It appears you don't have support to open PDFs in this web browser. To view this file, Open with your PDF reader
Abstract
Sense of time is a complex construct, and its neural correlates remain to date in most part unknown. To complicate the frame, physical attributes of the stimulus, such as its intensity or movement, influence temporal perception. Although previous studies have shown that time perception can be compromised after a brain lesion, the evidence on the role of the left and right hemispheres are meager. In two experiments, the study explores the ability of temporal estimation of multi-second actions and non-biological movements in 33 patients suffering from unilateral brain lesion. Furthermore, the modulatory role of induced embodiment processes is investigated. The results reveal a joint contribution of the two hemispheres depending not only on different durations but also on the presence of actions. Indeed, the left hemisphere damaged patients find it difficult to estimate 4500 ms or longer durations, while the right hemisphere damaged patients fail in 3000 ms durations. Furthermore, the former fail when a biological action is shown, while the latter fail in non-biological movement. Embodiment processes have a modulatory effect only after right hemisphere lesions. Among neuropsychological variables, only spatial neglect influences estimation of non-biological movement.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details

1 University of Bordeaux, Groupe d’Imagerie NeurofonctionnelleInstitut des Maladies Neurodégénératives-UMR 5293, CNRS, CEA, Bordeaux Cedex, France (GRID:grid.412041.2) (ISNI:0000 0001 2106 639X); Sorbonne Universities, Brain Connectivity and Behaviour Laboratory, Paris, France (GRID:grid.462844.8) (ISNI:0000 0001 2308 1657)
2 University of Verona, NPSY-Lab.VR, Department of Human Sciences, Verona, Italy (GRID:grid.5611.3) (ISNI:0000 0004 1763 1124)
3 Hospital Trust of Verona, Neurorehabilitation Unit, Department of Neurosciences, Verona, Italy (GRID:grid.5611.3)
4 University La Sapienza, Department of Psychology, Rome, Italy (GRID:grid.7841.a)
5 IRCSS Sacro Cuore Don Calabria, Department of Rehabilitation, Negrar, Verona, Italy (GRID:grid.7841.a)
6 Fresco Institute Italy - NYU Langone, Department of Neurorehabilitation, Parkinson’s Disease Excellence Center, Vicenza, Italy (GRID:grid.7841.a)