Species with slow life history strategies typically have few offspring and high investment in both social relationships and parental care. Consequently species with slow life history strategies are less able to compensate for increased mortality rates (Purvis et al., 2000), including mortality associated with human disturbance (Quetglas et al., 2016; Wheeler et al., 2020). For example, fishing affects slow-strategy species (elasmobranchs) but not fast-strategy species (cephalopods), due to differences in generation times and population turnover (Quetglas et al., 2016). For threatened carnivore and primate species, a long gestation is correlated with higher extinction risk (Purvis, 2001) which may be exacerbated for animals using human-modified landscapes that are subject to rapid changes in the selective environment (Palumbi, 2001). Therefore, species with slow strategies may have to adjust their behavior within their lifetime (Sih et al., 2011) to survive in human-modified landscapes, which often manifests as crop- or urban-foraging behavior (Bateman & Fleming, 2012; Chiyo et al., 2012; Fehlmann et al., 2021; McLennan et al., 2017; Moss et al., 2016; Warren et al., 2007).
Chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) are a typical slow life history species, living in cohesive mixed-sex social groups with strong social relationships (Cheney et al., 2016; Silk et al., 2010). Female baboons show high maternal investment (Altmann & Samuels, 1992) giving birth to a single infant after a gestation period of 6 months (Altmann et al., 1977; Archie et al., 2014), carrying the infant for several months after birth (Altmann & Samuels, 1992), and lactating for up to a year (Archie et al., 2014; Zipple et al., 2017). Mothers reduce activity levels in the first few months after birth (Barrett et al., 2006) because of both high energetic costs due to lactation (Altmann, 1983; Barrett et al., 2006) and the risk of infanticide by adult males (Zipple et al., 2017). As is the case with other primates (Dunayer & Berman, 2018), new mothers receive increased social attention as other individuals inspect and handle their offspring (Altmann, 2001; Henzi & Barrett, 2002).
Chacma baboons living at the edge of the City of Cape Town, South Africa, frequently use urban spaces (Bracken et al., 2021; Fehlmann, O'Riain, Kerr-Smith, Hailes, et al., 2017), resulting in high levels of spatial overlap and negative interactions with people (Hoffman & O'Riain, 2012). Human residents and land-owners often experience baboon-caused damage to their property and economic losses (e.g., when baboons forage in commercial vineyards) (Fehlmann, O'Riain, Kerr-Smith, Hailes, et al., 2017; Kaplan et al., 2011; van Doorn & O'Riain, 2020). Consequently, residents harbor negative attitudes toward baboons (Mormile & Hill, 2017), and baboons experience high levels of human-induced mortality and injury (Beamish, 2009). While urban spaces in Cape Town can provide baboons with higher energy foods (Fehlmann et al., 2021; Fehlmann, O'Riain, Kerr-Smith, Hailes, et al., 2017; Hoffman & O'Riain, 2012; Kaplan et al., 2011) they are also more costly than other habitats because of increased activity demands and risks of being “herded” away from urban space by the employed “baboon rangers” (Fehlmann, O'Riain, Kerr-Smith, Hailes, et al., 2017; Fehlmann, O'Riain, Kerr-Smith, & King, 2017). To exploit foraging opportunities presented by urban space while avoiding detection by the rangers, baboons living at the edge of the City of Cape Town therefore tend to make short visits to urban space alone or in small groups (Bracken et al., 2021; Fehlmann, O'Riain, Kerr-Smith, Hailes, et al., 2017).
During the study of a baboon troop (n = 21 adults; 2 males and 19 females) that ranges within and on the periphery of the Da Gama Park suburb of Cape Town, several females gave birth (Figure 1), one of which (a low-ranking [ranked 13 out of 19 females] female baboon: F13) was wearing a GPS-equipped tracking collar (collars were deployed on n = 16 adults [Bracken et al., 2021; Bracken et al., 2022; McCann et al., 2021]; see Appendix). The birth occurred 32 days into the time the female's GPS was recording (she was confirmed as “with infant” on 03/09/2018), with a further 16 days of GPS data collected after birth (after which her GPS stopped recording continuous data). These GPS data provided an opportunity to examine potential changes in this female's use of natural and urban space. By creating a polygon around the urban/natural space in QGIS (QGIS.org, 2020), we calculated the time the female spent in urban space using the package “Recurse” in R (Bracis et al., 2018), and created a 95% home range (estimated using fixed kernel densities: Calenge, 2006), based on movements before and after birth. Additionally, since giving birth can reduce activity levels in new baboon mothers (Barrett et al., 2006), we calculated daily distance traveled before and after parturition using the function “as.ltraj” in the “adehabitat” package in R (Calenge, 2006). Lastly, because giving birth is associated with changes in social interactions (Alberts, 2019; Seyfarth, 1978), we estimated individual strength (Morrow et al., 2019; Peignier et al., 2019; Silk et al., 2003) and eigenvector centrality (Bracken et al., 2021; Morrow et al., 2019) based on a network of associations (proximity-based using the package “Spatsoc” in R, see: Bracken et al., 2021, 2022) before and after birth, for collared individuals with overlapping data for both time periods (n = 11 individuals). At the time of our study, the troop contained two adult males (the alpha: M1, and the beta: M2) with an unstable dominance relationship (Bracken et al., 2021). The beta male attacked a (non-collared) female's infant during the study period with the infant dying 10 days later. For further details on our analyses, see Appendix.
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
We found that the female baboon stopped using urban space after birth (Figure 2a) while other individuals continued to do so (Figures A1 and A2). A similar result was obtained when randomly sub-sampling the “before birth” period and analyzing the same number of GPS days (n = 16) before and after birth (Figure A3). The female's daily travel distances were not affected (Figure 2b), though her home range after birth reflected the noticeable absence of urban space use (Figure 2c). The female's association patterns within the troop were similar before and after birth, and while her relative strength and centrality scores increased in the period after birth, this increase in strength and centrality was seen across all individuals (Figure 2d,e).
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
To our knowledge, this is the first documented evidence of cessation of urban space use by an animal after giving birth. Because this female's change of urban space use occurs without any significant change in distance traveled or social association patterns, it is unlikely that the change is a consequence of a general risk-sensitive behavior strategy after birth. Instead, it is likely that the risks specific to, and associated with, the urban space relative to natural space led to adapted movement decisions by the mother with a vulnerable infant. Though we only have GPS data for one baboon, we observed the same pattern of urban avoidance postpartum in two other non-collared females during the study (A. M. Bracken & C. Christensen, personal observation).
In our study system, the urban space poses specific risks in the form of residents, dogs, vehicles, electric fences, and herding behavior by designated field rangers employed to reduce baboons' urban space use (Beamish & O'Riain, 2014). These risks are absent or reduced in natural space (Bracken et al., 2022; Fehlmann, O'Riain, Kerr-Smith, Hailes, et al., 2017; van Doorn & O'Riain, 2020). Additionally, urban space use tends to cause group fragmentation, both when the whole troop is in town (Bracken et al., 2022), and when individuals split from the troop in natural space to use urban space (Bracken et al., 2021). We have previously shown in the troop studied here, that these individuals tend to be socially peripheral, low-ranking females, which capitalize on the opportunity to use urban space (Bracken et al., 2021), as management tends to focus on adult males. Additionally, postpartum, females may be subject to infanticide risk when using urban space if they become isolated from the troop and, by extension, the protection of the alpha male (who is herded out by management). Avoidance of urban space due to infanticide from males has also been observed in female black bears (Merkle et al., 2013). The beta male in our study troop posed an infanticidal threat, and while we do not know whether the alpha or beta male sired F13's infant (which we focus on here), a high risk of infant mortality from infanticide attempts (Cheney et al., 2006; Palombit, 2003; van Doorn et al., 2010) and defense of infants by alpha males from attacks by other males (Cowlishaw, 1999) may have contributed to F13 remaining with the main troop and using natural space more after birth.
We therefore suggest that the specific risks baboons encounter in urban space as a consequence of their interaction with people (Kaburu et al., 2019) and the urban environment (Soulsbury & White, 2016) combined with group fragmentation and reduced protection of the alpha male when in urban space (Bracken et al., 2021, 2022) resulted in the female's decision to avoid urban space. This observation is important for baboon management in the region. The Cape baboon population is growing consistently (Beamish & O'Riain, in press) with numbers of individuals in managed peri-urban troops increasing from 248 individuals in 2006 to 445 in 2020 (in TimesLive, 2020). Consequently, contraception has been proposed as a potential non-lethal mechanism to restrict numbers within troops on the Cape (O'Riain, 2021; Richardson, 2020). While contraception has been successfully used for captive baboon species (Plowman et al., 2005), it is unclear how it would influence the behavior and movement of females who use the urban space as much as, or more than, males (Bracken et al., 2021). Indeed, our findings suggest that if females are prevented from reproducing, this could translate to even more urban space use by the City's chacma baboons. Additionally, since reproduction is fundamental for female baboons' life histories (signaling female quality: Domb & Pagel, 2001 and driving social relationships between males: Seyfarth, 1978 and other females: Seyfarth, 1976), suppression of the reproductive cycle may have a number of unintended social consequences.
We hope this study will initiate further research into how animals, which invest heavily in parental care, are responding to the benefits and risks associated with human-modified landscapes. For slow strategy species, parental care is essential to infant survival and in this study, we suggest that the reduction in female urban space use postpartum is an adaptive behavioral response that will improve both her and her offspring's survival. A larger sample size of data for female baboons that are using urban areas before and after birth will provide important insights not only into how wildlife is responding to the Anthropocene, but also into the methods used for managing wildlife on the urban edge.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Anna M. Bracken: Conceptualization (equal); data curation (equal); formal analysis (lead); investigation (equal); methodology (equal); visualization (lead); writing – original draft (lead). Charlotte Christensen: Data curation (equal); investigation (equal); writing – review and editing (equal). M. Justin O'Riain: Project administration (equal); writing – review and editing (equal). Ines Fürtbauer: Funding acquisition (equal); project administration (equal); resources (equal); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (equal). Andrew J. King: Conceptualization (equal); funding acquisition (equal); methodology (equal); project administration (equal); resources (equal); supervision (lead); visualization (supporting); writing – review and editing (equal).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Fieldwork was approved by the Baboon Technical Team (BTT) in the Cape Peninsula and by Research Agreement with South African National Parks (SANParks). We thank Gaëlle Fehlmann, Phil Hopkins, and Mark Holton for help and advice with building the tracking collars. We thank Human Wildlife Solutions and their rangers, veterinarian Dorothy Breed, Gary Buhrman, and Esme Beamish for their assistance in the field; and Lucy Robertson, Charlotte Solman, and Francesca Marshall-Stochmal for assistance with fieldwork. AMB thanks Alexis Malagnino for his help and advice with GPS processing, AJK and IF thank Layla King for her support. MJO was supported by NRF incentive funding. AMB and CC were supported by College of Science/Swansea University PhD scholarships.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT
The authors declare no competing interests.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
All data to reproduce results are available in Dryad: .
APPENDIX
Methods
GPS data
N = 16 adult baboons were fitted with SHOAL group in-house constructed collars (F2HKv3), which recorded GPS positional data at 1 fix/second between 08:00 and 20:00 local time (GiPSy 5 tags; TechnoSmArt). We use “daytime” hours for the focal female and troop analyses (08:00 – 18:00 local time) as baboons were often at their urban sleepsite after 18:00. Data from 15 collars were retrieved (one collar was not found after release) and two collars failed to record GPS, resulting in individual GPS data for n = 13 baboons (which represented 61% of all adults in the troop), over mean ± SD of 42.77 ± 9.92 days, range = 21–54 days (Table A1). For the adult females in the troop that had GPS-recording collars during our study, five had offspring and were lactating (ages unknown, but at least 7 months old, as all had brown adult coloration: Dezeure et al. (2021) and were present at the start of the field season), and five did not have dependent offspring. Only one female (the focal female in this study) gave birth when the GPS was recording (going from “without infant” to “with infant” on 03/09/2018).
Urban space and home range
We defined the focal females 95% home ranges before (32 days) and after (16 days) birth, using fixed kernel densities and an ad hoc method for selecting the smoothing parameter, using the function “getvolumeUD” in the “adehabitat” package, R (Calenge, 2006). Within the home range, we designated urban space as the area dominated by residential buildings and surfaced roads, and drew a polygon around this using QGIS (QGIS.org, 2020). All areas outside this urban space were defined as natural space (Figure 2c). To estimate the females’ proportion of the day spent in urban space, we used the function “getRecursionsInPolygon” from the package “Recurse” in R (Bracis et al., 2018). To estimate the females’ daily distances travelled we used the function “as.ltraj” in the “adehabitat” package in R (Calenge, 2006).
Spatial associations
We extracted individual strength and eigenvector centrality from proximity-based social networks using the package “Spatsoc” in R (Robitaille et al., 2019) for n = 11 individuals, constructed before and after the focal female gave birth (mean ± SE minutes before birth: 12220 ± 76; mean ± SE minutes after birth: 1527 ± 34). Two females were removed from analyses as they did not have data after birth (F9 & F14; see Figure A1). Networks were based on times all baboons were outside the urban polygon (as proximity to humans is known to affect social networks: Bracken et al., 2022; Morrow et al., 2019) and for times when 10+ collars were active and recording synchronous data (see: Bracken et al., 2021). GPS locations were grouped temporally (1-min intervals) and spatially (within 5 m, using the “chain rule”: Castles et al., 2014). Network edges were weighted using the simple ratio index.
TABLE A1 Details of the GPS recording period for each collared adult baboon (F = female, M = male in column “Individual”).
Collar | Individual | GPS start | GPS end | Total duration (days) |
1 | M1 | 30-07-2018 | 11-09-2018 | 44 |
2 | M2 | 30-07-2018 | 10-09-2018 | 43 |
3 | F1 | no data | ||
4 | F2 | 25-07-2018 | 13-09-2018 | 50 |
5 | F4 | no data | ||
6 | F5 | 26-07-2018 | 09-09-2018 | 46 |
7 | F6 | 26-07-2018 | 07-09-2018 | 44 |
8 | F7 | 26-07-2018 | 09-09-2018 | 46 |
9 | F9 | 26-07-2018 | 15-08-2018 | 21 |
10 | F10 | 30-07-2018 | 12-09-2018 | 45 |
11 | F13 | 02-08-2018 | 24-09-2018 | 54 |
12 | F14 | 02-08-2018 | 25-08-2018 | 24 |
13 | F15 | 26-07-2018 | 07-09-2018 | 44 |
14 | F17 | 02-08-2018 | 19-09-2018 | 49 |
15 | F18 | 26-07-2018 | 09-09-2018 | 46 |
16 | F19 | no data |
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
Alberts, S. C. (2019). Social influences on survival and reproduction: Insights from a long‐term study of wild baboons. Journal of Animal Ecology, 88, 47–66.
Altmann, J. (1983). Costs of reproduction in baboons (Papio cynocephalus). In W. P. Aspey, & S. I. Lustick (Eds.), Behavioral Energetics: The Cost of Survival in Vertebrates (pp. 67–88). Ohio State University Press.
Altmann, J. (2001). Baboon mothers and infants. University of Chicago Press.
Altmann, J., Altmann, S. A., Hausfater, G., & McCuskey, S. A. (1977). Life history of yellow baboons: Physical development, reproductive parameters, and infant mortality. Primates, 18, 315–330.
Altmann, J., & Samuels, A. (1992). Costs of maternal care: Infant‐carrying in baboons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 29, 391–398.
Archie, E. A., Altmann, J., & Alberts, S. C. (2014). Costs of reproduction in a long‐lived female primate: Injury risk and wound healing. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 68, 1183–1193.
Barrett, L., Halliday, J., & Henzi, S. P. (2006). The ecology of motherhood: The structuring of lactation costs by chacma baboons. Journal of Animal Ecology, 75, 875–886.
Bateman, P. W., & Fleming, P. A. (2012). Big city life: Carnivores in urban environments. Journal of Zoology, 287, 1–23.
Beamish, E., & O'Riain, M. J. (in press). Understanding and mitigating conflict between baboons and people in a large city. In J. Wallis (Ed.), Baboons: Behaviour, ecology and taxonomy. Cambridge University Press.
Beamish, E. K. (2009). Causes and consequences of mortality and mutilation in the Cape Peninsula baboon population, South Africa. Masters thesis, University of Cape Town.
Beamish, E. K., & O'Riain, M. J. (2014). The effects of permanent injury on the behavior and diet of commensal chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) in the Cape Peninsula, South Africa. International Journal of Primatology, 35, 1004–1020.
Bracis, C., Bildstein, K. L., & Mueller, T. (2018). Revisitation analysis uncovers spatio‐temporal patterns in animal movement data. Ecography, 41, 1801–1811.
Bracken, A. M., Christensen, C., O'Riain, M. J., Fehlmann, G., Holton, M. D., Hopkins, P. W., Fürtbauer, I., & King, A. J. (2021). Socioecology explains individual variation in urban space use in response to management in Cape chacma baboons (Papio ursinus). International Journal of Primatology, 48, 1–18.
Bracken, A. M., Christensen, C., O'Riain, M. J., Fürtbauer, I., & King, A. J. (2022). Flexible group cohesion and coordination, but robust leader–follower roles, in a wild social primate using urban space. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 289, 20212141.
Calenge, C. (2006). The package “adehabitat” for the R software: A tool for the analysis of space and habitat use by animals. Ecological Modelling, 197, 516–519.
Castles, M., Heinsohn, R., Marshall, H. H., Lee, A. E. G., Cowlishaw, G., & Carter, A. J. (2014). Social networks created with different techniques are not comparable. Animal Behaviour, 96, 59–67.
Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Fischer, J., Beehner, J. C., Bergman, T. J., Johnson, S. E., Kitchen, D. M., Palombit, R. A., Rendall, D., & Silk, J. B. (2006). Reproduction, mortality, and female reproductive success in chacma baboons of the Okavango Delta, Botswana. In L. Swedell & S. R. Leigh (Eds.), Reproduction and fitness in baboons: Behavioral, ecological, and life history perspectives. Developments in primatology: Progress and prospects (pp. 147–176). Springer.
Cheney, D. L., Silk, J. B., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2016). Network connections, dyadic bonds and fitness in wild female baboons. Royal Society Open Science, 3, [eLocator: 160255].
Chiyo, P. I., Moss, C. J., & Alberts, S. C. (2012). The influence of life history milestones and association networks on crop‐raiding behavior in male African elephants. PLoS One, 7, [eLocator: e31382].
Cowlishaw, G. (1999). Ecological and social determinants of spacing behaviour in desert baboon groups. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 45, 67–77.
Dezeure, J., Dagorrette, J., Baniel, A., Carter, A. J., Cowlishaw, G., Marshall, H. H., Martina, C., Raby, C. L., & Huchard, E. (2021). Developmental transitions in body color in chacma baboon infants: Implications to estimate age and developmental pace. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 174, 89–102.
Domb, L. G., & Pagel, M. (2001). Sexual swellings advertise female quality in wild baboons. Nature, 410, 204–206.
Dunayer, E. S., & Berman, C. M. (2018). Infant handling among primates. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 31, 1‐31.
Fehlmann, G., O'Riain, M. J., Fürtbauer, I., & King, A. J. (2021). Behavioral causes, ecological consequences, and management challenges associated with wildlife foraging in human‐modified landscapes. Bioscience, 71, 40–54.
Fehlmann, G., O'Riain, M. J., Kerr‐Smith, C., Hailes, S., Luckman, A., Shepard, E. L. C., & King, A. J. (2017). Extreme behavioural shifts by baboons exploiting risky, resource‐rich, human‐modified environments. Scientific Reports, 7, 15057.
Fehlmann, G., O'Riain, M. J., Kerr‐Smith, C., & King, A. J. (2017). Adaptive space use by baboons (Papio ursinus) in response to management interventions in a human‐changed landscape. Animal Conservation, 20, 101–109.
Henzi, S. P., & Barrett, L. (2002). Infants as a commodity in a baboon market. Animal Behaviour, 63, 915–921.
Hoffman, T. S., & O'Riain, M. J. (2012). Landscape requirements of a primate population in a human‐dominated environment. Frontiers in Zoology, 9, 1.
Kaburu, S. S., Marty, P. R., Beisner, B., Balasubramaniam, K., Bliss‐Moreau, E., Kaur, K., Mohan, L., & McCowan, B. (2019). Rates of human–macaque interactions affect grooming behavior among urban‐dwelling rhesus macaques. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 168, 92–103.
Kaplan, B. S., O'Riain, M. J., van Eeden, R., & King, A. J. (2011). A low‐cost manipulation of food resources reduces spatial overlap between baboons (Papio ursinus) and humans in conflict. International Journal of Primatology, 32, 1397–1412.
McCann, R., Bracken, A. M., Christensen, C., Fürtbauer, I., & King, A. J. (2021). The relationship between GPS sampling interval and estimated daily travel distances in chacma baboons (Papio ursinus). International Journal of Primatology, 42, 589–599.
McLennan, M. R., Spagnoletti, N., & Hockings, K. J. (2017). The implications of primate behavioral flexibility for sustainable human–primate coexistence in anthropogenic habitats. International Journal of Primatology, 38, 105–121.
Merkle, J. A., Robinson, H. S., Krausman, P. R., & Alaback, P. (2013). Food availability and foraging near human developments by black bears. Journal of Mammalogy, 94, 378–385.
Mormile, J. E., & Hill, C. M. (2017). Living with urban baboons: Exploring attitudes and their implications for local baboon conservation and management in Knysna, South Africa. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, 22, 99–109.
Morrow, K. S., Glanz, H., Ngakan, P. O., & Riley, E. P. (2019). Interactions with humans are jointly influenced by life history stage and social network factors and reduce group cohesion in moor macaques (Macaca maura). Scientific Reports, 9, 1–12.
Moss, W. E., Alldredge, M. W., & Pauli, J. N. (2016). Quantifying risk and resource use for a large carnivore in an expanding urban–wildland interface. Journal of Applied Ecology, 53, 371–378.
O'Riain, J. (2021). ‘Cape Town's baboon programme: Successful coexistence between wildlife and urban communities through trial and error’. Daily Maverick, 19th July. Available at: https://www.dailymaverick.co.za/article/2021‐07‐19‐cape‐towns‐baboon‐programme‐successful‐coexistence‐between‐wildlife‐and‐urban‐communities‐through‐trial‐and‐error/ (Last accessed: 27‐03‐2023).
Palombit, R. A. (2003). Male infanticide in wild savanna baboons: Adaptive significance and intraspecific variation. In C. B. Jones (Ed.), Sexual selection and reproductive competition in primates: New perspectives and directions (pp. 367–412). American Society of Primatologists.
Palumbi, S. R. (2001). Humans as the world's greatest evolutionary force. Science, 293, 1786–1790.
Peignier, M., Webber, Q. M. R., Koen, E. L., Laforge, M. P., Robitaille, A. L., & Vander Wal, E. (2019). Space use and social association in a gregarious ungulate: Testing the conspecific attraction and resource dispersion hypotheses. Ecology and Evolution, 9, 5133–5145.
Plowman, A. B., Jordan, N. R., Anderson, N., Condon, E., & Fraser, O. (2005). Welfare implications of captive primate population management: Behavioural and psycho‐social effects of female‐based contraception, oestrus and male removal in hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 90, 155–165.
Purvis, A. (2001). Mammalian life histories and responses of populations to exploitation. In J. Reynolds, G. Mace, K. Redford, & J. Robinson (Eds.), Conservation of exploited species (pp. 169–181). Cambridge University Press.
Purvis, A., Gittleman, J. L., Cowlishaw, G., & Mace, G. M. (2000). Predicting extinction risk in declining species. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 267, 1947–1952.
QGIS.org. (2020). QGIS geographic information system. Open Source Geospatial Foundation project. http://qgis.osgeo.org
Quetglas, A., Rueda, L., Alvarez‐Berastegui, D., Guijarro, B., & Massutí, E. (2016). Contrasting responses to harvesting and environmental drivers of fast and slow life history species. PLoS One, 11, [eLocator: e0148770].
Richardson, H. (2020). ‘Urban wildlife: Managing Cape Town's baboons’, Mongabay, 28th January. Available at: https://news.mongabay.com/2020/01/urban‐wildlife‐managing‐cape‐towns‐baboons/ (Last accessed: 27‐03‐2023).
Robitaille, A. L., Webber, Q. M. R., & Vander Wal, E. (2019). Conducting social network analysis with animal telemetry data: Applications and methods using spatsoc. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 10, 1203–1211.
Seyfarth, R. M. (1976). Social relationships among adult female baboons. Animal Behaviour, 24, 917–938.
Seyfarth, R. M. (1978). Social relationships among adult male and female baboons. II. Behaviour throughout the female reproductive cycle. Behaviour, 64, 227–247.
Sih, A., Ferrari, M. C. O., & Harris, D. J. (2011). Evolution and behavioural responses to human‐induced rapid environmental change. Evolutionary Applications, 4, 367–387.
Silk, J. B., Alberts, S. C., & Altmann, J. (2003). Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science, 302, 1231–1234.
Silk, J. B., Beehner, J. C., Bergman, T. J., Crockford, C., Engh, A. L., Moscovice, L. R., Wittig, R. M., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2010). Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Current Biology, 20, 1359–1361.
Soulsbury, C. D., & White, P. C. L. (2016). Human‐wildlife interactions in urban areas: A review of conflicts, benefits and opportunities. Wildlife Research, 42, 541–553.
TimesLive. (2020). ‘Baby boom sends baboon numbers soaring on Cape peninsula’, 2nd September. Available at: https://www.timeslive.co.za/news/south‐africa/2020‐09‐02‐baby‐boom‐sends‐baboon‐numbers‐soaring‐on‐cape‐peninsula/ (Last accessed: 27‐03‐2023).
van Doorn, A. C., & O'Riain, M. J. (2020). Nonlethal management of baboons on the urban edge of a large metropole. American Journal of Primatology, 82, [eLocator: e23164].
van Doorn, A. C., O'Riain, M. J., & Swedell, L. (2010). The effects of extreme seasonality of climate and day length on the activity budget and diet of semi‐commensal chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) in the Cape Peninsula of South Africa. American Journal of Primatology, 72, 104–112.
Warren, Y., Buba, B., & Ross, C. (2007). Patterns of crop‐raiding by wild and domestic animals near Gashaka Gumti National Park, Nigeria. International Journal of Pest Management, 53, 207–216.
Wheeler, C. R., Gervais, C. R., Johnson, M. S., Vance, S., Rosa, R., Mandelman, J. W., & Rummer, J. L. (2020). Anthropogenic stressors influence reproduction and development in elasmobranch fishes. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries, 30, 373–386.
Zipple, M. N., Grady, J. H., Gordon, J. B., Chow, L. D., Archie, E. A., Altmann, J., & Alberts, S. C. (2017). Conditional fetal and infant killing by male baboons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 284, 20162561.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2023. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the "License"). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Species with slow life history strategies that invest in few offspring with extended parental care need to adapt their behavior to cope with anthropogenic changes that occur within their lifetime. Here we show that a female chacma baboon (
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details





1 School of Biodiversity, One Health and Veterinary Medicine, Glasgow, UK, Biosciences, School of Biosciences, Geography and Physics, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, Swansea, UK
2 Biosciences, School of Biosciences, Geography and Physics, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, Swansea, UK, Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland
3 Department of Biological Sciences, Institute for Communities and Wildlife in Africa, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
4 Biosciences, School of Biosciences, Geography and Physics, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Swansea University, Swansea, UK