Introduction
Meaning plays a central role in our wellbeing, particularly in high stress conditions such as cancer. An important element of adapting to cancer is meaning reconstruction which is described by the integrative meaning-making model of coping [1]. However, meaning-making to disease is still poorly understood and its theoretical model has not been comprehensively verified yet. Such verification would consider its complexity, dynamics, reciprocity, and intraindividual variability. Therefore, we plan to conduct the intensive longitudinal research aimed at an in-depth understanding of the meaning-reconstruction process in cancer. The theoretical basis of the research will be the integrative meaning-making model of coping [1] extended by the psychological flexibility model [2].
Integrative meaning-making model of coping
Based on the integrative meaning-making model [1], meaning-making (i.e. process of searching for meaning and explanation for adversity) depends on global meaning (i.e. core schemas through which people perceive themselves and their surroundings, interpret the past, anticipate the future and follow their behavior), situational meaning (i.e. apprised meaning of a particular situation), and distress related to the discrepancy between them. Meaning-making impacts the meanings made (i.e. the perception of positive changes resulting from successful coping with adversity; benefit finding) and then wellbeing, which makes it a mediator in the model [1]. Meaning-making refers to approach-oriented intrapsychic efforts, which involve increasing the matching (or reduction of discrepancies) between global and situational meaning by changing either the meaning of the situation itself or one’s global beliefs and goals [1].
The complete verification of the model has not been made. Reports on the relationships between global meaning (i.e. beliefs, goals, sense of purpose) and wellbeing are predominant and show their positive relationships [3], also in the affected persons [4,5]. Situational meaning can play a mediating function in these relationships through more challenging, controllable and less threatening appraisal of an event, albeit so far outside the disease context [1,6]. There are also data indicating that the disease may impair general beliefs and life goals, which is associated with patient higher distress, anxiety and depression and lower quality of life [7–9]. Research on the relationships between meaning-making and adjustment to illness brings inconsistent findings indicating positive [10,11] and negative associations [4,12,13]. The relationships between meaning-making and meanings made are poorly investigated and are inconclusive, although most studies were not disease-related. In cancer survivors, meanings made mediated the association between meaning-making and longitudinal adjustment [9]. However, outside the disease context, meaning-making completed with meanings made was necessary, unnecessary or irrelevant for better adjustment [1]. Most studies focused on the effect of meanings made on adjustment to disease, without a simultaneous assessment of meaning-making. These studies were mostly cross-sectional and indicated positive or insignificant effects [14]. A meta-analysis of benefit finding following various stress conditions found that it was related to less depression and more positive wellbeing, but unrelated to anxiety, global distress, quality of life, or subjective physical health [15]. On the contrary, the review of longitudinal studies in the disease context found a favorable effect of meanings made on physical aspects of adjustment rather than psychological ones [14].
Psychological flexibility model
Psychological flexibility is defined as an individual’s ability to freely choose the action that is consistent with one’s own goals and values, regardless of what thoughts, emotions and impressions accompany it [2]. The psychological flexibility model includes six key processes, i.e. acceptance, cognitive defusion, contact with the present moment, perspective-taking, values clarification, and committed action [2]. Acceptance and defusion are the most important skills that increase a person’s openness to direct experiences (through the attitude of interest and curiosity) and allow for diversity in action, which enables people to freely take action consistent with the values of the individual. Contact with the present moment focuses on the experiences of the present and perspective-taking allows one to view them from a broader perspective. Values clarification and committed action bring vitality and meaning to the actions taken through readiness to valuable engaging in life. Due to identification of life values, it is possible to link current behavior with what is important and meaningful. Each key process of psychological flexibility plays a decisive role in a person’s ability to adapt to adversity [2].
A number of studies have found that psychological flexibility is associated with more adaptive coping [16], adaptive psychological traits, including higher conscientiousness and openness to experience [17], and better wellbeing, including physical health, quality of life, and emotional wellbeing in the healthy population [2,18]. Favorable results of psychological flexibility on physical and emotional wellbeing were also found in patients with chronic diseases [19,20]. Psychological flexibility has also been proven to be a buffer that mitigates the effects of stress on wellbeing in healthy persons [16] and patients [21]. We have not found any research that would explicitly test the relationships between psychological flexibility and global or situational meaning, meaning-making or meanings made. However, some data indicated that people characterized by openness and curiosity showed a higher tendency to benefit finding [22,23].
Rationale for the present study
Although the process of meaning-reconstruction in disease is an important element of adaptation, little is known about it. Earlier studies failed to address the complexity or dynamics of the meaning-making process. As a result, the model has not been fully verified and the available data apply only to the between-person differences. The solution to this problem is to conduct the real-life research using the intensive longitudinal approach which allows for studying the dynamic intraindividual variability [24]. Research on the meaning-making process in disease should also consider all the components of the model to determine the adaptive value of the process. Previous research did not differentiate between the process and its results. Most studies did not measure meaning-making and meanings made simultaneously. Therefore, drawing clear conclusions is difficult in terms of whether meaning-making is associated with adjustment to disease to the extent that meaning is made. It is likely that meaning-making adaptability depends on meanings made. However, this has not been clearly confirmed yet [1]. Furthermore, studies should examine moderation and mediation relationships. Data are missing on what mechanisms lie behind the meaning-making process. Completing the model with psychological flexibility seems to be a promising solution. Psychological flexibility promotes acceptance of what is difficult to change or is not subject to change, taking responsibility for one’s own experiences and actions, and creating a meaningful life by engaging in activities that are consistent with one’s values. Increasing psychological flexibility should therefore foster the creation of meaning in disease by building more flexible and workable meaning-making explanations of disease. Thus, psychological flexibility may constitute the missing mechanism of meaning-making strategies in the integrative meaning-making model.
The purpose of our research will be (i) the identification of individual trajectories and sources of variation of the meaning-reconstruction process during a chronic disease, (ii) the investigation of psychological flexibility as a possible mediator of the meaning-reconstruction process, and (iii) the determination of within-person dynamic and reciprocal associations in the extended meaning-making model (see Fig 1) in an observational intensive longitudinal study among patients following hematopoietic cell transplantation (HCT). HCT is a highly invasive and life-threatening treatment of hematologic neoplasms associated with burdensome adverse effects and a strong medical regimen for patients [25]. However, this procedure gives patients hope for recovery or long-term remission. Therefore, HCT can represent a mini-seismic event during coping with cancer (the turning point in patient lives) posing a challenge to the patient meaning structures. The post-HCT period may prompt reflection on the meaning of HCT and the patient current situation, which are part of meaning-making. In addition, HCT is mostly performed after the period known as the shock and denial phase. Potentially, this is the period during which the patient can start more reflective, meaning-making coping.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Study research questions and hypotheses
We formulated the following research questions: (1) If and what are the individual trajectories of meaning-making across first 14 days after discharge (within-person level) and how do those trajectories differ from person to person (between-person level)? (2) What processes underlie the individual meaning-making fluctuations and how do the patients differ in this process? In particular: (a) Does daily meaning-making mediate the effect of daily fluctuations in situational meaning and distress on fluctuations in meanings made and wellbeing? (b) Does daily meaning-making mediate the effect of baseline to follow-up changes in global meaning, meanings made, and wellbeing? (c) Do daily meanings made moderate the association between daily fluctuations in meaning-making and wellbeing? (d) Does psychological (in)flexibility mediate the effect of daily fluctuations in distress on fluctuations in meaning-making? (e) Does psychological (in)flexibility mediate the effect of daily fluctuations in meaning-making on fluctuations in meanings made? (f) Do these relationships occur when the direction of the relationships between variables is reversed? (g) Do demographic, situational and individual factors moderate the individual trajectories and tested relationships?
We expect that various trajectories of meaning-making following HCT will be identified with various directions (increase vs. decrease) and dynamics over time (Hypothesis 1; H1). In addition, we expect the support for assumptions of the meaning-making model, importance of daily psychological (in)flexibility in this process, and reciprocal associations between variables in the extended meaning-making model. Based on the integrative meaning-making model, we predict that daily meaning-making will mediate between daily fluctuations in antecedents/distress and outcomes (H2), as well as between baseline and follow-up changes in global meaning and outcomes (H3). Also, positive effects of daily fluctuations in meaning-making on wellbeing will occur on the days when meaning is given (H4). Referring to the psychological flexibility model, we assume that daily psychological flexibility can determine meaning-making and therefore can mediate between daily distress and meaning-making (H5). Alternatively, daily psychological flexibility can be determined by meaning-making and can mediate between daily meaning-making and meanings made (H6). Without hypotheses, but more exploratively, we also postulate the existence of additional pathways of relationships of a reciprocal nature, both in relation to the relationships between distress, psychological (in)flexibility and meaning-making, and the other elements of the model, i.e. global and situational meaning, as well as outcomes of the meaning-making process. Finally, we suppose that demographic (e.g. age and sex of patients), situational (e.g. time since the diagnosis) and individual factors (e.g. global and situational meaning, baseline wellbeing) could be potential moderators of the meaning-reconstruction process.
Materials and methods
Ethics
The study received ethical approval from the Ethical Review Board at SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Faculty of Psychology in Warsaw (Decision No. 26/2022 of April 12, 2022) and adheres to the ethical guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants will be requested to give written informed consent prior to participation.
Design
This is an observational study with an intensive longitudinal design. Patients admitted for autologous (patient’s own stem cells) or allogeneic (donor stem cells) HCT will be recruited.
Setting
Patients will be recruited from the Department of Bone Marrow Transplantation and Oncohematology of the Maria Sklodowska-Curie National Research Institute of Oncology (MSCNRIO) Gliwice Branch, Poland. Approximately 215 primary transplants are performed there annually (approx. 246 HCT in total).
Participants and sample size
The sample size was estimated using a target power of 80%, at alpha of 0.05, and was calculated relative to the small effect size in the latent growth analysis using an a-priori sample size calculator [26]. A minimum of 125 patients should be sufficient to obtain the adequate power analyses. Considering the potential attrition rate of 20%, the final sample is 150 patients. Similar values (for two-level model with random slopes and within-person mediation model) come from simulation studies using the Monte Carlo approach [24,27].
The inclusion criteria will include qualification of the patient for the first autologous or allogeneic HCT due to hematologic and lymphatic cancer, ≥ 18 years, and written informed consent. The exclusion criteria will be as follows: the presence of any other major medical or psychiatric disorder other than cancer that would impede the ability to participate in the study, insufficient reading and writing skills of patients, and the evidence of patient unreliability.
Recruitment process
Recruitment will start after elective admission to the transplantation unit due to HCT. We assume that it will take place approximately two days after admission and before the conditioning treatment. Every two days, research team member (AK) will review the lists of patients enrolled for HCT. Then, another research team member (recruiter) will ask patients who met the study criteria about their interest in participating in the study. Those interested will attend an individual initial meeting during which the recruiter will describe the course of the study in detail and will ask for consent to participate. The patient will be enrolled in the study if they provide written informed consent. The consent can be withdrawn at any time without any negative consequences for participants. The recruitment is anticipated to cover the period from May 2022 to January 2024 (approx. 21 months; considering the decline rate of approximately 40%), depending on the COVID-19 pandemic. Participants will be paid PLN 100 (~USD 22) for the participation in the study.
Data collection methods
Data will be collected via self-reported electronic surveys at baseline and follow-up, and a 14-day diary (Table 1). The baseline survey will be administered directly after obtaining written informed consent. Daily-diary will start on the second day after hospital discharge and will take 14 days. A follow-up survey will be administered on Day 15. All tools within the diary procedure will be shortened so that the number of items measuring any given indicator ranges from two to six, which is a common practice in such research [24]. Participants who will give their written approval will receive daily text messages that will remind them to complete a diary. During the study, research team member (JD) will contact the participant by phone to resolve any issues and answer questions.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Measures
Patients will be asked to complete the following measures at baseline and follow-up:
Global meaning.
Global meaning will include a sense of meaning and illness perception, which will be assessed using the 10-item Meaning in Life Questionnaire (MLQ) [28] and the 8-item Illness Perception Questionnaire (B-IPQ) [29], respectively.
Meanings made.
Meanings made (i.e. positive psychological changes) will be measured using the modified 10-item Post-Traumatic Growth Inventory-Short Form (C-PTGI-SF) [30,31]. The modification consists in changing the wording of the items to refer to the current state instead of recalling and comparing pre- and post-event status [31].
Wellbeing.
Wellbeing will include symptoms of depression, anxiety, loneliness, and the indicators of health-related quality of life (HRQOL), which will be assessed with the 10-item Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale Short Form (CES-D-SF) [32,33], the 7-item Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale (GAD-7) [34], the 20-item Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale [35], and the 30-item EORTC QLQ-C30 Questionnaire [36,37], respectively.
In daily-dairy, patients will be asked to complete the following measures:
Situational meaning.
Daily situational meaning will be measured using indicators of primary and secondary appraisal of the current situation following HCT using five items from the Stress Appraisal Measure (SAM) adapted to the daily procedure and study context [38]. The secondary appraisal will be additionally measured with six items from the Coping Self-Efficacy Scale (CSE) adapted to this study [39].
Distress.
Daily meaning-related distress refers to daily beliefs and goal violation and will be assessed using selected nine items from the Global Meaning Violation Scale (GMVS) adapted to the daily procedure and study context [40].
Meaning-making.
Daily meaning-making will be measured using selected six items from the Core Beliefs Inventory (CBI) [41] and eight items from the Perceived Ability to Cope with Trauma (PACT) scale adapted to the daily procedure [42]. Both tools measure meaning-making efforts, i.e. CBI—reconsideration of global beliefs [40], whereas PACT—remembering the event and reflecting on its meaning (subscale: trauma focus) [43].
Meanings made and wellbeing.
Daily meanings made will be measured using five items from the modified Post-Traumatic Growth Inventory-Short Form (C-PTGI-SF) [31]. Daily wellbeing will include daily somatic symptoms and affect, which will be assessed using the self-reported symptom checklist [44] and the 12-item adjective scale (reflecting positive and negative affect of various arousal) based on the circumplex model of emotion by Larsen and Diener [45], respectively.
Psychological flexibility.
Psychological flexibility will be measured with the short form of the Multidimensional Psychological Flexibility Inventory (MPFI) adopted to the daily procedure. This inventory assesses six flexible and six inflexible processes [46].
Other measures.
At baseline, the demographic data (i.e. age, gender, education, marital status, employment, having children, socioeconomic status) will be assessed. Clinical data (i.e. diagnosis, time since diagnosis, type of HCT, conditioning, concomitant diseases, treatment toxicity, complications after HCT e.g. graft-versus-host disease in allogeneic HCT recipients) will be obtained from the medical records by a physician (MSK).
In the original and Polish versions, all the tools (except GMVS whose psychometric properties will be tested in this study) are characterized by satisfactory psychometric properties. Table 1 shows the standard protocol items of the study.
Statistical analysis
Analyses will be conducted using the latest Mplus statistical package [47] and IBM SPSS (IBM Corp.; Armonk, NY). We will use the standard p < .05 or 95% confidence interval for determination of value probability. The collected data will be first analyzed in terms of sample characteristics and comparisons (frequency, descriptive statistics; ANOVA, t-test or their nonparametric counterparts; Chi2; Pearson’s or Kendall’s correlation), missing data (frequency, multilevel modeling), and sample attrition (logistic regression analysis). Multilevel confirmatory factor analysis (MCFA) will be performed to calculate the indicator reliabilities (omega coefficient) at the within- and between-person levels and establish the respective measurement models [24,48]. To examine research questions, the multilevel (MSEM) and dynamic structural equation modeling (DSEM) will be applied [24,49]. Both methods allow for the examination of time course, simple between- and within-person associations, and more advanced associations such as mediations and moderations. Moreover, they allow the most recent flexible approach to the missing data (the full information maximum likelihood) [50,51], which is possible due to the setting and daily-diary procedure.
Hypotheses 1–6 and the research question 2g will be mostly verified using MSEM. In MSEM, random coefficient models with maximum likelihood as an estimator will be applied. Predictors will be divided into within-person (the deviation from the person mean) and between-person (stable between-person mean for each person across all their diary days) indicators, which allowed separation of within-person change from between-person differences of the predictor. Centered linear time trend will be controlled in the analyses. In all models, possible confounders (i.e. demographics, clinical factors, other confounders) will be considered after preliminary selection. For exploratory reasons, lagged models that predict outcomes based on the previous-day predictors will also be considered. Significant interactions (H4, question 2g) will be graphed and probed with simple slope analyses [52].
Research question 2f and H3 will be verified using a multilevel VAR(1) model in DSEM (other hypotheses and questions can also be verified with DSEM). VAR(1) model consists of a set of regression equations, in which each endogenous variable is regressed on its own lagged values (autoregression) and the lagged values of the other variables (cross-regression) for each individual, which allows for the estimation of reciprocal associations. We will use the Mplus default priors (mean = 0, variance = 1010) and the Bayesian estimator (specific to DSEM). Within- and between-person associations will be automatically distinguished in DSEM. To compare the strength of cross-lagged associations, we will use the within standardization (i.e. standardization using the within-person variance) [53].
Limitations of the study
A limitation of the research is the restriction of observation to post-HCT patients undergoing the first transplant engraftment. However, this subgroup allows for gender balance compared to other frequently observed subgroups (e.g., breast cancer women or prostate cancer men), and for the observation of the meaning-reconstruction process after the challenging treatment procedure. Furthermore, the assumed observation time (i.e. 14 days), may turn out to be insufficient to observe the effects of fluctuations in the meaning-making process (if these processes fluctuate at longer intervals than overnight or effects of their fluctuations are time-postponed). In that case, however, the research will provide valuable information on the dynamics of meaning-making processes.
Conclusion
The presented research project will fill in the gaps in health psychology in the understanding of the meaning-reconstruction process in cancer and the mechanisms of this process by a comprehensive verification of the integrative meaning-making model and its extension by the psychological flexibility model. The data obtained in the study will be used to design the experimental research on the effects of the psychological flexibility-based intervention on the meaning-making process.
Acknowledgments
We thank Prof. Sebastian Giebel (head of the Department of Bone Marrow Transplantation and Oncohematology in Maria Sklodowska-Curie National Research Institute of Oncology (MSCNRIO) Gliwice Branch) for agreeing to conduct research in the clinic.
Citation: Kroemeke A, Dudek J, Sobczyk-Kruszelnicka M (2022) The role of psychological flexibility in the meaning-reconstruction process in cancer: The intensive longitudinal study protocol. PLoS ONE 17(10): e0276049. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276049
About the Authors:
Aleksandra Kroemeke
Roles: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Faculty of Psychology in Warsaw, Department of Health Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8707-742X
Joanna Dudek
Roles: Methodology, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Faculty of Psychology in Warsaw, Department of Behavioral Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7114-8884
Małgorzata Sobczyk-Kruszelnicka
Roles: Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Bone Marrow Transplantation and Oncohematology, Maria Sklodowska-Curie National Research Institute of Oncology (MSCNRIO), Gliwice, Poland
1. Park CL. Making sense of the meaning literature: An integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychol Bull. 2010;136: 257–301. pmid:20192563
2. Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, Wilson KG. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: The process and practice of mindful change. Second. Guilford Press; 2011.
3. Czekierda K, Banik A, Park CL, Luszczynska A. Meaning in life and physical health: systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2017;11: 387–418. pmid:28488471
4. Dezutter J, Casalin S, Wachholtz A, Luyckx K, Hekking J, Vandewiele W. Meaning in life: An Important factor for the psychological well-being of chronically Ill patients? Rehabil Psychol. 2013;58: 334–341. pmid:24295525
5. Loeffler S, Poehlmann K, Hornemann B. Finding meaning in suffering?—Meaning making and psychological adjustment over the course of a breast cancer disease. Eur J Cancer Care (Engl). 2018;27: e12841. pmid:29575157
6. Folkman S, Moskowitz JT. Positive affect and meaning-focused coping during significant psychological stress. In: Hewstone M, Schut H, De Wit JBF, Van Den Bos K, Stroebe M, editors. The scope of social psychology: Theory and applications. New York, NY, US: Psychology Press; 2007. pp. 193–208.
7. Kuijer RG, de Ridder DTD. Discrepancy in illness-related goals and quality of life in chronically ill patients: The role of self-efficacy. Psychol Health. 2003;18: 313–330.
8. Nordin K, Wasteson E, Hoffman K, Glimelius B, Sjödén PO. Discrepancies between attainment and importance of life values and anxiety and depression in gastrointestinal cancer patients and their spouses. Psychooncology. 2001;10: 479–489. pmid:11747060
9. Park CL. Testing the meaning making model of coping with loss. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2008;27: 970–994.
10. Boehmer S, Luszczynska A, Schwarzer R. Coping and quality of life after tumor surgery: personal and social resources promote different domains of quality of life. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2007;20: 61–75. pmid:17999215
11. Pakenham KI. Making sense of multiple sclerosis. Rehabil Psychol. 2007;52: 380–389.
12. Kernan WD, Lepore SJ. Searching for and making meaning after breast cancer: prevalence, patterns, and negative affect. Soc Sci Med. 2009;68: 1176–1182. pmid:19157667
13. Tomich PL, Helgeson VS. Five years later: a cross-sectional comparison of breast cancer survivors with healthy women. Psychooncology. 2002;11: 154–169. pmid:11921331
14. Algoe SB, Stanton AL. Is benefit finding good for individuals with chronic disease? In: Park CL, Lechner SC, Antonii MH, Stanton AL, editors. Medical illness and positive life change: Can crisis lead to personal transformation? Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; 2009. pp. 173–193. https://doi.org/10.1037/11854-010
15. Helgeson VS, Reynolds KA, Tomich PL. A meta-analytic review of benefit finding and growth. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006;74: 797–816. pmid:17032085
16. Brandtstädter J, Rothermund K. The life-course dynamics of goal pursuit and goal adjustment: A two-process framework. Dev Rev. 2002;22: 117–150.
17. Kashdan TB, Rottenberg J. Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30: 865–878. pmid:21151705
18. Marshall E-J, Brockman RN. The relationships between psychological flexibility, self-compassion, and emotional well-Being. J Cogn Psychother. 2016;30: 60–72. pmid:32755906
19. González-Fernández S, Fernández-Rodríguez C, Mota-Alonso MJ, García-Teijido P, Pedrosa I, Pérez-Álvarez M. Emotional state and psychological flexibility in breast cancer survivors. Eur J Oncol Nurs. 2017;30: 75–83. pmid:29031317
20. McCracken LM, Gutiérrez-Martínez O, Smyth C. “Decentering” reflects psychological flexibility in people with chronic pain and correlates with their quality of functioning. Health Psychol. 2013;32: 820–823. pmid:22545976
21. Swash B, Bramwell R, Hulbert-Williams NJ. Unmet psychosocial supportive care needs and psychological distress in haematological cancer survivors: The moderating role of psychological flexibility. J Context Behav Sci. 2017;6: 187–194.
22. Higgins ET. Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Psychol Rev. 2006;113: 439–460. pmid:16802877
23. Kashdan TB, Steger MF. Expanding the topography of social anxiety. An experience-sampling assessment of positive emotions, positive events, and emotion suppression. Psychol Sci. 2006;17: 120–128. pmid:16466419
24. Bolger N, Laurenceau J-P. Intensive longitudinal methods: An introduction to diary and experience sampling research. New York: Guilford Press; 2013.
25. Copelan EA. Hematopoietic stem-cell transplantation. N Engl J Med. 2006;354: 1813–1826. pmid:16641398
26. Soper DS. A-priori sample size calculator for for structural equation Models [Software]. 2020 [cited 5 Aug 2020]. Available: https://www.danielsoper.com/statcalc/references.aspx?id=89.
27. Sagan A. Sample size in multilevel structural equation modeling–the Monte Carlo approach. Econometrics. 2019;23: 63–79.
28. Steger MF, Frazier P, Oishi S, Kaler M. The meaning in life questionnaire: Assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. J Couns Psychol. 2006;53: 80–93.
29. Weinman J, Petrie KJ, Moss-morris R, Horne R. The illness perception questionnaire: A new method for assessing the cognitive representation of illness. Psychol Health. 1996;11: 431–445.
30. Cann A, Calhoun LG, Tedeschi RG, Taku K, Vishnevsky T, Triplett KN, et al. A short form of the Posttraumatic Growth Inventory. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2010;23: 127–137. pmid:19582640
31. Kaur N, Porter B, LeardMann CA, Tobin LE, Lemus H, Luxton DD, et al. Evaluation of a modified version of the Posttraumatic Growth Inventory-Short Form. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2017;17: 69. pmid:28427350
32. Andresen EM, Malmgren JA, Carter WB, Patrick DL. Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale). Am J Prev Med. 1994;10: 77–84. pmid:8037935
33. Zhang W, O’Brien N, Forrest JI, Salters KA, Patterson TL, Montaner JSG, et al. Validating a shortened depression scale (10 item CES-D) among HIV-positive people in British Columbia, Canada. PloS One. 2012;7: e40793. pmid:22829885
34. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW, Löwe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166: 1092–1097. pmid:16717171
35. Russell DW. UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. J Pers Assess. 1996;66: 20–40. pmid:8576833
36. Aaronson NK, Ahmedzai S, Bergman B, Bullinger M, Cull A, Duez NJ, et al. The European Organization for Research and Treatment of Cancer QLQ-C30: a quality-of-life instrument for use in international clinical trials in oncology. J Natl Cancer Inst. 1993;85: 365–376. pmid:8433390
37. Sprangers MA, Cull A, Groenvold M, Bjordal K, Blazeby J, Aaronson NK. The European Organization for Research and Treatment of Cancer approach to developing questionnaire modules: an update and overview. EORTC Quality of Life Study Group. Qual Life Res. 1998;7: 291–300.
38. Peacock EJ, Wong PTP. The stress appraisal measure (SAM): A multidimensional approach to cognitive appraisal. Stress Med. 1990;6: 227–236.
39. Chesney MA, Neilands TB, Chambers DB, Taylor JM, Folkman S. A validity and reliability study of the coping self-efficacy scale. Br J Health Psychol. 2006;11: 421–437. pmid:16870053
40. Park CL, Riley KE, George LS, Gutierrez IA, Hale AE, Cho D, et al. Assessing disruptions in meaning: Development of the Global Meaning Violation Scale. Cogn Ther Res. 2016;40: 831–846.
41. Cann A, Calhoun L, Tedeschi R, Kilmer R, Gil-Rivas V, Vishnevsky T, et al. The Core Beliefs Inventory: A brief measure of disruption in the assumptive world. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2009;23: 19–34. pmid:19326274
42. Bonanno GA, Pat-Horenczyk R, Noll J. Coping flexibility and trauma: The Perceived Ability to Cope With Trauma (PACT) scale. Psychol Trauma Theory Res Pract Policy. 2011;3: 117–129.
43. Bonanno GA. Meaning making, adversity, and regulatory flexibility. Memory. 2013;21: 150–156. pmid:23311413
44. Kroemeke A, Sobczyk-Kruszelnicka M, Kwissa-Gajewska Z. Everyday life following hematopoietic stem cell transplantation: decline in physical symptoms within the first month and change-related predictors. Qual Life Res. 2018;27: 125–135. pmid:28900828
45. Larsen RJ, Diener E. Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion. In: Clark MS, editor. Review of personality and social psychology, No 13, Emotion. Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc; 1992. pp. 25–59.
46. Rolffs JL, Rogge RD, Wilson KG. Disentangling components of flexibility via the hexaflex model: Development and validation of the Multidimensional Psychological Flexibility Inventory (MPFI). Assessment. 2018;25: 458–482. pmid:27152011
47. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. Seventh Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998.
48. Finch WH, Bolin JE. Multilevel modeling using Mplus. New York: Chapman and Hall/CRC; 2017. https://doi.org/10.1201/9781315165882
49. Asparouhov T, Hamaker EL, Muthén B. Dynamic structural equation models. Struct Equ Model. 2018;25: 359–388.
50. Enders CK, Bandalos DL. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Struct Equ Model. 2001;8: 430–457.
51. Newman DA. Missing data: Five practical guidelines. Organ Res Methods. 2014;17: 372–411.
52. Jaccard J, Wan CK, Turrisi R. The detection and interpretation of interaction effects between continuous variables in multiple regression. Multivar Behav Res. 1990;25: 467–478. pmid:26820822
53. Schuurman NK, Ferrer E, de Boer-Sonnenschein M, Hamaker EL. How to compare cross-lagged associations in a multilevel autoregressive model. Psychol Methods. 2016;21: 206–221. pmid:27045851
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2022 Kroemeke et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Objectives
Meaning-making is an important element of adapting to disease. However, this process is still poorly understood and the theoretical model has not been comprehensively verified yet, particularly in terms of complexity, dynamics, and intraindividual variability. The aim of this study is a deeper understanding of the meaning-reconstruction process in cancer and empirical verification of the integrative meaning-making model of coping extended by the psychological flexibility model. We postulate that psychological flexibility can foster the meaning-making in cancer by building more flexible and workable meaning-making explanations of disease.
Design
A daily-diary study conducted for 14 days in patients following the first autologous or allogeneic hematopoietic cell transplantation (HCT).
Methods
Participants (at least 150) will be requested to complete the daily-diary related to daily situational meaning, meaning-related distress, meaning-making, psychological flexibility, meanings made, and wellbeing for 14 days after hospital discharge following HCT. Also, baseline and follow-up assessment of global meaning, wellbeing, and meanings made will be performed. Statistical analysis of the data will be conducted using the multilevel and dynamic structural equation modeling.
Conclusions
The study will fill in the gaps in health psychology in the understanding of the meaning-reconstruction process in cancer by within- and between-person verification of the integrative meaning-making model and its extension by the psychological flexibility model. The data obtained will be used in further research on the development of meaning-making by means of interventions based on psychological flexibility.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer