Introduction
Females and males share the common goal of siring offspring. This central tenet of sexual reproduction enforces a certain degree of cooperation between the sexes. However, anisogamy frequently leads to distinct sex roles and thus general asymmetries in the reproductive evolutionary interests of females and males, which can in turn result in diverging intensity and form of sexual selection across the sexes (Arnqvist and Rowe, 2005; Chapman et al., 2003a; Janicke et al., 2016; Winkler et al., 2021). This phenomenon, termed sexual conflict, favors traits in one sex that might be costly for the other (Parker, 1979), and can thus lead to antagonistic female-male coevolution (Arnqvist and Rowe, 2005). Sexually antagonistic co-evolution has received much attention and is recognized as a fundamental process in evolution due to its role in shaping male and female adaptations (Bonduriansky et al., 2008), in contributing to drive reproductive isolation and speciation (Arnqvist et al., 2000; Bonduriansky, 2011; Bonduriansky and Chenoweth, 2009; Gavrilets, 2014), and as a major determinant of population demography (Kokko and Brooks, 2003; Bonduriansky and Chenoweth, 2009; Berger et al., 2016). Specifically, sexual conflict has been shown to have profound consequences for female fitness and population growth when it favors male reproductive traits that increase male intra-sexual competitive ability at the expense of harming females (i.e. male harm, Crudgington and Siva-Jothy, 2000; Gómez-Llano et al., 2023; Wigby and Chapman, 2004).
Harmful male adaptations are widespread and incredibly diverse and sophisticated across the tree of life (Arnqvist and Rowe, 2005). For example, male harassment of females during pre-copulatory competition for mating has been documented in myriad vertebrate and invertebrate species (Gómez-Llano et al., 2023), driving antagonistic female-male co-evolution in a host of behavioral and morphological traits (Arnqvist and Rowe, 2005). Male harm adaptations in the context of post-copulatory competition are similarly widespread in invertebrates, featuring (amongst others) toxic ejaculates (Wigby and Chapman, 2005), love darts (Koene and Schulenburg, 2005), and a range of male adaptations for traumatic insemination that range from genital ablation to spiny penises (Crudgington and Siva-Jothy, 2000; Lange et al., 2013). Importantly, beyond driving female and male phenotypes and associated diversification processes, male harm generally leads to a ‘reproductive tragedy of the commons’ that can substantially impact population demography by depressing net female productivity (Arnqvist and Tuda, 2010; Berger et al., 2016; Holland and Rice, 1999; Rankin et al., 2011), and even facilitate population extinction (Le Galliard et al., 2005). Understanding what factors underlie male harm evolution, its diversity in form, strength, and outcomes, is thus a main concern in evolutionary biology.
Despite a growing number of studies in the field of sexual conflict, most have been conducted under uniform laboratory conditions, frequently in populations adapted to stable environments for hundreds of generations (Chapman et al., 2003b; Hopkins et al., 2020; Wigby and Chapman, 2004). In contrast, recent research has highlighted the role of ecology in shaping the evolution of traits under sexual conflict (Arbuthnott et al., 2014; García‐Roa et al., 2019; MacPherson et al., 2018; Perry and Rowe, 2018; Yun et al., 2017), including habitat complexity (Malek and Long, 2019; Miller and Svensson, 2014; Myhre et al., 2013), nutritional status (Fricke et al., 2010), or sex ratio and population density (Chapman et al., 2003a). For example, Gomez-Llano et al., 2018 recently showed that conspecific densities and the presence of heterospecifics modify the intensity and outcome of sexual conflict in the banded demoiselle (
Temperature is recognized as a crucial abiotic ecological factor due to its impact on life history traits and physiological and behavioral responses (De Lisle et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2020; Miler et al., 2020; Monteiro et al., 2017). Furthermore, temperature varies in nature widely within and across spatiotemporal scales (e.g. daily, inter-seasonal, and intra-seasonal variation). Consequently, it may have short, medium, and long-term effects on organism phenotypes that can impact many different aspects of its reproductive behavior (e.g. sex-specific potential reproductive rates, operational sex ratios, density, etc.; García-Roa et al., 2020). In fact, a recent meta-analysis suggests that temperature may have a sizeable effect on sexual selection processes even when fluctuations occur well within the normal range of temperature variation for the studied species (García-Roa et al., 2020). This latter finding is particularly relevant given that we know almost nothing about how average temperature fluctuations, such as those experienced by wild populations during their reproductive season, affect male harm and sexual selection at large.
Our aim was to contribute to fill this gap in knowledge by studying how male harm responds to temperature variation that mimics average fluctuations that are normal during the reproductive season, using
Briefly, we collected flies from a continental wild population in Requena (Spain) that experiences significant fluctuations in temperature even during the mildest months when it is reproductively active (e.g. July: average: 24.9°C, average min: 19.8°C, average max: 30.1°C, Fick and Hijmans, 2017). After acclimation of the resulting population to laboratory conditions under a fluctuating temperature regime mimicking natural conditions, we conducted five different experiments to gauge how temperature variation within a normal range (i.e. 20°C, 24°C, and 28°C) affects: (a) the overall impact of male-male competition on female lifetime reproductive success (i.e. male harm), (b) how the net effects of harm are accomplished in terms of different female fitness components (i.e. reproductive rate, actuarial aging, and reproductive aging), and (c) underlying male pre-copulatory (i.e. harassment) and post-copulatory (i.e. ejaculate toxicity) harm mechanisms (Figure 1).
Figure 1.
Schematic overview of the study.
(A) Our aim was to study how temperature variation, across a range at which reproduction is optimum in the wild, may affect: the net decrease in female fitness resulting from male harm, what female fitness components are mainly affected by male harm, and pre-copulatory (i.e. sexual harassment) and post-copulatory (i.e. ejaculate effects on female receptivity, short-term fecundity, and survival) mechanism of harm. (B) General design of the study: (1) We sampled a wild population of
Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
Fitness and behavioural assay design (Experiment 1).
Figure 1—figure supplement 2.
Receptivity assay design (Short treatment duration – 48 hr, experiment 2).
Figure 1—figure supplement 3.
Receptivity assay design (Long treatment duration – 13 days, experiment 3).
Figure 1—figure supplement 4.
Fecundity and survival assay design (Short treatment duration – 48 hr, experiment 4).
Figure 1—figure supplement 5.
Fecundity and survival assay design (Long treatment duration – 13 days, experiment 5).
Materials and methods
Field collection
In October 2018, we used banana traps to sample
Stock maintenance and acclimation
We carried out all experiments between March 2020 and April 2021, using individuals from the VG field population kept in the laboratory with overlapping generations at an average temperature of 24°C with daily pre-programmed fluctuations (±4°C) mimicking natural daily temperature conditions during the reproductively active season, at ~60% humidity and on a 12:12 hr light:dark cycle (Pol Eko ST 1200 incubator). The lowest temperature was set up 1 hr after sunrise and the highest 1 hr after midday. It is important to note that our stock population of flies was kept under a programmed fluctuating temperature regime that mimics their average circadian rhythm in the field, but temperature fluctuations in nature will be inherently subject to minor stochastic variations whose effects we controlled for (and thus did not capture) in this experiment. We used maize-malt medium (7 g of agar, 72 g of malt, 72 g of maize, 9 g of soya, 15 g of live yeast, 20 g of molasses, and 33 ml of Nipagin mix –3 g of methyl 4-hydroxy-benzoate and 28 ml of ethanol 90%– per 1000 ml of water) as a food source throughout maintenance and experiments. To collect experimental flies, we introduced yeasted grape juice agar plates into stock populations to induce female oviposition. We then collected eggs and placed them in bottles containing ~75 ml of medium to be incubated at 24 ± 4°C at a mean density of 223 ± 14.3 (95% CI) (Clancy and Kennington, 2001). We collected virgin flies within 6 hr of eclosion, under ice anesthesia, and then sexed and kept them in vials with food until their use (~3 days later), at 24 ± 4°C (see below for more details).
Net impact of male harm on female fitness and underlying behavioural mechanisms (experiment 1)
Fitness assay
To study whether male harm is affected by temperature, we established a factorial design to measure survival and lifetime reproduction success (LRS) of female flies under monogamy (i.e. one male and one female per vial) vs. polyandry (i.e. three males and one female per vial), across three stable temperature treatments typical of this population during their reproductively active period in the wild: 20°C, 24°C, and 28 °C. Comparison of female fitness at monogamy vs. polyandry is standard procedure to gauge male harm in
We first collected virgin flies into same-sex vials of 15 individuals and then randomly allocated them to either of the three temperature treatments 48 hr before starting the experiment, at which temperatures they remained until its end. To estimate LRS, we transferred flies to fresh vials twice a week using mild CO2 anesthesia. We incubated the vials containing female eggs at 24 ± 4°C for 15–20 days (~15 days for vials coming from 28°C, ~17 days for 24°C and ~20 days for 20°C) to allow F1 offspring emergence, after which we froze them at –21°C for later counting. The differences in incubation time are due to differences in developmental time caused by temperature differences during the first 3–4 days of each vial (i.e. the time eggs remained at their respective temperature treatments before flipping females to new fresh vials and incubation at 24 ± 4°C). We discarded and replaced males with young (2–4 days old) virgin males (receiving the same treatment as described above for original males) three weeks after starting the experiment (at the same time for all treatments). In addition, we kept a stock of replacement males maintained at each of the three temperatures to replace dead male flies if needed. We kept focal female flies under these conditions for six weeks, after which we discarded males and followed females until they died for survival analysis (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1 for an overview of the experimental design).
We started the experiment with 468 females (78 per temperature and mating treatment) and 936 males (234 per temperature in polyandry and 78 per temperature in monogamy). Due to discarded (e.g. accidentally damaged during handling) and escaped flies, final (female) sample sizes were: (a) at 20°C, npolyandry = 74 and nmonogamy = 76, (b) at 24°C: npolyandry = 72 and nmonogamy = 77, and (c) at 28°C: npolyandry = 70 and nmonogamy = 75. We estimated the overall degree of male harm by calculating relative harm (H) following Yun et al., 2021:
where
Using the data collected above, we partitioned overall LRS effects into effects on early reproductive rate (i.e. offspring produced during the first two weeks of age), actuarial aging (i.e. lifespan), and reproductive aging (i.e. offspring produced over weeks 1–2 vs. 3–4). We used weeks 3–4 as an estimate of late reproductive rate because mortality was already evident at this point (i.e. reflecting aging) and then was very high from week 5 onwards (Figure 4—figure supplement 1; thus preventing accurate estimation of reproductive success).
Finally, we also calculated rate-sensitive fitness estimates for each individual female and treatment population. Rate-sensitive fitness estimates take into account when offspring are produced, not just how many offspring are produced, and thus allow estimating fitness subject to the population growth rate (Edward et al., 2011). It is important to understand how differences in the number and timing of offspring production translate into fitness under different demographic scenarios. For example, early reproduction is particularly favored in increasing populations whereas late reproduction gains in importance in decreasing populations. Thus, while LRS is most suited to estimate individual fitness in stable populations, rate-sensitive estimates are preferred when r ≠ 0 (Brommer et al., 2002). We calculated both individual (ωind) and population (ωpop) rate-sensitive fitness for the following intrinsic rates of population growth:
Behavioral measures
Immediately after the fitness experiment started, we conducted behavioral observations on the first day of the experiment across all treatments (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Our aim was to investigate the behavioral mechanisms that might underlie the potential fitness effects evaluated above. Due to logistic limitations, we conducted behavioral observations in the same temperature control room, so we had to conduct trials at 20°C, 24°C, and 28°C over three consecutive days (with both monogamy and polyandry treatments evaluated at the same time for each temperature), in a randomized order (i.e. 20°C, 28°C, and 24°C). Note that we collected virgin flies over three consecutive days to ensure all flies were 5 days-old at the start of the experiment. We recorded the following behaviors: (a) courtship intensity (number of courting males per female per hour), (b) male-male aggression rate (i.e. number of aggressions per hour), and (c) female rejection (i.e. number of rejections per hour; see Bastock and Manning, 1955; Connolly and Cook, 1973 for behavioral descriptions). We also recorded the number of total matings during the observation period.
Observations started at lights-on (10 a.m.) and lasted for 8 hr, during which time we continuously recorded reproductive behaviors using scan sampling of vials. Each complete scan lasted approximately 8 min, so that we always conducted one complete scan every 10 min to ensure the recording of all matings (see below). Scans consisted in observing all vials in succession for ca. 3 s each and recording all occurrences of the behaviors listed above (i.e. all-occurrences recording of target behaviors combined with scan sampling). We interspersed behavioral scans with very quick (<1 min) mating scans where we rapidly swept all vials for copulas at the beginning, in the middle, and at the end of each complete scan. This strategy ensured that we recorded all successful matings (>10 min), which typically last between 15 and 25 min in our population of
Mating effect on female reproduction and survival (experiments 2 to 5)
To examine post-mating mechanisms that might underlie the fitness effects observed in our first experiment, we conducted four additional experiments to test whether temperature modulates the well-documented effects that mating with a male has on female receptivity, short-term fecundity, and survival. In
Then, we measured how the reception of a treated male’s ejaculate after a single mating in a common garden environment (i.e. all matings at 24 °C) affected female fecundity, survival, and reproduction, following standard assays to gauge male ejaculate effects on females in
Receptivity assays (experiments 2 and 3)
We first collected focal males as virgins (i.e. within 6 hr of eclosion) under ice anesthesia and randomly placed them either individually (low SCR) or in a same-sex group of eight (high SCR) in plastic vials with food. Next, we randomly divided them into three groups that we allocated to the different stable temperature treatments for either 48 hr (i.e. short treatment duration, experiment 2, Figure 1—figure supplement 2) or 13 days (i.e. long treatment duration, experiment 3, Figure 1—figure supplement 3) immediately before the beginning of each experiment. For experiment 3, we depleted the sperm and seminal fluid of focal males before allocating them to different temperature treatments by housing them with four standard virgin females for 24 hr, given that three successive matings are enough to deplete the accessory glands of male
We collected all females and competitor males (i.e. standard males without any previous treatment) used in receptivity assays as virgins and held them in same-sex groups of 15–20 flies at 24 ± 4°C. Experiments started by exposing all virgin females to single focal males for 2.5 hr at 24°C. After a successful copulation, we separated the mated females from the focal males and isolated them until the remating trial. We discarded unmated females and focal males. 72 hr after this first mating with the focal treated male, we individually exposed females to single virgin competitor males for 12 hr. After each trial, we transferred unmated females into a new vial with food, until the next remating trial on the next day (Figure 1—figure supplements 2 and 3). We repeated remating trials for three consecutive days, which allowed us to calculate the cumulative percentage of remated females (and associated re-mating latencies; see below) for each of the three days of each experiment. Due to a large number of vials/flies involved, we conducted the experiments in two blocks each: with n=390 females per batch in experiment 2 (n=436 rematings) and n=420 females per batch in experiment 3 (n=676 rematings). We also recorded mating duration for the first mating (i.e. with the focal treated male), the remating latency (i.e. the time lapse between males being introduced into the female-containing vial and copulation), and mating duration for re-matings. Females and focal and competitor males were 4 days old for experiment 2. In experiment 3, females and competitor males were 4 days old, while focal males were 18 days old.
Fecundity and survival assays (experiments 4 and 5)
To study the effects of a single mating on female short-term fecundity and long-term survival, we performed two experiments (experiments 4 and 5, Figure 1—figure supplements 4 and 5, respectively) where we compared female fecundity and F1 egg-to-adult viability of females mated with male flies subject to the same factorial design imposed in receptivity experiments (here experiment 4 had a treatment duration of 48 hr while experiment 5 had a treatment duration of 13 days). We collected and treated all focal males as in the receptivity assays described above, and then proceeded to mate virgin females in single pairs with focal males for 2.5 hr at 24°C. After copulation, we separated mated females from focal males and kept them individually in single vials. We discarded unmated females and focal males. We then transferred females to fresh vials every 24 hr for 4 days, and then every 3 days twice. Finally, we followed females until they died by combining them into same-treatment vials of 10 females that were flipped once a week. We removed dead flies at each flip and scored deaths on a daily basis. We counted eggs laid during the first 3 days and incubated vials from days 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 8 until adults emerged to count progeny and determine egg-to-adult viability (Figure 1—figure supplements 4 and 5). Sample sizes were 545 females for experiment 4, and 480 females for experiment 5. Females and focal or competitor males were 4 days old for experiment 4. In experiment 5, females and competitor males were 4 days old, while focal males were 18 days old.
Statistical analyses
We performed all statistical analyses using R statistical software (version 3.5.2). In all cases, we assessed fit and validated models by visual inspections of diagnostic plots on raw and residual data (Zuur et al., 2010). In all models, we used ANOVA type III test ‘F’ to compute
Experiment 1
To examine temperature effects on male harm, we evaluated the interaction between mating system and temperature on female fitness (LRS), early reproductive rate, reproductive aging, actuarial aging, and male and female reproductive behaviors (courtship intensity and female rejection; experiment 1). We fitted generalized linear models (GLMs) with temperature, mating system, and their interaction as fixed effects. Graphical inspection of LRS, actuarial aging, and reproductive behaviors (courtship intensity and male-male aggression) revealed that the normality assumption was apparently violated, as well as the independence assumption for LRS. Box–Cox transformation (Quinn and Keough, 2002) solved these problems and allowed us to run a GLM with a Gaussian error distribution. We compared GLMs with their corresponding null GLMs using the likelihood ratio test only to test the significance of the independent variables in the full model. We detected collinearity between the mating system and the interaction in LRS, early reproductive, reproductive aging, actuarial aging, courtship intensity, and female rejection models. In all these cases, we thus refitted the model without the main mating system effect (which was not our main interest). As a complementary analysis for LRS, we also ran a model with temperature as a factor and a predetermined quadratic contrast table (given the relationship between LRS and temperature is clearly non-linear, Figure 2), and obtained similar results.
Figure 2.
Female lifetime reproductive success (mean ± SEM) across temperature and mating system treatments.
20°C: npolyandry = 73 and nmonogamy = 74. 24°C: npolyandry = 71 and nmonogamy = 74. 28°C: npolyandry = 66 and nmonogamy = 71.
Figure 2—figure supplement 1.
Violin plot for female reproductive success across temperature and mating system treatments.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2.
Early reproductive rate (number of offspring produced during the first two weeks of age), late reproductive rate (number of offspring produced during the second two weeks of age), and reproductive aging (number of offspring produced over weeks 1–2 vs. 3-4) plots.
(a) Mean ± SEM number of offspring in monogamy vs. polyandry mating system treatments, across the different temperature treatments. 20°C: npolyandry = 73 and nmonogamy = 74. 24°C: npolyandry = 71 and nmonogamy = 74. 28°C: npolyandry = 66 and nmonogamy = 71. (b) Violin plots.
We also used Cox proportional hazards survival model to analyze potential differences in mortality risk across treatments, using the
Experiments 2–5
To examine the effect of temperature on post-copulatory effects, we evaluated the effect of SCR, temperature, treatment duration, and their interaction on receptivity (mating duration and remating latency -experiments 2 and 3) and fecundity -oviposition and egg viability- and female survival (experiments 4 and 5). For mating duration, remating latency and egg viability we fitted generalized linear models (GLMs) with temperature, SCR level, treatment duration, and their interaction as fixed effects. We assessed the significance of factors by dropping individual terms from the full model using the ‘drop1’ function, refitting models without the triple interaction where necessary. We detected a problem of collinearity between SCR and the interactions, as well as between treatment duration and the interactions in mating duration, remating latency, and egg viability models. In all these cases, we refitted the model without the main SCR and treatment duration effects (which were not our main interest). For the mating duration, we used a Gamma distribution. For remating latency and egg fertility, we used a Gaussian distribution. For oviposition, we fitted a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with temperature, SCR level, treatment duration and their interaction as fixed effects and day as a random effect. Initially, we run a model with a zero-inflated distribution, in which the zero values are modeled separately from the non-zero values (Zuur et al., 2010). However, we detected problems of collinearity, including treatment duration as an effect. We thus run two separate models for each treatment duration using Hurdle models without the main SCR effect. Finally, for survival, we used a Cox proportional hazards survival model to analyze potential differences in mortality risk across treatments, including the females lost during manipulations as ‘right censored’ individuals.
Results
Net impact of male harm on female fitness and underlying behavioral mechanisms (experiment 1)
LRS
We detected a significant temperature by mating system interaction for female lifetime reproductive success (
Table 1.
Output from separate generalized linear models (GLMs) for each temperature level to explore significant interactions between temperature and mating system effects on female fitness components.
LRS | Reproductive aging | Actuarial aging | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
|
|
|
| ||||
20° | 4.41,145 | 0.039 | 1.07 | 12.11,145 | <0.001 | –7.99 | 39.6 1,148 | <0.001 | 7.44 |
24° | 16.61,142 | <0.001 | 22.39 | 35.31.142 | <0.001 | –17.2 | 32.2 1,143 | <0.001 | 4.84 |
28° | 2.21,135 | 0.137 | 1.88 | 14.11,135 | <0.001 | –11.87 | 19.7 1,137 | <0.001 | 2.97 |
Figure 3.
Rate-sensitive fitness estimates.
(a) Average rate-sensitive index fitness estimate of individual females (Mean ωind) for different population growth rates across temperature and mating system treatments (shaded areas denote SEM). 20°C: npolyandry = 73 and nmonogamy = 74. 24°C: npolyandry = 71 and nmonogamy = 74. 28°C: npolyandry = 66 and nmonogamy = 71. (b) Relative cost (
Early reproductive rate
We did not detect a significant temperature by mating system interaction (
Reproductive aging
We detected a significant temperature by mating system interaction for reproductive aging (
Actuarial aging
We detected a significant temperature by mating system effect for lifespan (
Figure 4.
Male harm effect on female lifespan (mean ± SEM) across temperature and mating system treatments.
20°C: npolyandry = 73 and nmonogamy = 74. 24°C: npolyandry = 71 and nmonogamy = 73. 28°C: npolyandry = 66 and nmonogamy = 73.
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.
Male harm effect on female lifespan across temperature and mating system treatments.
(a) Violin plot. (b) Survival plot from the Cox proportional hazard model as a complementary analysis.
Reproductive behaviour
The interaction between temperature and mating system was significant for courtship rate (
Figure 5.
Reproductive behaviors (mean ± SEM) across temperature and mating system treatments.
(a) Courtships per female per hour, (b) Female rejections per hour, and (c) Aggressions male-male per hour. 20°C: npolyandry = 74 and nmonogamy = 76. 24°C: npolyandry = 72 and nmonogamy = 77. 28°C: npolyandry = 70 and nmonogamy = 75.
Figure 5—figure supplement 1.
Violin plot for male harm effect on: (a) Courtship rate and (b) Rejection rate across temperature and mating system treatments; (c) Violin plot for polyandry mating system effect on aggression rate.
Figure 5—figure supplement 2.
Total number of matings across the 8 hr of observations.
(a) Mean ± SEM across temperature and mating system treatments. (b) Violin plot. Data from reproductive behaviour measures. 20°C: npolyandry = 74 and nmonogamy = 76. 24°C: npolyandry = 72 and nmonogamy = 77. 28°C: npolyandry = 70 and nmonogamy = 75.
Table 2.
Output from separate generalized linear models (GLMs) for each temperature level to explore significant interactions between temperature and mating system effects on underlying behaviorual mechanisms.
p-values were corrected for multiple testing using Benjamini-Hochberg correction.
Courtship rate | Rejection rate | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
|
| |||
20° | 0.41,148 | 0.546 | –0.04 | 0.201,148 | 0.654 | –0.05 |
24° | 21.81,147 | <0.001 | –0.40 | 10.91.147 | 0.001 | –17.2 |
28° | 40.21,143 | <0.001 | –0.63 | 19.31,143 | <0.001 | –11.87 |
Mating effects on female reproduction and survival (experiments 2 to 5)
Female receptivity (experiments 2 and 3)
For the duration of the first mating in our female receptivity assays, we detected significant SCR by temperature (
Figure 6.
Mean ± SEM for mating duration and remating latency.
(a) Mating duration of males exposed to high (8 males per vial) or low sperm competition risk (1 male per vial) for 48 hr or 13 days prior to mating at different temperatures. 20°C: nhigh/48hr = 91, nlow/48hr = 96, nhigh/13days = 121 and nlow/13days = 117. 24°C: nhigh/48hr = 85, nlow/48hr = 88, nhigh/13days = 119 and nlow/13days = 115. 28°C: nhigh/48hr = 92, nlow/48hr = 104, nhigh/13days = 99 and nlow/13days = 117. (b) Female remating latency following a single mating with either a male from a high or low sperm competition risk level for 48 hr or 13 days before mating across temperature treatments. 20°C: nhigh/48hr = 75, nlow/48hr = 73, nhigh/13days = 119 and nlow/13days = 113. 24°C: nhigh/48hr = 61, nlow/48hr = 70, nhigh/13days = 116 and nlow/13days = 113. 28°C: nhigh/48hr = 63, nlow/48hr = 82, nhigh/13days = 98 and nlow/13days = 117.
Figure 6—figure supplement 1.
Violin plots for (a) Mating duration of males exposed to a high (8 males per vial) or low sperm competition risk (1 male per vial) level 48 hr (experiment 2) and 13 days (experiment 3) before mating across temperature treatments, and (b) Female remating latency following a single mating with either a male from a high or low sperm competition risk level, for both 48 hr and 13 days of temperature treatment duration before mating in a common garden.
Figure 6—figure supplement 2.
Eggs produced by females during the first three days following a single mating with treated males.
(a) Mean ± SEM, 48 hr treatment duration. 20°C: nhigh/day 1 = 88, nlow/day 1 = 86, nhigh/day 2 = 88, nlow/day 2 = 86, nhigh/day 3 = 88 and nlow/day 3 = 86. 24°C: nhigh/day 1 = 87, nlow/day 1 = 88, nhigh/day 2 = 87, nlow/day 2 = 88, nhigh/day 3 = 87 and nlow/day 3 = 88. 28°C: nhigh/day 1 = 86, nlow/day 1 = 86, nhigh/day 2 = 86, nlow/day 2 = 86, nhigh/day 3 = 86 and nlow/day 3 = 86. (b) Violin plot, 48 hr treatment duration. (c) Mean ± SEM, 13 days treatment duration. 20°C: nhigh/day 1 = 74, nlow/day 1 = 76, nhigh/day 2 = 74, nlow/day 2 = 76, nhigh/day 3 = 74 and nlow/day 3 = 76. 24°C: nhigh/day 1 = 72, nlow/day 1 = 76, nhigh/day 2 = 72, nlow/day 2 = 76, nhigh/day 3 = 72 and nlow/day 3 = 76. 28°C: nhigh/day 1 = 75, nlow/day 1 = 65, nhigh/day 2 = 75, nlow/day 2 = 63, nhigh/day 3 = 75 and nlow/day 3 = 63. (d) Violin plot, 13 days treatment duration.
Figure 6—figure supplement 3.
Total of offspring produced by females during the days 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 8 after mating following a single mating with treated males.
(a) Mean ± SEM. 20°C: nhigh/48hr = 88, nlow/48hr = 86, nhigh/13days = 74 and nlow/13days = 76. 24°C: nhigh/48hr = 87, nlow/48hr = 88, nhigh/13days = 72 and nlow/13days = 76. 28°C: nhigh/48hr = 86, nlow/48hr = 86, nhigh/13days = 75 and nlow/13days = 63.(b) Violin plot.
Figure 6—figure supplement 4.
Female lifespan after mating following a single mating with treated males.
(a) Mean ± SEM. 20°C: nhigh/48hr = 80, nlow/48hr = 80, nhigh/13days = 71 and nlow/13days = 70. 24°C: nhigh/48hr = 81, nlow/48hr = 82, nhigh/13days = 60 and nlow/13days = 73. 28°C: nhigh/48hr = 80, nlow/48hr = 84, nhigh/13days = 69 and nlow/13days = 45. (b) Violin plot (c) Survival plot from the Cox proportional hazard model.
Table 3.
Model outputs from separate generalized linear models (GLMs) for each (a) temperature level and (b) treatment duration to explore significant interactions.
a) | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
|
| ||||||||
|
|
|
| ||||||||
20° |
| 3.91,423 | 0.046 | 0.03 | 0.951,377 | 0.330 | 27.9 | ||||
| 2.31,423 | 0.133 | –0.02 | 0.00061,377 | 0.980 | –0.73 | |||||
24° |
| 10.61,405 | 0.001 | 0.05 | 0.071,358 | 0.779 | –8.47 | ||||
| 3.71,405 | 0.054 | –0.02 | 0.041,358 | 0.842 | –6.24 | |||||
28° |
| 26.51,410 | <0.001 | 0.084 | 8.051,358 | 0.005 | 87.81 | ||||
| 0.61,410 | 0.451 | –0.12 | 9.731,358 | 0.002 | –97.65 | |||||
b) | |||||||||||
|
| ||||||||||
|
| ||||||||||
Short (48 hr) | 4.51,554 | 0.033 | 0.03 | ||||||||
Long (13 days) | 54.21,686 | <0.001 | 0.07 |
Female fecundity and survival (experiments 4 and 5)
For the number of eggs produced by females during the three first days, we did not detect significant interactions between temperature and SCR for either short or long treatment durations, nor a main significant effect for temperature (Figure 6—figure supplement 2 & Table 4—source data 1): (i) Short treatment duration, SCR by temperature interaction (
Table 4.
Summary statistics from fitting generalized linear models (GLMs) separately for each temperature level to explore the significant interaction between temperature and treatment duration effects for total offspring produced by females during days 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 8 after mating.
| |||
---|---|---|---|
|
| ||
20° | 0.61,322 | 0.454 | 1.42 (−2.30–5.15) |
24° | 4.61,321 | 0.032 | 4.11 (0.35–7.86) |
28° | 5.21,308 | 0.022 | 4.26 (0.62–7.89) |
Finally, we did not detect significant interactions in survival (SCR by treatment duration,
Discussion
We show that male harm exhibits complex plasticity in response to temperature changes within an optimal reproductive range (20–28°C) for a wild
Temperature effects on male harm and its consequences for populations
We found that temperature variation within the optimal reproductive range for our study population in the wild had a strong effect on net male harm levels. To gauge male harm, we used the standard procedure of comparing female LRS in monogamy, which imposes low male-male competition and thus low sexual conflict, vs. polyandry (i.e. a female with three males), which imposes high male-male competition and intensifies sexual conflict between the sexes (Yun et al., 2021). These sex ratios are common in mating patches in the wild, and are actually representative of the extremes in natural levels of male-male competition (Dukas, 2020). In monogamy, temperature did not affect female fitness (Figure 2), showing that female reproduction is indeed optimal within this range. In contrast, the net decrease in female LRS in polyandry vs. monogamy was highly dependent on the thermal environment, with an average decrease of H=0.36 at 24 °C, H=0.22 at 20 °C. and H=0.10 at 28 °C, at which temperature we did not find a statistically significant effect of mating system on female LRS (Figure 2).
Male harm effects are expected to be cumulative over time, so that their impact on female survival and reproductive output is unlikely to be constant across a female’s lifespan (Bonduriansky et al., 2008; Filice et al., 2020). At the same time, early vs. late-life reproduction weigh differently on both individual fitness and how this impacts background population growth, depending on whether such population is decreasing, stable, or growing (Edward et al., 2011; Priest et al., 2008). Thus, in order to evaluate how male harm is likely to impact populations across different temperatures, we calculated rate-sensitive fitness estimates for individual (Figure 3a) and population (Figure 3b) fitness across a range of demographic scenarios (i.e. decreasing, stable, and growing populations; Edward et al., 2011). Overall, the impact of male harm was higher in decreasing populations, where late-life reproduction gains importance, which is consistent with the idea that male harm effects are cumulative over the lifespan of females. Above and beyond this general effect, the observed interaction between temperature and female fitness is maintained irrespective of population demography. That is, male harm decreases female individual fitness more at 24°C than at 20°C and 28°C. Interestingly, though, temperature also has a clear effect on how demography affects population-level costs. At hotter temperatures the relative population costs of male harm vary considerably less with demography (i.e. background population growth) than at colder temperatures (Figure 3b). For a decreasing population, the relative population costs of male harm are significantly higher at 20°C than at 28°C, but this difference wanes as population growth rate (r) increases, to the point of reverting in a rapidly growing population (Figure 3b). This suggests that male harm has more impact on late-life female fitness at cold temperatures, and hints at the possibility that cold vs. hot temperature affect qualitatively distinct parameters of female fitness, and thus underlying mechanisms of male harm.
Looking at the effects on separate female fitness components yields results largely in agreement with the above ideas. Again, consistent with the fact that harm needs to accumulate in time to impact female fitness, temperature had no effect on how or whether male competition impacted early reproductive rate (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). Temperature did, however, modulate how male harm impacted on female actuarial vs. reproductive aging. We found clear differences in female lifespan across temperature and mating system treatments. Male harm effects on actuarial aging (an increase in mortality rate with age) were more severe at 20°C (35% decrease in female lifespan) than at 24°C (31% decrease) and at 28°C (22% decrease; Figure 3). In contrast, while male harm accelerated reproductive aging at all temperatures, this decrease was more marked at 24°C and at 28°C than at 20°C (Table 1; Figure 2—figure supplement 2). In accordance with available evidence in lab-adapted flies (García‐Roa et al., 2019), these results show that temperature does not seem to have a linear effect on aging processes.
To sum up, we offer strong evidence that different male harm mechanisms are sensitive to temperature in different ways, with ensuing modulation of its effects on different female fitness components. We underscore two potential consequences arising from these findings. The first is that the net fitness effects of male harm might be lower than expected when considered in its natural thermal setting because previous research has focused on studying male harm at average temperatures, precisely where we found it to be maximal. It follows that integrating harm across the natural temperature range will result in a lower net decrease in female fitness in the wild. The second is that environmental variability may foster the maintenance of genetic variation underlying different mechanisms of male harm, and even potentially divergent male-male competition strategies. Our work joins an increasing number of recent studies in highlighting the importance of evaluating more ecologically realistic scenarios in sexual conflict research, particularly how natural fluctuations in the ecology of the socio-sexual context may affect sexual conflict processes (García‐Roa et al., 2019, García-Roa et al., 2020; Gomez-Llano et al., 2018; Yun et al., 2017).
In addition, the above results open up the possibility that warm climates may lessen the impact of sexual conflict on population viability, perhaps facilitating evolutionary rescue. Male harm effects were found to be relatively lower in warmer temperatures and in decreasing populations, precisely the type of context that would be typical of a climate-change scenario. The effects found in this study were within the optimum reproductive range for this population, but similar results have been reported in response to stressful temperatures. For instance, temperature has been used to induce environmental stress in natural populations of seed beetles (
Temperature effects on sexual conflict mechanisms in
Prior to mating,
During mating,
All in all, our results suggest that at least some post-copulatory harm mechanisms are sensitive to temperature, because receipt of a male ejaculate resulted in a sharper decrease of female receptivity in high vs. low SCR at warm temperatures (particularly after 13 days of exposure). We speculate that this may contribute to explain why male harm drops so sharply at 28°C despite the fact that male harassment and male-male competition seem to be maximal at this temperature. In
We suggest future studies should explore these ideas by examining in detail how temperature affects the composition and transfer of SFPs to females, and how females respond to the transfer of these proteins and to male harm in general (i.e. effects on female resistance). In combination with experimental evolution at different temperatures, such an approach would allow us to disentangle between two causal hypotheses for the observed results. First, that warm temperatures may buffer sexual conflict in itself by aligning male and female reproductive interests. For example, if live-fast-die-young strategies fare relatively better for females at warm than cold temperatures, male and female optimal reproductive strategies may overlap more due to the fact that cumulative late-life effects of male harm might be diluted by the inherently high female intrinsic mortality at warm temperatures. Second, whether modulation of male harm at cold and (particularly) warm temperatures has to do with the fact that different male harm mechanisms are adapted to operate better at certain temperatures. For example, due to environmental effects on male activity or protein folding. In the latter case, male harm would be expected to increase as males adapt to higher or lower average temperatures, but sexual conflict per se (i.e. the degree to which male and female evolutionary interests overlap) would be expected to remain constant. Both of the above hypotheses could have broad consequences for our understanding of the evolution of sexual conflict across the tree of life.
Conclusions
Our findings may have implications for our understanding of how sexual conflict unfolds in nature, and its consequences for populations. First, they add to growing evidence (Gomez-Llano et al., 2018; MacPherson et al., 2018; Malek and Long, 2019; Perry and Rowe, 2018; Yun et al., 2017) indicating that ecological context is key in shaping sexually antagonistic coevolution and, in particular, suggest that temperature may be a particularly salient ecological factor to understand how sexual conflict evolves and operates in nature (García-Roa et al., 2020). Second, they highlight that male harm mechanisms can be highly plastic even in response to relatively minor fluctuations in temperature well within the optimal reproductive range, and suggest that different harm mechanisms are differently affected by temperature. Third, they suggest that male harm effects on female life-history and fitness components are asymmetrically modulated by temperature; male harm particularly decreased survival at cold and moderate temperatures, and reproductive aging at moderate and hot temperatures. In conjunction, these phenomena may have a bearing on evolutionary rescue and local adaptation processes. For example, in maintaining genetic variation in sexually selected traits in males, and/or in ameliorating the demographic impact of sexual conflict in populations facing environmental change.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2023, Londoño-Nieto et al. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Strong sexual selection frequently leads to sexual conflict and ensuing male harm, whereby males increase their reproductive success at the expense of harming females. Male harm is a widespread evolutionary phenomenon with a strong bearing on population viability. Thus, understanding how it unfolds in the wild is a current priority. Here, we sampled a wild
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer