the effort-based decision-making task.
EEfRT.
Similar to our naltrexone analysis, our hypotheses for nicotine were assessed using Type III Wald tests for our binomial mixed-effects regression model with choice for the hard task as the dependent variable. Full results are provided in Table 3.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Nicotine did not significantly affect choice of the hard task (Wald X2(1, N = 15) = 0.03, p = 0.86). See Fig 3. Also, contrary to our hypothesis, there were no significant interactions between drug, probability, and amount (Wald X2(2, N = 15) = 0.81, p = 0.67).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Estimated marginal means are plotted with standard errors from mixed-effects model (Placebo EMM = 0.56, SE = 0.14; Nicotine EMM = 0.55, SE = 0.15). No significant effect of nicotine dose on choice of hard task (p = 0.86).
There was a significant interaction between drug and probability (Wald X2(2, N = 15) = 7.02, p = 0.03), suggesting that nicotine had a larger effect on choice of the hard task at medium probability. However, post hoc tests revealed no significant differences between nicotine and placebo on choice of the hard task at low, medium, or high probability. As expected, participants chose the hard task less frequently on later trials (Wald X2(1, N = 15) = 19.39, p < 0.001). As the reward amount for successfully completing the hard task increased, participants selection of the hard task also increased (Wald X2(1, N = 15) = 32.73, p < 0.001), and selection of the hard task also increased with increasing probability of winning the reward (Wald X2(2, N = 15) = 73.61, p < 0.001). Lastly, there was a significant interaction between probability and amount such that higher expected values were related to increased choices of the hard task (Wald X2(2, N = 15) = 23.13, p < 0.001). Together this all suggests participants attended to the task and their responses were typically sensitive to variations in standard parameters.
Discussion
These two studies aimed to determine the acute effects of naltrexone and nicotine on willingness to exert effort for rewards in healthy adults using the EEfRT. Contrary to our hypotheses, neither naltrexone nor nicotine affected individuals’ willingness to exert effort for monetary rewards, even though both drugs produced characteristic effects on other subjective and cardiovascular measures. The results contrast with studies using dopaminergic medications, which affect motivation for effort-based decision making in humans, including on this same task [7–9, 51]. Our results do suggest the participants attended to the task, and that the task sensitively captured a range of reward-related decisions, as we saw typical effects of greater reward magnitudes and higher probability of receiving the reward increasing effort expenditure. They further suggest that the drug doses were adequate, as we saw significant characteristic drug effects on other measures. However, we did not see strong effects of either drug at any level of reward or at any point during the task.
Results from the naltrexone study differed from our expectations based on the prior literature on naltrexone in humans. We expected opioid blockade via naltrexone administration to reduce participants’ willingness to exert effort for rewards, particularly later in the task as participants accumulated experience with the hedonic impact of the rewards. Previous literature showed that naltrexone administration decreased the liking of food rewards and also decreased effort (i.e., lever presses) for drug rewards in rodents [21, 22, 52]. In humans, there were indications that naltrexone would play a similar role, as it slowed reward learning in decision-making tasks and reduced pleasure related to rewarding gambling outcomes [15, 24]. However, we saw neither an overall effect of naltrexone, nor a decrease over trials that could be related to learning about the reduced hedonic value of rewards. The results from our naltrexone study may have differed from our expectations and from previous literature due to the way effort was operationalized. Although previous studies in humans used the same naltrexone dose (50mg), these studies operationalized effort exerted indirectly, as attention allocated during a task or speed of learning. On the other hand, the EEfRT operationalizes effort exertion more directly, as selection for high cost/high rewards, which requires more key presses, over low cost/low rewards. Thus, the effect of naltrexone on willingness to exert effort does not appear to be strong or direct when measured explicitly, although this does not fully rule out more subtle effects that might require a larger sample or different measure to detect.
Regarding nicotine, we expected nicotine to increase participants’ willingness to exert effort for monetary rewards in our pilot study, based on the dual reinforcement theory. In non-human animal models, nicotine amplified the reinforcing value of rewards [28]. However, in humans, nicotine administration has impacted behavioral responding for sensory rewards only in some prior studies [29]. Our results are consistent with the idea that nicotine may only, or may more strongly, impact effort for sensory rewards, as we saw no strong effect on willingness to exert effort for a monetary reward here. Another potential explanation as to why we did not see an effect of nicotine on effort for monetary rewards may be due to sample differences. Some studies that did suggest nicotine enhancement of responsiveness for financial rewards only found this effect among individuals who were heavy smokers (i.e., reported smoking 15 or more cigarettes per day) [32]. However, in our sample, we only included light-smoking non-dependent individuals who reported smoking fewer than five cigarettes per day. Other studies that have included a similar sample to ours (i.e., reported smoking less than five cigarettes per day) also found no reinforcing effect of nicotine on money [35]. It is possible that the dual reinforcing effects of nicotine are stronger or more generalized in individuals who smoke more heavily, or that the effects on monetary rewards seen in these prior studies represented withdrawal relief rather than true enhancement.
These studies had several limitations. For both studies, the drug dosages were fixed, not based on individual bodyweight and body mass composition. However, the doses used for naltrexone are typical of those used clinically in treatment, while the dose of nicotine is a typical dose used for nicotine replacement therapy, suggesting these doses and results have real-world relevance in terms of the potential for effort-related side effects of these drugs. Second, due to the limitations of the dose response design in the parent study [41], we only tested the effects of an opioid antagonist and not an agonist. Future studies examining opioid agonists in relation to effort-based decision-making may provide additional insight into the role of the opioid system in this aspect of reward functioning. Third, in our naltrexone study, willingness to exert effort was lower than expected compared to other studies on the EEfRT [37], including our pilot study on nicotine, which may be related to unmeasured differences in the study settings or samples. Future studies examining the relationship between naltrexone and willingness to exert effort should consider replicating the laboratory conditions used in other EEfRT studies. Fourth, we did not collect baseline measures of effort. Previous studies in laboratory animals have suggested that baseline differences may determine the degree to which pharmacological manipulations change effort for rewards, including for nicotine [31]. We also did not directly contrast sensory and non-sensory rewards. Future studies of pharmacological manipulations should consider simultaneously investigating impacts on sensory rewards vs. non-sensory rewards to investigate this intriguing difference observed in the nicotine literature. Fifth, we did not collect measures of other variables that may help further understand effort-based decision-making, such as acute motivational state and trait characteristics. Future studies should consider including measures of these variables. Finally, our sample sizes were small, although consistent with the sample sizes needed to detect the effects of other pharmacological manipulations on this measure (e.g., 7,41), and adequate to demonstrate typical effects of the drugs on other measures. For naltrexone, a small observed effect size (d = 0.26) between placebo and 25mg and between placebo and 50mg (d = 0.24) indicate a study with N = 139 would be necessary to detect a significant effect of naltrexone dose. For nicotine, the very small effect size (d = 0.09) between placebo and nicotine in our pilot study suggests a study with N = 876 would be needed to detect a significant effect of nicotine. Thus, it seems unlikely that these effects would be strongly meaningful even if they might be detectable with a larger sample. Nonetheless, since our nicotine was a pilot study, it provides preliminary information, and it may be beneficial to conduct additional studies on nicotine and willingness to exert effort.
Overall, our analyses suggest neither naltrexone nor nicotine strongly impact effort exerted for monetary rewards. Our findings have some implications for the use of naltrexone for treatment of opioid use disorder. Although there has been some concern that naltrexone treatment of opioid use disorder could increase anhedonia [53], our results suggest this is not a major concern and are consistent with survey research with patients receiving the drug as treatment [53, 54]. Our nicotine findings also add to our understanding of the mechanisms by which nicotine may become addictive and may inform treatment interventions. Previous literature suggests nicotine primarily impacts the reinforcing effects of sensory rewards (i.e., audio, olfactory, taste, visual stimuli) and not non-sensory rewards (i.e., money) [29], which our findings suggested. Ultimately, this may be helpful for designing interventions to assist individuals who are attempting to quit smoking, as lapses may be more common in the context of sensory rewarding activities, while monetary rewards may be more consistently valued. This possibility is consistent with the efficacy of interventions such as contingency management, which offers monetary rewards to motivate cessation. In summary, despite studies in laboratory animals showing that opioidergic and nicotinergic systems influence effort-based decision-making, two commonly used substances in humans that affect these systems did not strongly affect willingness to exert effort for rewards at doses typically used in humans.
Supporting information
S1 Table. Naltrexone manipulation checks.
Omnibus Effects for Feel Drug on the Drug Effectiveness Questionnaire (DEQ), Dislike Drug on the DEQ, Fatigue on the Profile of Mood States, Systolic Blood Pressure, Diastolic Blood Pressure, and Heart Rate.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0275027.s001
(DOCX)
S2 Table. Nicotine manipulation checks.
Omnibus Effects for Feel Drug on the Drug Effectiveness Questionnaire (DEQ), Dislike Drug on the DEQ, Systolic Blood Pressure, Diastolic Blood Pressure, and Heart Rate.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0275027.s002
(DOCX)
Citation: Nunez C, Hoots JK, Schepers ST, Bower M, de Wit H, Wardle MC (2022) Pharmacological investigations of effort-based decision-making in humans: Naltrexone and nicotine. PLoS ONE 17(10): e0275027. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0275027
About the Authors:
Cecilia Nunez
Roles: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, University of Illinois Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1198-3885
Jennifer K. Hoots
Roles: Formal analysis, Writing – original draft
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, University of Illinois Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America
Scott T. Schepers
Roles: Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America
Michael Bower
Roles: Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America
Harriet de Wit
Roles: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America
Margaret C. Wardle
Roles: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, University of Illinois Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6071-6105
1. Galandra C, Basso G, Cappa S, Canessa N. The alcoholic brain: neural bases of impaired reward-based decision-making in alcohol use disorders [Internet]. Vol. 39, Neurological Sciences. 2018 [cited 2021 Mar 20]. p. 423–35. Available from: pmid:29188399
2. Sharma A, Wolf DH, Ciric R, Kable JW, Moore TM, Vandekar SN, et al. Common dimensional reward deficits across mood and psychotic disorders: A connectome-wide association study. Am J Psychiatry [Internet]. 2017 Jul 31 [cited 2021 Mar 20];174(7):657–66. Available from: pmid:28135847
3. Vujanovic AA, Wardle MC, Smith LJ, Berenz EC. Reward functioning in posttraumatic stress and substance use disorders. Curr Opin Psychol [Internet]. 2017;14:49–55. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28813319
4. Gold JM, Strauss GP, Waltz JA, Robinson BM, Brown JK, Frank MJ. Negative Symptoms of Schizophrenia Are Associated with Abnormal Effort-Cost Computations. Biol Psychiatry [Internet]. 2013 Jul 15 [cited 2018 Dec 21];74(2):130–6. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0006322313000358 pmid:23394903
5. Whitton AE, Treadway MT, Pizzagalli DA. Reward processing dysfunction in major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry [Internet]. 2015 Jan [cited 2021 Mar 30];28(1):7–12. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25415499
6. Salamone JD, Correa M, Yang JH, Rotolo R, Presby R. Dopamine, Effort-Based Choice, and Behavioral Economics: Basic and Translational Research. Front Behav Neurosci [Internet]. 2018 Mar 23 [cited 2018 Aug 14];12:52. Available from: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00052/full pmid:29628879
7. Wardle MC, Treadway MT, Mayo LM, Zald DH, de Wit H. Amping up effort: effects of d-amphetamine on human effort-based decision-making. J Neurosci [Internet]. 2011 Nov 16 [cited 2018 Apr 27];31(46):16597–602. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22090487
8. Zenon A, Devesse S, Olivier E. Dopamine Manipulation Affects Response Vigor Independently of Opportunity Cost. J Neurosci [Internet]. 2016 Sep 14 [cited 2018 May 11];36(37):9516–25. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27629704
9. Cawley E, Park S, aan het Rot M, Sancton K, Benkelfat C, Young S, et al. Dopamine and light: dissecting effects on mood and motivational states in women with subsyndromal seasonal affective disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci [Internet]. 2013 Nov 1 [cited 2018 May 11];38(6):388–97. Available from: http://jpn.ca/vol38-issue6/38-6-388/ pmid:23735584
10. Venugopalan V V, Casey KF, O’Hara C, O’Loughlin J, Benkelfat C, Fellows LK, et al. Acute phenylalanine/tyrosine depletion reduces motivation to smoke cigarettes across stages of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology [Internet]. 2011 Nov 20 [cited 2019 Apr 11];36(12):2469–76. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2011.135 pmid:21775977
11. van Steenbergen H, Eikemo M, Leknes S. The role of the opioid system in decision making and cognitive control: A review. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci [Internet]. 2019 Jun 8 [cited 2021 Mar 3];19(3):435–58. Available from: pmid:30963411
12. Fobbs WC, Mizumori SJY. Cost-benefit decision circuitry: Proposed modulatory role for acetylcholine. In: Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science [Internet]. Elsevier B.V.; 2014 [cited 2021 Mar 3]. p. 233–61. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24484704/
13. Barbano MF, Cador M. Opioids for hedonic experience and dopamine to get ready for it. Psychopharmacology (Berl) [Internet]. 2007 Mar 2 [cited 2021 Mar 24];191(3):497–506. Available from: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00213-006-0521-1
14. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Pleasure Systems in the Brain [Internet]. Vol. 86, Neuron. Cell Press; 2015 [cited 2018 Aug 16]. p. 646–64. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0896627315001336
15. Eikemo M, Biele G, Willoch F, Thomsen L, Leknes S. Opioid Modulation of Value-Based Decision-Making in Healthy Humans. Neuropsychopharmacology [Internet]. 2017 Aug 15 [cited 2019 Sep 4];42(9):1833–40. Available from: http://www.nature.com/articles/npp201758 pmid:28294136
16. Laurent V, Morse AK, Balleine BW. The role of opioid processes in reward and decision-making. Br J Pharmacol [Internet]. 2015 Jan [cited 2020 Nov 18];172(2):449–59. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24930675
17. Zhang M, Balmadrid C, Kelley AE. Nucleus accumbens opioid, GABaergic, and dopaminergic modulation of palatable food motivation: contrasting effects revealed by a progressive ratio study in the rat. Behav Neurosci [Internet]. 2003 Apr [cited 2021 May 26];117(2):202–11. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12708516
18. Stratford TR, Wirtshafter D. Effects of muscimol, amphetamine, and DAMGO injected into the nucleus accumbens shell on food-reinforced lever pressing by undeprived rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav [Internet]. 2012 May 1 [cited 2021 May 26];101(3):499–503. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0091305712000524?via%3Dihub pmid:22366216
19. Barbano MF, Le Saux M, Cador M. Involvement of dopamine and opioids in the motivation to eat: Influence of palatability, homeostatic state, and behavioral paradigms. Psychopharmacology (Berl) [Internet]. 2009 [cited 2021 May 26];203(3):475–87. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19015837/
20. Morales I, Font L, Currie PJ, Pastor R. Involvement of opioid signaling in food preference and motivation: Studies in laboratory animals. Prog Brain Res [Internet]. 2016 Jan 1 [cited 2021 May 11];229:159–87. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0079612316300620 pmid:27926437
21. Bossert JM, Hoots JK, Fredriksson I, Adhikary S, Zhang M, Venniro M, et al. Role of mu, but not delta or kappa, opioid receptors in context‐induced reinstatement of oxycodone seeking. Eur J Neurosci [Internet]. 2019 Aug [cited 2020 Nov 18];50(3):2075–85. Available from: pmid:29779230
22. Burattini C, Gill TM, Aicardi G, Janak PH. The ethanol self-administration context as a reinstatement cue: Acute effects of naltrexone. Neuroscience [Internet]. 2006;139(3):877–87. Available from: pmid:16516392
23. Eikemo M, Løseth GE, Johnstone T, Gjerstad J, Willoch F, Leknes S. Sweet taste pleasantness is modulated by morphine and naltrexone. Psychopharmacology (Berl) [Internet]. 2016 Oct;233(21–22):3711–23. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27538675/
24. Petrovic P, Pleger B, Seymour B, Kloppel S, De Martino B, Critchley H, et al. Blocking Central Opiate Function Modulates Hedonic Impact and Anterior Cingulate Response to Rewards and Losses. J Neurosci [Internet]. 2008 Oct 15 [cited 2019 Sep 4];28(42):10509–16. Available from: http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2807-08.2008 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18923027
25. Chelnokova O, Laeng B, Eikemo M, Riegels J, Løseth G, Maurud H, et al. Rewards of beauty: The opioid system mediates social motivation in humans [Internet]. Vol. 19, Molecular Psychiatry. Nature Publishing Group; 2014. p. 746–7. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2014.1
26. Chelnokova O, Laeng B, Løseth G, Eikemo M, Willoch F, Leknes S. The μ-opioid system promotes visual attention to faces and eyes. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci [Internet]. 2016;11(12):1902–9. Available from: pmid:27531386
27. Weber SC, Beck-Schimmer B, Kajdi ME, Müller D, Tobler PN, Quednow BB. Dopamine D2/3- and μ-opioid receptor antagonists reduce cue-induced responding and reward impulsivity in humans. Transl Psychiatry [Internet]. 2016 Jul 5 [cited 2018 May 3];6(7):e850. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27378550
28. Caggiula AR, Donny EC, Palmatier MI, Liu X, Chaudhri N, Sved AF, et al. The role of nicotine in smoking: A dual-reinforcement model. In: Bevins RA, Caggiula AR, editors. The motivational impact of nicotine and its role in tobacco use [Internet]. New York, NY, US: Springer Science and Business Media; 2009. p. 91–109. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19013940/
29. Perkins KA, Karelitz JL, Boldry MC. Nicotine acutely enhances reinforcement from non-drug rewards in humans. Front Psychiatry [Internet]. 2017;8(MAY). Available from: pmid:28507522
30. Chaudhri N, Caggiula AR, Donny EC, Palmatier MI, Liu X, Sved AF. Complex interactions between nicotine and nonpharmacological stimuli reveal multiple roles for nicotine in reinforcement. In: Psychopharmacology [Internet]. Springer; 2006. p. 353–66. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16240165/
31. Hosking JG, Lam FCW, Winstanley CA. Nicotine Increases Impulsivity and Decreases Willingness to Exert Cognitive Effort despite Improving Attention in “Slacker” Rats: Insights into Cholinergic Regulation of Cost/Benefit Decision Making. Abreu-Villaça Y, editor. PLoS One [Internet]. 2014 Oct;9(10):e111580. Available from: pmid:25353339
32. Dawkins L, Powell JH, West R, Powell J, Pickering A. A double-blind placebo controlled experimental study of nicotine: I—Effects on incentive motivation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) [Internet]. 2006;189(3):355–67. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17047930/
33. Al-Adawi S, Powell J. The influence of smoking on reward responsiveness and cognitive functions: a natural experiment. Addiction [Internet]. 1997 Dec;92(12):1773–82. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9581009/
34. Powell J, Dawkins L, Davis RE. Smoking, reward responsiveness, and response inhibition: tests of an incentive motivational model. Biol Psychiatry [Internet]. 2002 Jan 15 [cited 2021 Mar 2];51(2):151–63. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0006322301012082?via%3Dihub pmid:11822994
35. Perkins KA, Karelitz JL. Reinforcement enhancing effects of nicotine via smoking. Psychopharmacology (Berl) [Internet]. 2013 Aug [cited 2021 May 28];228(3):479–86. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23494236
36. Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, Schwartzman AN, Lambert WE, Zald DH. Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task as an Objective Measure of Motivation and Anhedonia. García AV, editor. PLoS One [Internet]. 2009 Aug 12 [cited 2018 Mar 20];4(8):e6598. Available from: pmid:19672310
37. Soder HE, Cooper JA, Lopez-Gamundi P, Hoots JK, Nunez C, Lawlor VM, et al. Dose-response effects of d-amphetamine on effort-based decision-making and reinforcement learning. Neuropsychopharmacology [Internet]. 2020 Jul 28 [cited 2021 Mar 2];1–8. Available from: pmid:32722661
38. Fischman MW, Foltin RW. Utility of subjective‐effects measurements in assessing abuse liability of drugs in humans. Br J Addict [Internet]. 1991 Dec [cited 2021 Mar 24];86(12):1563–70. Available from: pmid:1786488
39. Wolk R, Shamsuzzaman ASM, Svatikova A, Huyber CM, Huck C, Narkiewicz K, et al. Hemodynamic and autonomic effects of smokeless tobacco in healthy young men. J Am Coll Cardiol [Internet]. 2005 Mar 15 [cited 2020 Dec 15];45(6):910–4. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15766828/
40. de Wit H, Griffiths RR. Testing the abuse liability of anxiolytic and hypnotic drugs in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend [Internet]. 1991 [cited 2020 Jul 8];28(1):83–111. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/1679388/
41. Wardle MC, Bershad AK, De Wit H. Naltrexone alters the processing of social and emotional stimuli in healthy adults. Soc Neurosci [Internet]. 2016;11(6):579–91. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4958027/ pmid:26710657
42. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB. Strutured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders. New York: Biometrics Research Department; 1996.
43. Roche DJO, King AC. Sex differences in acute hormonal and subjective response to naltrexone: The impact of menstrual cycle phase. Psychoneuroendocrinology [Internet]. 2015 [cited 2020 Mar 25];52(1):59–71. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4482338/ pmid:25459893
44. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2018. Available from: https://www.r-project.org/
45. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015 Oct 1;67(1).
46. Bates DM, Kliegl R, Vasishth S, Baayen H. Parsimonious mixed models. ArXiv e-print, under Revis [Internet]. 2015 [cited 2018 Feb 13];1–27. Available from: https://arxiv.org/pdf/1506.04967.pdf
47. Baayen H, Bates D, Kliegl R, Vasishth S. RePsychLing: Data sets from Psychology and Linguistics experiments. R Packag version 004. 2015;
48. Lenth R. emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. R package version 1.4. 2019.
49. Fant R V, Henningfield JE, Shiffman S, Strahs KR, Reitberg DP. A pharmacokinetic crossover study to compare the absorption characteristics of three transdermal nicotine patches. Pharmacol Biochem Behav [Internet]. 2000 Nov [cited 2021 Mar 30];67(3):479–82. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11164075
50. Wardle MC, Treadway MT, de Wit H. Caffeine increases psychomotor performance on the effort expenditure for rewards task. Pharmacol Biochem Behav [Internet]. 2012 Oct 1 [cited 2018 May 14];102(4):526–31. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22750066
51. Venugopalan V V., Casey KF, O’Hara C, O’Loughlin J, Benkelfat C, Fellows LK, et al. Acute phenylalanine/tyrosine depletion reduces motivation to smoke cigarettes across stages of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology [Internet]. 2011 Nov 20 [cited 2020 Oct 8];36(12):2469–76. Available from: www.neuropsychopharmacology.org pmid:21775977
52. Richardson DK, Reynolds SM, Cooper SJ, Berridge KC. Endogenous opioids are necessary for benzodiazepine palatability enhancement: Naltrexone blocks diazepam-induced increase of sucrose-‘liking.’ Pharmacol Biochem Behav [Internet]. 2005 Jul;81(3):657–63. Available from: pmid:15961147
53. Krupitsky E, Zvartau E, Blokhina E, Verbitskaya E, Wahlgren V, Tsoy-Podosenin M, et al. Anhedonia, depression, anxiety, and craving in opiate dependent patients stabilized on oral naltrexone or an extended release naltrexone implant. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse [Internet]. 2016 Sep 2 [cited 2021 Jun 24];42(5):614–20. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27436632/
54. O’Brien CP, Gastfriend DR, Forman RF, Schweizer E, Pettinati HM. Long-term opioid blockade and hedonic response: preliminary data from two open-label extension studies with extended-release naltrexone. Am J Addict [Internet]. 2011/02/15. 2011;20(2):106–12. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3895092/ pmid:21314752
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2022 Nunez et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Many mental health disorders are characterized by an impaired ability, or willingness, to exert effort to obtain rewards. This impairment is modeled in effort-based decision tasks, and neuropharmacological studies implicate dopamine in this process. However, other transmitter systems such as opioidergic and cholinergic systems have received less attention. Here, in two separate studies we tested the acute effects of naltrexone and nicotine on effort-based decision-making in healthy adults. In Study 1, we compared naltrexone (50mg and 25mg) to placebo, and in Study 2, a pilot study, we compared nicotine (7mg) to placebo. In both studies, participants completed the Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task (EEfRT), which measured effort-based decision-making related to monetary rewards. Although subjects expended greater effort for larger reward magnitude and when there was a higher probability of receiving the reward, neither naltrexone nor nicotine affected willingness to exert effort for monetary rewards. Although the drugs produced significant and typical drug effects on measures of mood and behavior, they did not alter effort-based decision-making. This has implications both for the clinical use of these drugs, as well as for understanding the neuropharmacology of effort-related behavior.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer