1. Introduction
In the last several decades, cognitive neuroscience has assisted the development of powerful tools that allow the measurement and modulation of brain activity with good spatial resolution. Such advances have made it possible to answer specific questions about the relevance of individual brain areas in behavior and cognition, although, in a holistic perspective, complex networks throughout the brain are thought to drive higher-order cognitive processes, such as decision making. Nevertheless, even at this higher order of functioning, our brain can still follow very basic rules, and these might be more rigidly dependent on the activity of less extended circuits.
For instance, in the context of decision making, delay discounting (DD) is a phenomenon observed when subjects have to evaluate rewards of different magnitudes with delays to receipt. According to DD, the subjective value of a reward, normally measured trough intertemporal choice tasks (Figure 1a), declines with the delay to its receipt [1]. Although different models have been applied to fit the devaluation function of DD, the most established is the hyperbolic (Figure 1b, adapted from [2]). Both DD and hyperbolic devaluation have been reliably found across different species, indicating that such processes are developmentally conserved and thus of great importance for the survival of animals [3]. Even at a biopsychosocial level of analysis, DD plays a crucial role in economic decision and policy making [4]. The quantitative nature of the process also suggests that it can be treated as a psychophysical rule, even if in the domain of cognitive functioning.
As a further proof of its importance for physiological functioning, DD has been shown to be systematically compromised in several psychiatric conditions [5,6,7], and, thanks to its trans-species validity, it appears as a very promising avenue for translational research based on animal models [8].
Depending on the available neuroscientific evidence, several interpretative models for such phenomena have been proposed which are often mutually exclusive. Based on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments, McClure et al. hypothesized the existence of two interacting systems that support decision making, named β and δ, the former relating to the “immediacy” nature of the decision outcome and the latter to “all decisions” [9]; Kable and Glimcher proposed the existence of a unique system widespread in the brain that computes the subjective value of rewards [10], while Ballard et al. speculate that the magnitude and the delay of a reward are evaluated by different neuronal networks [11]. On the other hand, based on non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) experiments, Figner et al. suggested the existence of a dedicated brain area which actively suppresses the impulsive choice, the one providing an immediate reward [12], and Nejati et al. postulated the involvement of “hot”, i.e., high-order psychological processes related to motivation and emotion, and “cold”, purely cognitive, networks in the DD task [13]. Scherbaum et al. instead argued that performance in an intertemporal choice task can be described by an attractor model, where the delayed reward choice has to overcome the “attraction energy/potential landscape” of the immediate reward in order to be selected [14].
In addition to the quantity and type of neural systems required for the production of delay discounting, the localization of these systems within the brain remains a topic of ongoing debate. It is important to acknowledge that, despite the apparent simplicity of the underlying logic behind temporal discounting, it is probable that multiple neural systems interact during the decision-making process; for example, in these tasks, the subject must perceive and interpret the stimuli that are predictive of the future rewards [15], and sensory systems have already been implicated in decision processes [16]. Hence, these involved systems might contribute themselves to the encoding, at least in part, of devaluation. Although an extreme reductionist/localist approach would not then be appropriate in this context, the available studies do not reject a priori the hypothesis that DD could be mostly dependent on the activity of a specific brain area. Nevertheless, based on the above premises, different methodological approaches might lead to the identification of very different targets. For these reasons, in this review we will discuss correlative studies, i.e., studies based on measuring brain activity during DD tasks, separately from causal studies, i.e., studies based on brain lesions and neuromodulation, which we consider more valuable in this context.
2. Neural Correlates of Delay Discounting Assessed by Neuroimaging Studies
Numerous experimental approaches have been employed to investigate the neural mechanisms underlying DD. In the following, we conduct a qualitative examination of neuroimaging studies to examine the presence of any potential convergence among the brain regions identified. Voxel-based morphometry reveals that reduced white matter (WM) volume in the right prefrontal subgyral area and a higher WM volume in the right parahippocampus extending to the right hippocampus are associated with steeper DD performances [17]. Other authors found increased thickness of the bilateral medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the anterior/midcingulate cortices to be related to individuals’ tendencies for less discounting during DD tasks [18].
Neuroimaging experiments could potentially unveil the neuronal regions mostly involved in DD, yet the outcomes appear to be contradictory among comparable experimental settings: in many instances, not only are different brain regions identified as significantly active during the task, but also, when the active areas are the same, their activities are related to different features of the DD task. In a session of fMRI experiments, for instance, Ballard et al. pointed out that increasing future reward delay negatively correlated with the activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), the right posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and the left temporal-parietal junction (TPJ). The interaction of delay and magnitude negatively correlated with activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). Interestingly, increasing future reward magnitude correlated with the activity of the mesial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), the posterior cingulate cortex and the right nucleus accumbens (NAc) [11]. The NAc is also widely studied in DD experiments using rodents: indeed, dopamine (DA) release dynamics in the NAcs of rats account for the encoding of both reward magnitude and delay [19], and modulating the activity of serotoninergic neurons of the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) projecting into the NAc seems to alter intertemporal choices [20].
On the contrary, McClure et al. observed the activation of the midbrain dopamine system, including part of the paralimbic cortex, for immediate rewards (the β system) [9]. In rodents, also, the activity of DA neurons in the VTA increases constantly while the mouse is waiting for the reward, and optogenetically manipulating the DA activity in this region alters the duration of delay gratification [21]. On the other hand, the lateral prefrontal and associated parietal areas were activated by all types of intertemporal choices (the δ system). In another study, McClure et al. showed that the interval of time sensible to the activation of the β system depends on the type of reward, i.e., money or food [22]. Other authors ascertained that the ventral striatum, mPFC and posterior cingulate cortex are involved in computing the subjective value of rewards and that their activities correlate proportionally with subjective value. Curiously, all three regions seem to increase their activity after subjects receive a reward or immediately before an expected reward [10].
In the comparison with risky decision-making processes, Peters and Büchel [23] found similar activity patterns to Kable and Glimcher [10]. Furthermore, the OFC and ventral striatum were active during both tasks, which supports their role in encoding the value for the subjective stimulus. Experiments involving Go/NoGo tasks in a group divided into lower and higher levels of delay of gratification showed lower recruitment of the inferior frontal gyrus and greater recruitment of the ventral striatum [24], albeit DD tasks were not directly administered in this experiment. Hare et al. found that a subregion of the left dlPFC was more active when subjects chose the delayed choice and that, before the choice was made, the functional connectivity between the dlPFC and mPFC increased, especially for most delayed rewards [25]. Functional and morphological connectivity unveils the importance of striatal connections, in particular those with the dlPFC and the amygdala: the first is associated with less discounting, while the second is associated with a steeper value [26].
In conclusion, from brain imaging studies, the areas most involved appear to be the dlPFC, mPFC, OFC, PPC and ventral striatum. Nevertheless, from these experiments we cannot infer the effective involvement of these regions due to the non-specificity of BOLD signals or kindred indirect measures. Additionally, given the wide-ranging significance of these areas in decision making and cognate processes, the observed concurrent activation may simply be a byproduct of delay discounting (DD). Working memory, for instance, is implicated in intertemporal choices task [27]. However, the relationship between this cognitive domain and DD is still elusive: aging-dependent working memory decline cannot explain the enhanced ability of delay gratification in older people [4]. Moreover, the hippocampus and basolateral amygdala are able to modulate the discounting process as well, suggesting that episodic memory circuits of the medial temporal lobe are also engaged [28].
The results of these studies indicate that the neuronal circuits utilized for delay discounting tasks are highly diverse and likely intertwined with several psychological factors, sometimes leading to conflicting findings, which prevent the development of a comprehensive model of DD functioning. To address this issue, an active intervention to differentiate specific brain regions might be very effective. In the following paragraphs, we will present and discuss causal investigations based on the manipulation of brain activity.
3. Investigating the Causal Role of Neural Circuits in Delay Discounting
The conventional method for investigating the causal relationships, i.e., the precise role and function, of a specific brain region in a psychological process is through the study of the consequences of brain lesions, damage or injury to that neural circuit. Unfortunately, these studies provide a highly contentious picture. Indeed, several combinations of frontal lobe injuries in humans seem not to dramatically alter performance in DD tasks [29], while lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) increase preferences for small, immediate rewards [30]. It is worth mentioning that lesioned areas are not precisely defined for the different types of accidents that cause the lesions. Furthermore, plastic processes that follow brain injury could lead to important and heterogeneous changes in lesioned and proximal brain structures, likely causing great inter-subject variability. Several neuromodulation methods have flourished in recent years, including photobiomodulation [31] and optogenetic [32,33] and ultrasound [34] stimulation. However, at present, only a few experimental studies make use of the abovementioned techniques in humans. On the other hand, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) represent the most-exploited and -widespread techniques in the current neuroscientific literature and have already been applied in evaluating the role of several cortical regions in DD.
Effects of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) Neuromodulation on Delay Discounting
TMS, one of the main NIBS techniques, has proven successful in the treatment of many severe clinical conditions, including both psychiatric and neurological diseases [35]. For most clinical applications, the efficacy of TMS is thought to rely on the induction of synaptic plasticity and neuronal excitability in the targeted areas and circuits, which likely produces enduring changes in their activity levels [36,37,38]. In detail, low-frequency stimulation, of 1 Hz or less, causes a lasting decrease in cortical excitability, while high-frequency TMS stimulation, of 5 Hz or more, including either continuous or intermittent theta burst stimulation (TBS), has the opposite effect [39]. As for basic research, TMS represents a very valuable tool for investigating the functional roles of specific brain areas in the behavioral and cognitive functioning of human subjects. In the context of DD, the online manipulation of neuronal regions could more effectively reveal the functional role of a specific area, providing not only the opportunity to localize the phenomenon, but also the potential to decompose it into basic functional domains. This represents a crucial step towards a complete understanding of DD.
However, even using the very general search string “((delay discounting) OR (intertemporal choice) OR (impulsive choice) OR (temporal discounting)) AND (TMS)” in PubMed, only nine results were obtained at the time of writing this review. Most of the experiments described in these studies (summarized in Table 1) investigated the role of the dlPFC [12,15,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48], but the results remain controversial. Based on the literature described above, one might assume that low-frequency stimulation of the dlPFC would in turn augment the discounting rate in intertemporal choice tasks, while high-frequency stimulations, on the contrary, would diminish it. Indeed, high-frequency TMS, when applied to the dlPFC, reduces DD in smokers, but not the daily amount of cigarettes they smoke [42], while reducing the activity of the dlPFC with low-frequency stimulation increased discounting [46]. Nevertheless, 20 Hz TMS applied to the dlPFCs of subjects with depression did not affect DD performance [40]. In a comparison of neuromodulation of the left and right hemispheres, we also found some conflictual outcomes in the available literature: only low-frequency stimulation of the left dlPFC causes delay discounting rates to decrease [12,46].
Two studies investigated the role of the mPFC using high-frequency stimulation, leading to divergent findings: Zack et al. found that the stimulation had no effect [41], while Cho et al. obtained a decrease in DD [49]. In another study, continuous theta burst stimulation was applied to the posterior temporal-parietal junction (pTPJ), leading to an increase in delay discounting [50].
In the previous section, the OFC and ventral striatum were indicated as potentially involved regions, but, unfortunately, targeting these brain areas with TMS is rather challenging due to their locations: while the ventral striatum is too deep to be efficiently reached by the magnetic field, OFC stimulation, albeit practicable, is painful for the subject due to the unwanted contraction of facial muscles [51]. Therefore, we can argue that extensive investigation of the dlPFC compared to the OFC might be related to physical constraints rather than evidence-based hypotheses.
The absence of information regarding the impacts of stimulation on other relevant brain regions during delay discounting tasks constrains our ability to comprehend whether such regions play a direct or indirect role in the process. In addition, the inclusion of patients suffering from psychiatric disorders in some experiments [40,41,42,44] could introduce artifacts.
Table 1Studies investigating delay discounting performance modulation by TMS. MDD = major depression disorder; PG = pathological gambling; AUD = alcohol use disorder; MT = motor threshold.
Study | Brain Area | Protocol | Stimulation Intensity | n | Disease | Discounting Assessment | Effect |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cho et al., 2015 [51] | mPFC | 10 Hz-rTMS | 80% MT | 24 | - | ln (k) | Decreased delay discounting |
Zack et al., 2016 [40] | mPFC | rTMS | 80% MT | 9 | PG | k | - |
Zack et al., 2016 [40] | Right dlPFC | TBS | 80% MT | 9 | PG | k | - |
Cho et al., 2010 [42] | Right dlPFC | cTBS | 80% MT | 7 | - | k | Decreased delay discounting |
Figner et al., 2010 [12] | Right dlPFC | 1 Hz low-frequency rTMS | 54% MT | 19 | - | Immediate choices (%) | - |
Cho et al., 2012 [46] | Right dlPFC | cTBS | 80% MT | 8 | - | ln (k) | Decreased delay discounting |
Schluter et al., 2019 [43] | Right dlPFC | HF-rTMS | 110% MT | 40 | AUD | AUC | - |
Ballard et al., 2018 [45] | Right dlPFC | 1 Hz low-frequency rTMS | 120% MT | 12 | - | log (k) | Increased delay discounting |
Essex et al., 2012 [44] | Right dlPFC + right PPC | 1 Hz low-frequency rTMS | 54% MT | 16 | - | Immediate choices (%) | Decreased delay discounting |
Teti Mayer et al., 2019 [39] | Left dlPFC | 10 Hz | 110% MT | 20 | MDD | k | - |
Sheffer et al., 2013 [41] | Left dlPFC | HF rTMS | 110% MT | 47 | Smokers | k | Decreased delay discounting |
Figner et al., 2010 [12] | Left dlPFC | 1 Hz low-frequency rTMS | 54% MT | 18 | - | Immediate choices (%) | Increased delay discounting |
Ballard et al., 2018 [45] | Left dlPFC | 1 Hz low-frequency rTMS | 120% MT | 15 | - | log (k) | Increased delay discounting |
Yang et al., 2018 [52] | Left dlPFC | iTBS | 80% MT | 23 | - | ln (k) | - |
Essex et al., 2012 [44] | Left dlPFC + left PPC | 1 Hz low-frequency rTMS | 54% MT | 16 | - | Immediate choices (%) | Increased delay discounting |
Soutschek et al., 2016 [49] | Right pTPJ | cTBS | 80% MT | 22 | - | log (k) | Increased delay discounting |
Soutschek et al., 2016 [49] | Right pTPJ | cTBS | 80% MT | 20 | - | log (k) | Increased delay discounting |
Soutschek et al., 2016 [49] | Left S1 | cTBS | 80% MT | 21 | - | log (k) | - |
4. Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) Neuromodulation on Delay Discounting
Among non-invasive brain stimulation techniques, tDCS could be a cheaper and portable alternative to TMS. When one applies a positive current flow through one electrode, i.e., anodal stimulation, the excitability of the cortical neurons under that area of the scalp is increased, while a negative current (cathodal) decreases their excitability. Therefore, tDCS is thought to facilitate or inhibit the electrical activation of targeted brain regions [52].
Brunyé reviewed the latest research on the effects of non-invasive brain stimulation on decision-making processes and found that some protocols of tDCS can effectively modulate performance in several domains, such as economic, risky and perceptual decision making, as well as moral tasks [53].
Searching for tDCS applications in the field of DD, we entered the string “((delay discounting) OR (intertemporal choice) OR (impulsive choice) OR (temporal discounting)) AND (tDCS)” and were able to find a greater number of studies compared to the TMS case, although the figure was still as low as 20 (summarized in Table 2).
Most of the experiments investigated the role of dlPFC. Specifically, the anodal stimulation of the left dlPFC appears to reduce discounting rates in most trials [13,54,55,56,57,58]; nevertheless, some authors have found no significant effect of stimulation [59,60,61]. It is worth mentioning that the cathodal stimulation of this brain area strongly correlates with an increase in discounting rates [55,59]. The vmPFC also appears as a promising area, since its anodal stimulation leads to a reduction in the steepness of the DD curve [60,62]. The role of the OFC has been the focus of a study using tDCS: both anodal and cathodal stimulation at frontal sites caused a decrease in the discounting of delayed rewards [13]. Nevertheless, after performing a simulation based on SimNIBS (simnibs.github.io) of the electrical field produced by the electrodes’ configuration used in this study, we observed the current flowing in most of the PFC subregions, with only marginal, if any, involvement of the OFC (data not shown).
Finally, the stimulation of the cerebellum [63], the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG; [64]) and the motor cortex (M1; [61]) has been found to cause no alteration in intertemporal choice task performance.
One issue with tDCS relates to the fact that stimulation is not well-confined to specific brain regions, as compared to TMS, not only due to the dimensions of the electrodes but also depending on the placement of the reference electrode [65]. Even though high-density tDCS can be an effective choice for increasing spatial confinement, the use of computational modeling of current flows can also support the better design of experiments targeting specific regions, including subcortical ones [66]. As for TMS, the plethora of studies investigating DD with tDCS cannot provide us with enough information for understanding the neuronal circuits that are involved in this phenomenon. As a matter of fact, even with tDCS, there is a disproportionate number of studies investigating the dlPFC, although the OFC and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) clearly appear as promising targets.
Table 2Studies investigating delay discounting performance modulation by tDCS. PD = Parkinson’s disease; ADHD = attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; BN = bulimia nervosa; CUD = cocaine use disorder.
Study | Anode Position | Cathode Position | Current Intensity | n | Disease | Discounting Assessment | Effect |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Manuel at al., 2019 [63] | vmPFC | Vertex | 2.0 mA | 20 | - | log (k) | Decreased delay discounting |
Nejati et al., 2021 [61] | Right vmPFC | Left dlPFC | 1.0 mA | 20 | ADHD children | k | Decreased delay discounting |
Wang et al., 2021 [67] | FPC | Vertex | 1.5 mA | 90 | - | k | - |
Soutschek et al., 2017 |
Left FPC | Vertex | 1.0 mA | 27 | - | Immediate choices (%) | - |
To et al., 2018 |
Right IFG | Left IFG | 2.0 mA | 23 | Chocolate cravers | k | - |
Nejati et al., 2018 [13] | Right OFC | Left dlPFC | 1.5 mA | 24 | - | k | Decreased delay discounting |
He at al., 2016 |
Right dlPFC | - | 1.5 mA | 23 | - | k | - |
Shen et al., 2016 [56] | Right dlPFC | Left dlPFC | 2.0 mA | 39 | - | k | - |
Shen et al., 2016 [56] | Right dlPFC | - | 2.0 mA | 39 | - | k | - |
Xiong et al., 2019 [58] | Right dlPFC | Left dlPFC | 1.5 mA | 20 | - | k | - |
Kekic et al., 2017 [57] | Right |
Left |
2.0 mA | 39 | BN | δ | Decreased delay discounting |
Kekic et al., 2014 [70] | Right |
Left |
2.0 mA | 17 | Food cravers | k | - |
Hecht et al., 2013 [71] | Right |
Left |
1.6 mA | 14 | - | Immediate choices (%) | Decreased delay discounting |
Brunelin and Fecteau, 2021 |
Left |
Right |
2.0 mA | 15 | Acutely stressed | Immediate choices (%) | Decreased delay discounting |
He at al., 2016 |
Left dlPFC | - | 1.5 mA | 22 | - | k | Decreased delay discounting |
Shen et al., 2016 [56] | Left dlPFC | Right |
2.0 mA | 39 | - | k | - |
Shen et al., 2016 [56] | Left dlPFC | - | 2.0 mA | 39 | - | k | Decreased delay discounting |
Nejati et al., 2018 [13] | Left dlPFC | Right OFC | 1.5 mA | 24 | - | k | Decreased delay discounting |
Terenzi et al., 2021 [62] | Left dlPFC | Right shoulder | 1.5 mA | 28 | PD | log (k) | - |
Nejati et al., 2021 [61] | Left dlPFC | Right vmPFC | 1.0 mA | 20 | ADHD children | k | - |
Xiong et al., 2019 [58] | Left dlPFC | Right |
2.0 mA | 20 | - | k | Decreased delay discounting |
Kekic et al., 2017 [57] | Left dlPFC | Right |
2.0 mA | 39 | BN | δ | Decreased delay discounting |
Gaudreault et al., 2021 |
Left dlPFC | Right |
2.0 mA | 17 | CUD | k | Decreased delay discounting |
Colombo et al., 2020 [60] | Left dlPFC | Right triceps | 1.5 mA | 13 | - | RT | - |
Hecht et al., 2013 [71] | Left dlPFC | Right |
1.6 mA | 14 | - | Immediate choices (%) | Decreased delay discounting |
Manuel at al., 2019 [63] | Vertex | vmPFC | 2.0 mA | 20 | - | log (k) | - |
Wang et al., 2021 [67] | Vertex | FPC | 1.5 mA | 90 | - | k | - |
Soutschek et al., 2017 |
Vertex | Left FPC | 1.0 mA | 26 | - | Immediate choices (%) | - |
Terenzi et al., 2021 |
Left M1 | Right shoulder | 1.5 mA | 28 | PD | log (k) | - |
Wynn et al., 2019 |
Medial cerebellum | Right deltoid muscle | 2.0 mA | 26 | - | AUC | - |
Colombo et al., 2020 |
Right triceps | Left dlPFC | 1.5 mA | 13 | - | RT | Increased delay discounting |
Shen et al., 2016 |
- | Right dlPFC | 2.0 mA | 39 | - | k | - |
Shen et al., 2016 [56] | - | Left dlPFC | 2.0 mA | 39 | - | k | Increased delay discounting |
5. Conclusions, Limitations and Future Developments
Although the results of neuroimaging studies do not provide a definitive understanding of the specific contributions of brain regions to delay discounting tasks, they are valuable in identifying potential targets for investigation using non-invasive brain stimulation techniques. To synthesize the current state of research in this field, we present a schematic brain map (Figure 2) highlighting the brain regions implicated in delay discounting based on both neuroimaging studies (green areas) and neuromodulation studies (red areas), with yellow areas indicating the overlap between the two. If we consider the main hypothesis that DD is processed in a unique area, the left dlPFC appears to be the best candidate: excitatory stimulation of this area produces a decrease in discounting [13,42,54,55,56,57,58], while, on the contrary, its inhibition produces a steeper DD characteristic [12,46,55,59].
Nevertheless, there exist several caveats that might limit the significance of the results. In most of the studies evaluated here, for instance, the rewards were monetary (e.g., [13,55,57]), but, even if the general hyperbolic function applies, one cannot exclude that primary rewards (i.e., food [64,67] and sexually-related images [68]) are processed by distinct neural circuits [22]. The timing of rewards, moreover, is very heterogeneous among the experimental settings, and this could potentially result in the recruitment of completely different neuronal circuits [15]. Indeed, the left caudate nucleus, the ventral striatum and the putamen show significantly greater activation during trials in which the hypothetical rewards are associated with delays shorter than one year compared to those with delays longer than one year [69]. Finally, other confounds are introduced by the compositions of experimental groups, some of which include patients with psychiatric and/or neurological conditions.
Furthermore, investigating the roles of areas other than the dlPFC seems very promising, these areas being underrepresented. OFC activity, above all, seems to be engaged in DD, both in humans [9,10,22,23,30,70] and animals. Indeed, the optogenetical stimulation of serotoninergic terminals of the OFC, rather than the NAc and mPFC, promotes waiting for behavior in intertemporal choice tasks [71]. Being highly interconnected with the VTA through the NAc, dlPFC, DRN, amygdala and hippocampus [72], the OFC can easily access all the information required for the expression of functional DD and at the same time it can exert top-down control on areas involved in the encoding of reward values. Specifically, the medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is a component of the corticostriatal circuit implicated in the assessment of the subjective value of rewards and is thus likely to be involved in delay discounting [72].
Furthermore, the OFC is particularly vulnerable to age-related reduction in gray matter volume [74], possibly explaining why older people prefer delayed rather than immediate rewards. Finally, the WM of the OFC is altered in patients with anorexia nervosa, who, contrary to most of the psychiatric population, show a preference for most delayed rewards [75]. Unfortunately, only a few studies have investigated the effects of non-invasive neuromodulation of this region on DD (Figure 2).
Nevertheless, based on the available evidence, one could hardly formulate a theoretical model that describes the functioning of DD, since intertemporal choice tasks are likely to involve the interaction of complex systems comprising several brain regions. Hence, we believe that future studies should not only better elucidate the role of the OFC in this context, but also address the potential routes of its interaction with other brain areas known to be pivotal in decision making.
A potential caveat could arise from the poor comprehension of the effect of TMS and tDCS protocols on targeted neuronal circuits. For instance, there is a lack of consensus among authors regarding the extent to which certain TMS protocols can simulate a “virtual lesion” [76]. Additionally, in the reviewed experiments, delay discounting is evaluated either in real time, i.e., during the administration of the stimulation protocol, or after prolonged exposure to treatment.
The active manipulation of brain regions offers a more comprehensive means of dissecting and comprehending delay discounting than a solely descriptive approach, such as neuroimaging. Furthermore, given that delay discounting is compromised in a number of treatment-resistant psychiatric conditions, such as addiction [7], the utilization of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques may hold potential for clinical applications, as the rehabilitation of this psychological function may result in improved patient outcomes.
Conceptualization: A.S.M., D.S., M.F. and J.L.; writing—original draft preparation: A.S.M.; writing—review and editing: A.S.M., D.S. and J.L.; supervision: M.F., S.S., J.L. and A.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Not applicable.
Not applicable.
Data sharing is not applicable to this article, as no new data were created or analyzed in this study.
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Figure 1. Intertemporal choice tasks for characterizing delay discounting in decision making. (a) An exemplary question from an intertemporal choice task. (b) The hyperbolic discounting function fitted from the indifference points obtained from the intertemporal choice task.
Figure 2. Cortical areas involved in delay discounting. A qualitative reconstruction was obtained using the Desikan–Killiany cortical atlas and BrainPainter [73] to summarize the areas involved in delay discounting determined by neuroimaging studies (green areas) and non-invasive brain imaging (NIBS; red areas); yellow areas indicate the overlap between the two.
References
1. Mischel, W.; Shoda, Y.; Rodriguez, M.L. Delay of Gratification in Children. Science; 1989; 244, pp. 933-938. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.2658056]
2. Mazur, J.E. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. The Effect of Delay and of Intervening Events on Reinforcement Value; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1987; pp. 55-73.
3. Vanderveldt, A.; Oliveira, L.; Green, L. Delay discounting: Pigeon, rat, human—Does it matter?. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Learn. Cogn.; 2016; 42, pp. 141-162. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000097]
4. Hernandez, C.M.; Orsini, C.A.; Labiste, C.C.; Wheeler, A.R.; Eyck, T.W.T.; Bruner, M.M.; Sahagian, T.J.; Harden, S.W.; Frazier, C.J.; Setlow, B. et al. Optogenetic dissection of basolateral amygdala contributions to intertemporal choice in young and aged rats. Elife; 2019; 8, e46174. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46174]
5. de Wit, H. Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: A review of underlying processes. Addict. Biol.; 2009; 14, pp. 22-31. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00129.x] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18855805]
6. Castellanos-Ryan, N.; Struve, M.; Whelan, R.; Banaschewski, T.; Barker, G.J.; Bokde, A.L.W.; Bromberg, U.; Büchel, C.; Flor, H.; Fauth-Bühler, M. et al. Neural and Cognitive Correlates of the Common and Specific Variance Across Externalizing Problems in Young Adolescence. Am. J. Psychiatry; 2014; 171, pp. 1310-1319. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13111499]
7. Amlung, M.; Marsden, E.; Holshausen, K.; Morris, V.; Patel, H.; Vedelago, L.; Naish, K.R.; Reed, D.D.; McCabe, R.E. Delay Discounting as a Transdiagnostic Process in Psychiatric Disorders: A Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry; 2019; 76, pp. 1176-1186. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2102] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31461131]
8. Lamanna, J.; Sulpizio, S.; Ferro, M.; Martoni, R.; Abutalebi, J.; Malgaroli, A. Behavioral assessment of activity-based-anorexia: How cognition can become the drive wheel. Physiol. Behav.; 2019; 202, pp. 1-7. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.01.016] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30682332]
9. McClure, S.M.; Laibson, D.I.; Loewenstein, G.; Cohen, J.D. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science; 2004; 306, pp. 503-507. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1100907]
10. Kable, J.W.; Glimcher, P.W. The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nat. Neurosci.; 2007; 10, pp. 1625-1633. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn2007]
11. Ballard, K.; Knutson, B. Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting. Neuroimage; 2009; 45, pp. 143-150. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.004] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19071223]
12. Figner, B.; Knoch, D.; Johnson, E.J.; Krosch, A.R.; Lisanby, S.H.; Fehr, E.; Weber, E.U. Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice. Nat. Neurosci.; 2010; 13, pp. 538-539. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2516]
13. Nejati, V.; Salehinejad, M.A.; Nitsche, M.A. Interaction of the Left Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex (l-DLPFC) and Right Orbitofrontal Cortex (OFC) in Hot and Cold Executive Functions: Evidence from Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS). Neuroscience; 2018; 369, pp. 109-123. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.10.042]
14. Scherbaum, S.; Frisch, S.; Dshemuchadse, M. Step by step: Harvesting the dynamics of delay discounting decisions. Q. J. Exp. Psychol.; 2018; 71, pp. 949-964. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1307863] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28300478]
15. Frost, R.; McNaughton, N. The neural basis of delay discounting: A review and preliminary model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.; 2017; 79, pp. 48-65. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.022] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28465167]
16. Heekeren, H.R.; Marrett, S.; Ungerleider, L.G. The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.; 2008; 9, pp. 467-479. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2374] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18464792]
17. Yu, R. Regional white matter volumes correlate with delay discounting. PLoS ONE; 2012; 7. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032595] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22393420]
18. Bernhardt, B.C.; Smallwood, J.; Tusche, A.; Ruby, F.J.M.; Engen, H.G.; Steinbeis, N.; Singer, T. Medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortical thickness predicts shared individual differences in self-generated thought and temporal discounting. Neuroimage; 2014; 90, pp. 290-297. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.040]
19. Moschak, T.M.; Carelli, R.M. Impulsive rats exhibit blunted dopamine release dynamics during a delay discounting task independent of cocaine history. eNeuro; 2017; 4, pp. 1-12. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0119-17.2017]
20. Xu, S.; Das, G.; Hueske, E.; Tonegawa, S. Dorsal Raphe Serotonergic Neurons Control Intertemporal Choice under Trade-off. Curr. Biol.; 2017; 27, pp. 3111-3119.e3. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.008]
21. Gao, Z.; Wang, H.; Lu, C.; Lu, T.; Froudist-Walsh, S.; Chen, M.; Wang, X.J.; Hu, J.; Sun, W. The neural basis of delayed gratification. Sci. Adv.; 2021; 7, 111470. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abg6611]
22. McClure, S.M.; Ericson, K.M.; Laibson, D.I.; Loewenstein, G.; Cohen, J.D. Time discounting for primary rewards. J. Neurosci.; 2007; 27, pp. 5796-5804. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4246-06.2007] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17522323]
23. Peters, J.; Büchel, C. Overlapping and distinct neural systems code for subjective value during intertemporal and risky decision making. J. Neurosci.; 2009; 29, pp. 15727-15734. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3489-09.2009]
24. Casey, B.J.; Somerville, L.H.; Gotlib, I.H.; Ayduk, O.; Franklin, N.T.; Askren, M.K.; Jonides, J.; Berman, M.G.; Wilson, N.L.; Teslovich, T. et al. Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA; 2011; 108, pp. 14998-15003. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1108561108]
25. Hare, T.A.; Hakimi, S.; Rangel, A. Activity in dlPFC and its effective connectivity to vmPFC are associated with temporal discounting. Front. Neurosci.; 2014; 8, 50. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2014.00050]
26. van den Bos, W.; Rodriguez, C.A.; Schweitzer, J.B.; McClure, S.M. Connectivity strength of dissociable striatal tracts predict individual differences in temporal discounting. J. Neurosci.; 2014; 34, pp. 10298-10310. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4105-13.2014] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25080591]
27. Szuhany, K.L.; MacKenzie, D.; Otto, M.W. The impact of depressed mood, working memory capacity, and priming on delay discounting. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry; 2018; 60, pp. 37-41. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.03.001]
28. Peters, J.; Büchel, C. Episodic Future Thinking Reduces Reward Delay Discounting through an Enhancement of Prefrontal-Mediotemporal Interactions. Neuron; 2010; 66, pp. 138-148. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.026] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20399735]
29. Fellows, L.K.; Farah, M.J. Dissociable elements of human foresight: A role for the ventromedial frontal lobes in framing the future, but not in discounting future rewards. Neuropsychologia; 2005; 43, pp. 1214-1221. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.07.018]
30. Sellitto, M.; Ciaramelli, E.; Di Pellegrino, G. Myopic discounting of future rewards after medial orbitofrontal damage in humans. J. Neurosci.; 2010; 30, pp. 16429-16436. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2516-10.2010]
31. Rochkind, S. Photobiomodulation in Neuroscience: A Summary of Personal Experience. Photomed. Laser Surg.; 2017; 35, pp. 604-615. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1089/pho.2017.4381]
32. Chen, Z.; Tsytsarev, V.; Finfrock, Y.Z.; Antipova, O.A.; Cai, Z.; Arakawa, H.; Lischka, F.W.; Hooks, B.M.; Wilton, R.; Wang, D. et al. Wireless Optogenetic Modulation of Cortical Neurons Enabled by Radioluminescent Nanoparticles. ACS Nano; 2021; 15, pp. 5201-5208. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1021/acsnano.0c10436] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33625219]
33. Sahel, J.-A.; Boulanger-Scemama, E.; Pagot, C.; Arleo, A.; Galluppi, F.; Martel, J.N.; Esposti, S.D.; Delaux, A.; de Saint Aubert, J.-B.; de Montleau, C. et al. Partial recovery of visual function in a blind patient after optogenetic therapy. Nat. Med.; 2021; 27, pp. 1223-1229. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41591-021-01351-4] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34031601]
34. Darmani, G.; Bergmann, T.O.; Butts Pauly, K.; Caskey, C.F.; de Lecea, L.; Fomenko, A.; Fouragnan, E.; Legon, W.; Murphy, K.R.; Nandi, T. et al. Non-invasive transcranial ultrasound stimulation for neuromodulation. Clin. Neurophysiol.; 2022; 135, pp. 51-73. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2021.12.010] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35033772]
35. Rossi, S.; Antal, A.; Bestmann, S.; Bikson, M.; Brewer, C.; Brockmöller, J.; Carpenter, L.I.; Cincotta, M.; Chen, R.; Daskalakis, J.D. et al. Safety and recommendations for TMS use in healthy subjects and patient populations, with updates on training, ethical and regulatory issues: Expert Guidelines. Clin. Neurophysiol.; 2021; 132, pp. 269-306. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2020.10.003] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33243615]
36. Ferro, M.; Lamanna, J.; Spadini, S.; Nespoli, A.; Sulpizio, S.; Malgaroli, A. Synaptic plasticity mechanisms behind TMS efficacy: Insights from its application to animal models. J. Neural Transm.; 2022; 129, pp. 25-36. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00702-021-02436-7]
37. Garcia-Sanz, S.; Ghotme, K.A.; Hedmont, D.; Arévalo-Jaimes, M.Y.; Cohen Kadosh, R.; Serra-Grabulosa, J.M.; Redolar-Ripoll, D. Use of transcranial magnetic stimulation for studying the neural basis of numerical cognition: A systematic review. J. Neurosci. Methods; 2022; 369, 109485. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2022.109485]
38. Bestmann, S. The physiological basis of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Trends Cogn. Sci.; 2008; 12, pp. 81-83. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.12.002]
39. Di Lazzaro, V.; Dileone, M.; Pilato, F.; Capone, F.; Musumeci, G.; Ranieri, F.; Ricci, V.; Bria, P.; Di Iorio, R.; de Waure, C. et al. Modulation of motor cortex neuronal networks by rTMS: Comparison of local and remote effects of six different protocols of stimulation. J. Neurophysiol.; 2011; 105, pp. 2150-2156. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00781.2010]
40. Teti Mayer, J.; Nicolier, M.; Tio, G.; Mouchabac, S.; Haffen, E.; Bennabi, D. Effects of High Frequency Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (HF-rTMS) on Delay Discounting in Major Depressive Disorder. Encephale; 2019; 45, S68. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.encep.2019.04.013]
41. Zack, M.; Cho, S.S.; Parlee, J.; Jacobs, M.; Li, C.; Boileau, I.; Strafella, A. Effects of High Frequency Repeated Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation and Continuous Theta Burst Stimulation on Gambling Reinforcement, Delay Discounting, and Stroop Interference in Men with Pathological Gambling. Brain Stimul.; 2016; 9, pp. 867-875. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2016.06.003]
42. Sheffer, C.E.; Mennemeier, M.; Landes, R.D.; Bickel, W.K.; Brackman, S.; Dornhoffer, J.; Kimbrell, T.; Brown, G. Neuromodulation of delay discounting, the reflection effect, and cigarette consumption. J. Subst. Abuse Treat.; 2013; 45, pp. 206-214. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsat.2013.01.012] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23518286]
43. Cho, S.S.; Ko, J.H.; Pellecchia, G.; Van Eimeren, T.; Cilia, R.; Strafella, A.P. Continuous theta burst stimulation of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex induces changes in impulsivity level. Brain Stimul.; 2010; 3, pp. 170-176. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2009.10.002] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20633446]
44. Schluter, R.S.; van Holst, R.J.; Goudriaan, A.E. Effects of Ten Sessions of High Frequency Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (HF-rTMS) Add-on Treatment on Impulsivity in Alcohol Use Disorder. Front. Neurosci.; 2019; 13, 1257. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.01257]
45. Essex, B.G.; Clinton, S.A.; Wonderley, L.R.; Zald, D.H. The impact of the posterior parietal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices on the optimization of long-term versus immediate value. J. Neurosci.; 2012; 32, pp. 15403-15413. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6106-11.2012] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23115178]
46. Ballard, I.C.; Aydogan, G.; Kim, B.; McClure, S.M. Causal Evidence for the Dependence of the Magnitude Effect on Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex. Sci. Rep.; 2018; 8, 16545. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-34900-y] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30410093]
47. Cho, S.S.; Pellecchia, G.; Ko, J.H.; Ray, N.; Obeso, I.; Houle, S.; Strafella, A.P. Effect of continuous theta burst stimulation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on cerebral blood flow changes during decision making. Brain Stimul.; 2012; 5, pp. 116-123. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.007]
48. Yang, C.C.; Khalifa, N.; Lankappa, S.; Völlm, B. Effects of intermittent theta burst stimulation applied to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on empathy and impulsivity in healthy adult males. Brain Cogn.; 2018; 128, pp. 37-45. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2018.11.003]
49. Cho, S.S.; Koshimori, Y.; Aminian, K.; Obeso, I.; Rusjan, P.; Lang, A.E.; Daskalakis, Z.J.; Houle, S.; Strafella, A.P. Investing in the future: Stimulation of the medial prefrontal cortex reduces discounting of delayed rewards. Neuropsychopharmacology; 2015; 40, pp. 546-553. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.211]
50. Soutschek, A.; Ruff, C.C.; Strombach, T.; Kalenscher, T.; Tobler, P.N. Brain stimulation reveals crucial role of overcoming self-centeredness in self-control. Sci. Adv.; 2016; 2, pp. 2-10. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1600992]
51. Do, M.; Byrne, L.K.; Pearce, A.J. Examining the feasibility and tolerability of a clinically informed multisite, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation protocol. J. Neurosci. Methods; 2016; 258, pp. 24-27. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.10.013]
52. Das, S.; Holland, P.; Frens, M.A.; Donchin, O. Impact of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) on Neuronal Functions. Front. Neurosci.; 2016; 10, 550. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00550] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27965533]
53. Brunyé, T.T. Non-invasive Brain Stimulation Effects on the Perceptual and Cognitive Processes Underlying Decision-making: A Mini Review. J. Cogn. Enhanc.; 2021; 5, pp. 233-244. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s41465-020-00186-0]
54. He, Q.; Chen, M.; Chen, C.; Xue, G.; Feng, T.; Bechara, A. Anodal stimulation of the left DLPFC increases IGT scores and decreases delay discounting rate in healthy males. Front. Psychol.; 2016; 7, 1421. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01421] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27703440]
55. Shen, B.; Yin, Y.; Wang, J.; Zhou, X.; McClure, S.M.; Li, J. High-definition tDCS alters impulsivity in a baseline-dependent manner. Neuroimage; 2016; 143, pp. 343-352. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.006] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27608604]
56. Kekic, M.; McClelland, J.; Bartholdy, S.; Boysen, E.; Musiat, P.; Dalton, B.; Tiza, M.; David, A.S.; Campbell, I.C.; Schmidt, U. Single-session transcranial direct current stimulation temporarily improves symptoms, mood, and self-regulatory control in bulimia nervosa: A randomised controlled trial. PLoS ONE; 2017; 12, e0167606. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167606] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28121991]
57. Xiong, G.; Li, X.; Dong, Z.; Cai, S.; Huang, J.; Li, Q. Modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex changes intertemporal choice for loss: A transcranial direct current stimulation study. Front. Hum. Neurosci.; 2019; 13, 167. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00167]
58. Brunelin, J.; Fecteau, S. Impact of bifrontal transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on decision-making and stress reactivity. A pilot study. J. Psychiatr. Res.; 2021; 135, pp. 15-19. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.12.068]
59. Colombo, B.; Iannello, P.; Puglisi, G.; Antonietti, A. Personal Impulsivity Mediates the Effects of Neuromodulation in Economic Intertemporal Choices: A Pilot Study. Stud. Psychol.; 2020; 62, pp. 123-137. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.31577/sp.2020.02.795]
60. Nejati, V.; Sarraj Khorrami, A.; Nitsche, M.A. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Improves Reward Processing in Children With ADHD. J. Atten. Disord.; 2021; 25, pp. 1623-1631. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1087054720923094]
61. Terenzi, D.; Catalan, M.; Polverino, P.; Bertolotti, C.; Manganotti, P.; Rumiati, R.I.; Aiello, M. Effects of tDCS on reward responsiveness and valuation in Parkinson’s patients with impulse control disorders. J. Neurol.; 2022; 269, pp. 1557-1565. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00415-021-10733-0]
62. Manuel, A.L.; Murray, N.W.G.; Piguet, O. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over vmPFC modulates interactions between reward and emotion in delay discounting. Sci. Rep.; 2019; 9, 18735. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-55157-z]
63. Wynn, S.C.; Driessen, J.M.A.; Glennon, J.C.; Brazil, I.A.; Schutter, D.J.L.G. Cerebellar Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Improves Reactive Response Inhibition in Healthy Volunteers. Cerebellum; 2019; 18, pp. 983-988. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12311-019-01047-z]
64. To, C.; Falcone, M.; Loughead, J.; Logue-Chamberlain, E.; Hamilton, R.; Kable, J.; Lerman, C.; Ashare, R.L. Got chocolate? Bilateral prefrontal cortex stimulation augments chocolate consumption. Appetite; 2018; 131, pp. 28-35. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.08.032]
65. Sparing, R.; Mottaghy, F.M. Noninvasive brain stimulation with transcranial magnetic or direct current stimulation (TMS/tDCS)—From insights into human memory to therapy of its dysfunction. Methods; 2008; 44, pp. 329-337. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ymeth.2007.02.001] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18374276]
66. Huang, Y.; Datta, A.; Bikson, M.; Parra, L.C. ROAST: An Open-Source, Fully-Automated, Realistic Volumetric-Approach-Based Simulator For TES. Proceedings of the 2018 40th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC); Honolulu, HI, USA, 18–21 July 2018; pp. 3072-3075.
67. Kekic, M.; McClelland, J.; Campbell, I.; Nestler, S.; Rubia, K.; David, A.S.; Schmidt, U. The effects of prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on food craving and temporal discounting in women with frequent food cravings. Appetite; 2014; 78, pp. 55-62. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.03.010]
68. Soutschek, A.; Ugazio, G.; Crockett, M.J.; Ruff, C.C.; Kalenscher, T.; Tobler, P.N. Binding oneself to the mast: Stimulating frontopolar cortex enhances precommitment. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci.; 2017; 12, pp. 635-642. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw176]
69. Wittmann, M.; Leland, D.S.; Paulus, M.P. Time and decision making: Differential contribution of the posterior insular cortex and the striatum during a delay discounting task. Exp. Brain Res.; 2007; 179, pp. 643-653. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0822-y] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17216152]
70. Schoenbaum, G.; Shaham, Y. The Role of Orbitofrontal Cortex in Drug Addiction: A Review of Preclinical Studies. Biol. Psychiatry; 2008; 63, pp. 256-262. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.003]
71. Miyazaki, K.; Miyazaki, K.W.; Sivori, G.; Yamanaka, A.; Tanaka, K.F.; Doya, K. Serotonergic projections to the orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortices differentially modulate waiting for future rewards. Sci. Adv.; 2020; 6, eabc7246. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abc7246] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33246957]
72. Fettes, P.; Schulze, L.; Downar, J. Cortico-Striatal-Thalamic Loop Circuits of the Orbitofrontal Cortex: Promising Therapeutic Targets in Psychiatric Illness. Front. Syst. Neurosci.; 2017; 11, 25. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2017.00025]
73. Marinescu, R.V.; Eshaghi, A.; Alexander, D.C.; Golland, P. BrainPainter: A software for the visualisation of brain structures, biomarkers and associated pathological processes. Proceedings of the Multimodal Brain Image Analysis and Mathematical Foundations of Computational Anatomy: 4th International Workshop, MBIA 2019, and 7th International Workshop, MFCA 2019; Shenzhen, China, 17 October 2019; pp. 112-120.
74. McNamara, R.K.; Liu, Y.; Jandacek, R.; Rider, T.; Tso, P. The aging human orbitofrontal cortex: Decreasing polyunsaturated fatty acid composition and associated increases in lipogenic gene expression and stearoyl-CoA desaturase activity. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fat. Acids; 2008; 78, pp. 293-304. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.plefa.2008.04.001] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18499418]
75. Shott, M.E.; Pryor, T.L.; Yang, T.T.; Frank, G.K.W. Greater Insula White Matter Fiber Connectivity in Women Recovered from Anorexia Nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology; 2016; 41, pp. 498-507. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.172] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26076832]
76. Miniussi, C.; Ruzzoli, M.; Walsh, V. The mechanism of transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognition. Cortex; 2010; 46, pp. 128-130. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.03.004] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19356747]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
In decision making, the subjective value of a reward declines with the delay to its receipt, describing a hyperbolic function. Although this phenomenon, referred to as delay discounting (DD), has been extensively characterized and reported in many animal species, still, little is known about the neuronal processes that support it. Here, after drawing a comprehensive portrait, we consider the latest neuroimaging and lesion studies, the outcomes of which often appear contradictory among comparable experimental settings. In the second part of the manuscript, we focus on a more recent and effective route of investigation: non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS). We provide a comprehensive review of the available studies that applied transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to affect subjects’ performance in DD tasks. The aim of our survey is not only to highlight the superiority of NIBS in investigating DD, but also to suggest targets for future experimental studies, since the regions considered in these studies represent only a fraction of the possible ones. In particular, we argue that, based on the available neurophysiological evidence from lesion and brain imaging studies, a very promising and underrepresented region for future neuromodulation studies investigating DD is the orbitofrontal cortex.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details



1 Department of Psychology, Sigmund Freud University of Milan, 20143 Milan, Italy; Center for Behavioral Neuroscience and Communication (BNC), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, 20132 Milan, Italy
2 Department of Psychology, Sigmund Freud University of Milan, 20143 Milan, Italy
3 Department of Psychology, Sigmund Freud University of Milan, 20143 Milan, Italy; Child and Youth Lab, Sigmund Freud University of Milan, 20143 Milan, Italy
4 Center for Behavioral Neuroscience and Communication (BNC), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, 20132 Milan, Italy; Faculty of Psychology, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, 20132 Milan, Italy