1. Introduction
Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) [1] is a serious public health issue [2]. Indeed, its lifetime prevalence is high among the French population (15–20%) [3,4], and one in every three patients suffering from MDD has to stop working at some point [5]. Indeed, MDD causes functional impairment that seems to be mediated by cognitive dysfunction [6]. Consistently, meta-analyses have showed the presence of cognitive disturbances among depressed patients, affecting multiple cognitive processes such as verbal memory, processing speed, and executive function [7,8,9]. Moreover, some results indicate a positive association between depressive symptoms and cognitive deficits [10]. Other works also suggest that cognitive impairments are associated with a poorer response to antidepressant treatment or with more residual symptoms [11,12,13,14]. Finally, such impairments seem to be present during the first episode [15] and remain after remission [8,16,17,18]. As we said earlier, this result is important as cognitive impairments are associated with functional impairment such as social and occupational alterations [19]. In line with this, some studies have highlighted that cognitive impairments are the mediator between functional alterations and MDD [20,21]. More specifically, one of them showed that the relationship between MDD and functional impairment was fully mediated by executive dysfunction [22].
Even if the presence of cognitive impairments seems clear in MDD, there is a lack of studies performed among patients suffering from treatment-resistant depression (TRD), defined as a non-response to at least two different classes of antidepressant treatments at appropriate dosage and time treatment [23]. To the best of our knowledge, only three studies have been performed. The first one compared 53 patients suffering from a first episode of MDD and 53 patients suffering from TRD. The results found poorer performance in executive tasks (TMT-B, Wisconsin Card Sorting Task, and Towers of London) by patients suffering from TRD as compared to those experiencing a first episode [24]. A second study set out that cognitive deficit is among the significant predictive factors of treatment resistance in MDD [12]. This study compared 125 non-TRD subjects to 104 patients suffering from TRD. They found that TRD was associated with worse performances concerning verbal memory, processing speed, and executive function. Finally, a large cohort study found that TRD patients showed serious cognitive impairments that are associated with poorer daily functioning [25]. Taken together, all these findings suggest that cognitive impairments should be a target and that cognitive remediation trials could be successfully proposed for managing such patients and therefore improving daily functioning, e.g., [26]. However, it is still unclear when such interventions should be delivered. Indeed, if patients suffering from MDD partly recover their cognitive abilities, it would be irrelevant to propose cognitive remediation during the episode. Such interventions would be more appropriate after therapeutic response or a remitted state. Moreover, no study has explored if cognitive impairments could be predictive for a therapeutic nonresponse within patients suffering from TRD.
Therefore, the main objective of this study was to describe the course of cognitive impairments in responder versus non-responder TRD patients at one-year follow-up. The second aim was to evaluate the predictive aspect of cognitive impairments to treatment resistance in patients suffering from TRD.
2. Method
2.1. Study Design
Participants were recruited from 13 Centers of Expertise for Treatment-Resistant Depression (FACE-DR) that all followed the same standardized clinical assessments [27]. To be considered a FACE-DR center, each investigator site had to employ at least a secretary involved in monitoring patients’ appointments and one psychiatrist along with one neuropsychologist trained in the standardized, comprehensive, and multidimensional assessment of FACE-DR that was performed every year for four consecutive years. Psychiatrists undertook at least yearly clinical evaluations of patients’ videos to ensure concordance among raters. Due to the important rate of dropout, we proposed to consider the data primarily related to depression severity, neuropsychological impairment, and daily functional disability and collected during the first two visits at the one-year interval. We used the following outcomes of the battery: neuropsychological functioning, daily functioning, and depression.
2.2. Settings
The Centers of Expertise for Treatment-Resistant Depression (FACE-DR) were widely distributed over 13 French cities, including Paris/Créteil, Paris Fernand-Widal, Besançon, Bordeaux, Brest, Clermont-Ferrand, Grenoble, Marseille, Montpellier, Lille, Lyon, Toulouse, and Tours. To be eligible for the study, patients had to be adults and suffer from treatment-resistant depression.
2.3. Participants
Depressed patients were recruited and assessed individually in ambulatory settings. We recruited 131 depressed (83 women) patients. As described elsewhere [27], they met DSM-IV criteria for major depression. They experienced depressive symptom intensities corresponding to scores above 19 on the MADRS. They were clinically unresponsive to two successive and adequate sequences of antidepressants from two different pharmacological classes corresponding to at least stage II of the staging criteria proposed by Thase for defining TRD [28]. The exclusion criteria were suffering from bipolar disorders, psychotic disorders, OCD, eating disorders (with BMI < 15), somatoform disorders, and mood disorders related to substance abuse or misuse. From the general cohort (n = 397), we included only patients who fully completed the first (inclusion visit, V0) and the second visits (1 year later, V1) (n = 183). From the latter, we removed patients with a history of electro-convulsive therapy (ECT) during the last 6 months, sensory disorder, dyslexia, dysorthographia, dyscalculia, dysphasia, dyspraxia, language delay, epilepsy, meningitis, or multiple sclerosis. We also excluded patients who did not undergo depression and/or neuropsychological evaluations in one of two visits. In the end, 131 patients including 48 men and 83 women with the mean age of 51.22 (12.44) and the mean education level of 13.37 (2.91) (12 corresponding to high school diploma) meeting all the criteria were considered for this study. The neuropsychological evaluation took two hours, and the order of the tests was fixed. Some patients were not able or did not accept to perform all tests, explaining the missing data. At V1, we distinguished responder and non-responder patients. Patients were considered as responders if they showed a reduction of at least 50% of their Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) score at V1. The characteristics of the overall population are presented in Table 1 and Table 2.
2.4. Variables
2.4.1. Clinical Assessment
At baseline, a trained psychiatrist interviewed the participants using the DSM-IV Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). He also collected information about the patient’s education, marital status, onset and course of MDD, clinical features, and psychiatric comorbidities. Education level was determined as the number of school years from the first year of primary school. Twelve years corresponds to a high school diploma. We then included different standardized measures. Depression severity was rated and coupled with the evaluation of executive functions and processing speed, as previous studies already set out serious impairments among these cognitive processes in depressed subjects [8,25]. The impact of depression and related cognitive alterations on daily functioning was also assessed 6. Then, we used the following standardized measures:
The Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale—MADRS [29].
This is a 10-item semi-structured interview that allows for the measurement of depressive symptoms. Clinicians had to rate the different symptoms based on verbal or non-verbal information. Each item scored between 0 and 6. A greater score indicates more severe depressive symptoms. The French version of the MADRS presents a good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α from 0.85 to 0.94) [30]. The MADRS also presents a good sensitivity and specificity, 66 and 60%, respectively [31].
-
The Functional Assessment Short Test—FAST [32].
This is a 24-item semi-structured interview assessing daily functioning across different areas (autonomy, occupational functioning, cognitive functioning, financial problems, interpersonal problems, and hobbies). A greater score indicates more important difficulties. The French version of the FAST demonstrated a good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.97) and a good validity [33].
2.4.2. Neuropsychological Testing
-
The RL/RI-16 task.
This task evaluates verbal episodic memory. Patients had to learn 16 words associated with 16 cues [34]. Thereafter, they had to recall as many words as possible for two minutes. After this time, cues were given to the patient if necessary. Three free and cued recalls were performed immediately after learning with a distractive task between each recall. A recognition task was also performed. Patients were asked to recognize the words learned among distractors. Finally, a delayed recall was conducted 20 min after the third recall.
-
The D2 task.
This task assesses focused and sustained attention [35]. Patients had to cross d with two lines, among distractors.
-
The trail making test (TMT).
This test measures visual scanning and flexibility [36]. In the first condition (TMT-A), patients were invited to connect numbers in ascending order. In the second condition (TMT-B), they were asked to connect alternating numbers and letters in an assembling or alphabetic order.
-
The coding subtest of the WAIS-IV.
This test measures processing speed [37]. The patients were asked to copy as many symbols as possible, depending on a discriminative stimulus over two minutes.
-
The symbol subtest of the WAIS-IV.
This test measures processing speed [37]. The patients were required to search and cross symbols among distractors.
-
The arithmetic subtest of the WAIS-IV.
This test assesses working memory [37]. The patients had to orally solve mathematical problems.
-
The digit span subtest of the WAIS-IV.
This test evaluates working memory [37]. The patients had to recall digit sequences in front order, back order, and ascending order.
-
The similarities subtest of the WAIS-IV.
This test assesses verbal knowledge and abstraction [37]. The patients had to explain the similarities between two concepts.
-
The verbal fluencies test.
This test evaluates lexical access and flexibility [38]. In the first condition, patients had to provide as many words as possible belonging to a given semantic category (semantic fluencies). In a second condition, participants had to deliver as many words beginning with a letter as possible (phonological fluencies).
-
The double Baddeley task.
In this task, participants had to cross a line and then perform a digit span task [38]. They performed these tasks separately and then undertook both tasks simultaneously. This allows us to calculate a Mu score comparing the performance of patients while doing two tasks separately and simultaneously. These measure coordination abilities recruiting the central executive system in the working memory model [39].
2.5. Statistical Analysis
We first performed the power analysis. We used Gpower to do so. By convention, we fixed the alpha value at 0.05 and the beta value at 0.10. Based on a previous meta-analysis performed on cognitive deficits among depressed patients [8], we fixed the effect size as d = 0.38. This corresponds to the mean size of the smallest effect found on cognition. In this situation, the software proposes a sample size of 61 participants to identify such an effect.
We used normative data to transform raw scores from the neuropsychological tasks into normalized scores; 0 represents the mean and 1 represents the standard deviation. Then, the normality of the distribution was first assessed through graphical representations and showed an acceptable fit to continue with parametric tests. After that, we performed every further analysis using the normalized scores. First, we compared the mean performances of the patients to the mean 0 to assess the cognitive impairments. Only D2 scores were calculated in percentiles and involved a comparison to a different mean (m0 = 50). Then, we used paired t test comparisons to assess the evolution of the patients’ performances from V0 to V1. We calculated the delta of the different scores (Δ = V1scores − V0 score) and evaluated the correlations between the MADRS delta and the neuropsychological standardized scores delta. Finally, we used t tests to compare the performances of responder (N = 48) and non-responder (N = 83) patients at V1. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS, 23rd version.
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Analyses
At V1, among the 131 participants, two-thirds did not respond (n = 83, 47 women), while one-third responded (n = 48; 30 women). Descriptive data are displayed in Table 1 and Table 2.
3.2. Comparison to the Norm
For the complete study sample, at baseline, TRD patients performed worse than the norm of the general population expected for all tasks, except for in the TMT-A, Baddeley task, phonologic fluencies, and similarities test. At M12, the cognitive performances of participants did not significantly differ from the average theoretical performance of the general population for the symbol task, TMT-B task, arithmetic task, and draw 1 task.
At M0, the group of responder patients performed worse than the norm of the general population in all tasks, except for the TMT-A and -B, similarities, and draw 2 tasks. However, at M12, the difference from the norm was not significant for the symbol task, digit span, arithmetic task, free recall 1, and draw 1.
Finally, when we compared the D2 percentile scores to the mean 50, we found that the patients’ scores were always under the norm except at V0 and V1 and for both responder and non-responder patients (t between −7.73 and −2.18 and p < 0.05). The details of the results are presented in Table 3.
3.3. Comparison between V0 and V1
Among non-responder patients, we found a significant improvement in cognitive performances from V0 to V1 in multiple scores: MADRS, symbol task, TMT-B, free recall 1, and draw 1. We found even more evolution among responder patients, who improved in the MADRS, code task, symbol task, digit span, arithmetic task, similarities, D2, free recall 1 and 3, and draw 2. The details of the results are presented in Table 4.
3.4. Correlation between Δ-Depression and Δ-Neuropsychological Tasks
We only found a significant correlation between Δ-MADRS and the Δ-D2 (r = −0.26; p = 0.037; N = 62) and the Δ-digit span (r = −0.23; p = 0.044; n = 74).
3.5. Comparison between Responder and Non-Responder Patients
Comparing responder and non-responder patients, we found that responder patients performed worse at V0 in verbal fluencies. In contrast, they showed a greater score for the MADRS score and in the arithmetic task. The details of the results are presented in Table 5.
4. Discussion
The aim of this study was to characterize the course of cognitive impairments among patients suffering from TRD over a one-year follow-up. Consistent with previous results, we found the existence of cognitive impairments among both non-responder [7,8,9] and responder TRD patients [8,16,17,18]. The cognitive impairments were preferentially observed for speed processing, executive functions, and episodic memory. Cognitive impairment was less marked at one year compared to baseline, thereby leading us to consider that there may be an alleviation of cognitive impairments over time in TRD patients. This improvement was found for all cognitive areas and was more important for responder than non-responder patients at M12. This observation strengthens the idea of a causal relationship between depressive symptoms and cognitive impairments. Overall, the multiple forms of cognitive impairments support the global-diffuse hypothesis suggesting that cognitive deficit in depression is rather due to an overall decline in attention and not to specific alterations of distinct functions [40,41]. Moreover, earlier studies have supported that the cognitive disturbances were associated with a reduced connectivity of the fronto-parietal control system in depressed subjects. This may explain a more important engagement in negative self-referential thoughts than in the external environment [42].
This study also aimed to evaluate the potential prediction of neurocognitive performance on therapeutic response among patients suffering from TRD. Surprisingly, we found that responder patients perform worse in verbal fluency at baseline than non-responder patients. This is incongruent with previous results supporting the notion that cognitive impairments are associated with a poorer response to antidepressants [11,12,13,14]. This may suggest that cognitive impairments are not a significant predictor of therapeutic response among patients suffering from TRD. Another explanation could be that cognitive impairments are able to predict clinical outcomes after the first line of antidepressants but not the response to subsequent therapies.
These findings are particularly relevant for the further development of personalized and innovative treatment strategies targeting cognitive deficits. Indeed, it seems that depression contributes to significantly altered cognitive functioning. Therefore, it may be more relevant to evaluate the patient’s cognitive impairments after a therapeutic response. The main focus during the episode should be the treatment of the depressive episode, as cognitive impairments tend to reduce with symptom reduction. The treatment of cognitive impairments may follow. At this point, cognitive remediation should be proposed to the patients to increase their daily functioning.
Even if we know that patients suffering from MDD present cognitive impairments during the episode and after a therapeutic response, it remains unclear if such impairments are already present before the episode. Longitudinal studies could focus on such questions to identify the potential causal role of cognitive impairments in the development of MDD.
5. Limitations
The main limit of our study was the comparison of the patients’ performances to theoretical performances. Indeed, a more adapted methodological approach would have been the classical use of a control group. A second limit is a reduced statistical power among this sample. Furthermore, we only performed a one-year follow-up, while depression is a chronic disease. Further studies using long-term follow-up should focus on the evolution of cognitive impairments. Finally, we did not control confounding variables in order not to reduce the statistical power. In this context, the impact of concomitant medical treatments such as benzodiazepines/hypnotics on cognitive functions should further be taken into account in larger cohort studies on TRD.
6. Conclusions
Patients suffering from TRD have cognitive impairments affecting especially processing speed, executive functions, and verbal episodic memory during and after the episode. However, cognitive functioning tends to improve in relation to the alleviation of depressive symptoms. This suggests that cognitive impairments should be targeted in the second stage of therapeutic intervention.
Conceptualization, all authors; methodology, all authors; formal analysis: A.V.; investigation: all authors; data curation: A.V.; writing, A.V.; writing—review and editing, A.V., W.E.H., A.B. and D.B.; supervision, W.E.H. and D.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Patient consent was waived due as we performed retrospective data analysis.
Data are available on reasonable request from the first author.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
(a) Descriptive continuous variables at V0. (b) Descriptive continuous variables at V1. (c) Comorbidities.
(a) | |||||||||
Non-Responders | Responders | Total | |||||||
N | Mean | SD | N | Mean | SD | N | Mean | SD | |
Sociodemographic and clinical information | |||||||||
Age | 83 | 49.47 | 12.64 | 48 | 54.06 | 12.03 | 138 | 51.22 | 12.44 |
Education level | 78 | 14.90 | 3.20 | 44 | 14.14 | 3.35 | 122 | 13.37 | 2.91 |
Beginning of |
38 | 34.29 | 13.78 | 27 | 36.63 | 15.24 | 65 | 35.26 | 23.27 |
CGI | 82 | 4.98 | .875 | 47 | 5.09 | 0.90 | 135 | 5.01 | 0.86 |
FAST | 67 | 43.72 | 12.02 | 37 | 42.03 | 13.99 | 108 | 42.93 | 12.75 |
MADRS | 83 | 29.13 | 6.54 | 48 | 27.31 | 6.73 | 136 | 28.55 | 6.57 |
Neuropsychological raw scores | |||||||||
CODE | 69 | 57.51 | 20.32 | 46 | 50.89 | 17.17 | 119 | 54.79 | 19.03 |
SYMBOL | 69 | 26.84 | 9.77 | 46 | 24.57 | 7.93 | 119 | 25.90 | 8.99 |
DIGIT_SPAN | 68 | 23.78 | 6.42 | 45 | 23.04 | 5.61 | 117 | 23.46 | 6.01 |
TMTA_RT | 71 | 41.54 | 23.18 | 45 | 42.64 | 14.35 | 120 | 42.07 | 19.97 |
TMTB_RT | 70 | 97.37 | 49.55 | 44 | 108.68 | 65.45 | 118 | 101.90 | 55.44 |
TMTA_E | 70 | 1.29 | 8.93 | 45 | 0.64 | 1.97 | 120 | 0.37 | 1.58 |
TMTB_E | 69 | 2.71 | 15.28 | 44 | 1.25 | 2.83 | 118 | 1.03 | 3.54 |
FV_semantic | 69 | 27.86 | 9.36 | 46 | 23.30 | 9.32 | 119 | 25.91 | 9.46 |
FV_phonologic | 69 | 22.97 | 8.03 | 46 | 19.39 | 7.26 | 119 | 21.39 | 7.83 |
ARITMETIC | 66 | 13.67 | 4.34 | 41 | 13.20 | 3.49 | 111 | 13.36 | 4.01 |
SIMILITUDES | 67 | 21.64 | 6.39 | 45 | 20.27 | 5.87 | 116 | 21.01 | 6.19 |
D2GZF | 61 | 363.80 | 98.01 | 38 | 320.13 | 93.15 | 103 | 344.52 | 97.13 |
Baddeley task | 51 | 103.03 | 74.67 | 35 | 65.75 | 68.57 | 88 | −0.31 | 6.05 |
Immediate recall | 69 | 15.35 | 1.00 | 45 | 15.58 | 0.81 | 118 | 15.44 | 0.92 |
Free recall 1 | 69 | 8.32 | 1.94 | 45 | 7.82 | 2.35 | 118 | 8.10 | 2.09 |
Total recall 1 | 69 | 14.59 | 1.86 | 45 | 14.18 | 2.42 | 118 | 14.41 | 2.08 |
Free recall 2 | 69 | 10.00 | 2.31 | 45 | 9.56 | 2.18 | 118 | 9.79 | 2.29 |
Total recall 2 | 69 | 15.19 | 1.57 | 45 | 15.11 | 1.42 | 118 | 15.18 | 1.49 |
Free recall 3 | 69 | 11.30 | 2.61 | 45 | 10.51 | 2.55 | 118 | 10.93 | 2.60 |
Total recall 3 | 69 | 15.41 | 1.78 | 45 | 15.02 | 1.96 | 118 | 15.25 | 1.83 |
Sematic distractors | 69 | 0.06 | 0.29 | 44 | 0.36 | 2.41 | 117 | 0.17 | 1.49 |
Neutral distractors | 69 | 0.01 | 0.12 | 44 | 0.36 | 2.41 | 117 | 0.15 | 1.48 |
Delayed free recall | 69 | 11.36 | 2.78 | 45 | 11.00 | 3.01 | 118 | 11.16 | 2.84 |
Delayed total recall | 69 | 15.62 | 0.89 | 45 | 14.87 | 2.21 | 118 | 15.32 | 1.57 |
Intrusions | 68 | 0.32 | 1.00 | 45 | 0.82 | 2.46 | 117 | 0.51 | 1.72 |
Draw1 | 43 | 55.21 | 22.51 | 28 | 50.71 | 17.64 | 72 | 53.47 | 20.56 |
Draw2 | 41 | 45.78 | 18.60 | 28 | 38.46 | 14.42 | 70 | 42.81 | 17.17 |
Neuropsychological standardized scores expressed in standard deviations | |||||||||
CODE | 69 | −0.43 | 1.08 | 46 | −0.75 | 0.91 | 119 | −0.57 | 1.01 |
SYMBOL | 69 | −0.32 | 0.95 | 46 | −0.51 | 0.76 | 119 | −0.41 | 0.87 |
DIGIT_SPAN | 68 | −0.29 | 1.02 | 45 | −0.37 | 0.89 | 117 | −0.34 | 0.96 |
TMTA_RT | 70 | −0.17 | 1.11 | 45 | 0.05 | 0.75 | 119 | −0.10 | 1.01 |
TMTB_RT | 69 | −0.56 | 1.46 | 44 | −0.07 | 2.48 | 117 | −0.39 | 1.90 |
FV_semantic | 66 | −0.65 | 1.16 | 46 | −1.10 | 1.02 | 116 | −0.86 | 1.11 |
FV_phonologic | 67 | 0.01 | 1.28 | 46 | −0.57 | 1.17 | 117 | −0.25 | 1.25 |
ARITMETIC | 66 | −0.29 | 1.12 | 41 | −0.44 | 0.84 | 111 | −0.38 | 1.02 |
SIMILITUDES | 67 | 0.37 | 1.26 | 45 | 0.10 | 1.21 | 116 | 0.24 | 1.24 |
Baddeley task | 51 | 0.90 | 6.23 | 35 | −2.13 | 5.58 | 88 | 88.13 | 73.32 |
Free recall 1 | 69 | −0.66 | 0.82 | 45 | −0.80 | 1.03 | 118 | −0.73 | 0.89 |
Free recall 2 | 69 | −0.66 | 0.88 | 45 | −0.77 | 0.88 | 118 | −0.73 | 0.90 |
Free recall 3 | 69 | −0.71 | 1.09 | 45 | −0.97 | 1.07 | 118 | −0.84 | 1.09 |
Delayed free recall | 69 | -.83 | 1.12 | 45 | −0.89 | 1.31 | 118 | −0.88 | 1.19 |
Draw1 | 44 | −1.07 | 1.44 | 28 | −1.26 | 1.11 | 73 | −1.14 | 1.31 |
Draw2 | 41 | −1.05 | 1.26 | 28 | −1.42 | 1.03 | 70 | −1.20 | 1.17 |
Neuropsychological standardized scores expressed in percentiles | |||||||||
D2GZF | 61 | 31.71 | 29.53 | 38 | 20.92 | 23.19 | 103 | 26.66 | 27.49 |
(b) | |||||||||
Non-Responders | Responders | Total | |||||||
N | Mean | SD | N | Mean | SD | N | Mean | SD | |
Sociodemographic and clinical information | |||||||||
CGI | 79 | 4.51 | 1.16 | 48 | 2.15 | 1.17 | 130 | 3.58 | 1.64 |
MADRS | 83 | 24.86 | 7.86 | 48 | 6.52 | 4.37 | 133 | 17.96 | 11.17 |
Neuropsychological raw scores | |||||||||
CODE | 45 | 57.33 | 16.81 | 38 | 56.24 | 15.34 | 85 | 56.75 | 15.88 |
SYMBOL | 45 | 29.18 | 10.83 | 38 | 27.61 | 9.19 | 85 | 28.40 | 9.97 |
DIGIT_SPAN | 42 | 24.00 | 6.58 | 33 | 24.42 | 4.55 | 77 | 24.10 | 5.68 |
TMTA_RT | 46 | 39.52 | 23.43 | 36 | 39.08 | 13.70 | 84 | 39.17 | 19.45 |
TMTB_RT | 45 | 91.27 | 53.40 | 34 | 90.47 | 39.22 | 81 | 90.74 | 47.02 |
TMTA_E | 47 | 0.36 | 1.44 | 36 | 0.48 | 1.75 | 85 | 0.20 | 0.48 |
TMTB_E | 47 | 0.58 | 2.31 | 34 | 0.89 | 1.54 | 83 | 0.59 | 1.47 |
FV_semantic | 44 | 24.32 | 11.92 | 34 | 22.59 | 7.57 | 79 | 23.61 | 10.17 |
FV_phonologic | 44 | 22.95 | 11.97 | 34 | 19.53 | 4.53 | 79 | 21.37 | 9.55 |
ARITMETIC | 39 | 14.23 | 4.25 | 32 | 15.13 | 3.77 | 75 | 22.95 | 6.46 |
SIMILITUDES | 40 | 22.78 | 7.08 | 33 | 23.18 | 5.79 | 75 | 22.95 | 6.46 |
D2GZF | 39 | 389.64 | 102.13 | 32 | 385.34 | 68.69 | 73 | 385.78 | 87.60 |
Baddeley task | 30 | 90.70 | 22.73 | 30 | 85.49 | 20.73 | 61 | −0.32 | 1.71 |
Immediate recall | 46 | 15.57 | 0.75 | 36 | 15.42 | 0.84 | 84 | 15.51 | 0.78 |
Free recall 1 | 46 | 8.83 | 2.52 | 36 | 8.94 | 2.55 | 84 | 8.89 | 2.49 |
Total recall 1 | 46 | 14.98 | 1.25 | 36 | 14.58 | 1.71 | 84 | 14.81 | 1.47 |
Free recall 2 | 46 | 10.15 | 2.91 | 36 | 10.06 | 2.66 | 84 | 10.14 | 2.76 |
Total recall 2 | 46 | 15.54 | 0.84 | 36 | 15.19 | 1.56 | 84 | 15.39 | 1.20 |
Free recall 3 | 46 | 11.54 | 2.87 | 36 | 11.22 | 3.08 | 84 | 11.44 | 2.93 |
Total recall 3 | 46 | 15.76 | .74 | 36 | 15.61 | 0.90 | 84 | 15.70 | 0.80 |
Sematic distractors | 46 | 0.0 | 0.0 | 36 | 0.03 | 0.16 | 84 | 0.01 | 0.11 |
Neutral distractors | 46 | 0.0 | 0.0 | 36 | 0.03 | 0.16 | 84 | 0.01 | 0.11 |
Delayed free recall | 45 | 11.51 | 2.88 | 36 | 11.72 | 2.93 | 83 | 11.64 | 2.86 |
Delayed total recall | 45 | 15.49 | 1.34 | 36 | 15.69 | 0.90 | 83 | 15.58 | 1.15 |
Intrusions | 44 | 0.20 | 0.59 | 36 | 0.36 | 1.15 | 82 | 0.29 | 0.88 |
Draw1 | 20 | 62.50 | 23.18 | 21 | 64.14 | 18.41 | 42 | 63.26 | 20.38 |
Draw2 | 20 | 49.05 | 18.21 | 21 | 52.81 | 15.55 | 42 | 51.10 | 16.61 |
Neuropsychological standardized scores expressed in standard deviations | |||||||||
CODE | 45 | −0.49 | 0.98 | 38 | −0.38 | 0.92 | 85 | −0.45 | 0.94 |
SYMBOL | 45 | −0.03 | 1.14 | 38 | −0.12 | 0.92 | 85 | −0.09 | 1.03 |
DIGIT_SPAN | 42 | −0.31 | 0.99 | 33 | −0.12 | 0.73 | 77 | −0.24 | 0.88 |
TMTA_RT | 46 | 0.093 | 0.92 | 36 | 0.09 | 0.75 | 84 | 0.10 | 0.84 |
TMTB_RT | 46 | −0.23 | 1.40 | 34 | −0.06 | 1.18 | 82 | −0.16 | 1.30 |
FV_semantic | 51 | −0.49 | 1.33 | 9 | −0.42 | 1.33 | 60 | 0.48 | 1.28 |
FV_phonologic | 31 | 0.58 | 1.95 | 28 | −0.59 | 0.76 | 60 | 0.02 | 1.60 |
ARITMETIC | 39 | −0.12 | 1.11 | 32 | 0.13 | 1.08 | 75 | 0.70 | 1.34 |
SIMILITUDES | 40 | 0.68 | 1.40 | 33 | 0.75 | 1.27 | 75 | 0.70 | 1.34 |
Baddeley task | 30 | −0.12 | 1.84 | 30 | −0.52 | 1.61 | 61 | 88.14 | 21.55 |
Free recall 1 | 46 | −0.38 | 1.00 | 36 | −0.29 | 1.14 | 84 | −0.33 | 1.05 |
Free recall 2 | 46 | −0.55 | 1.19 | 36 | −0.56 | 1.10 | 84 | −0.55 | 1.13 |
Free recall 3 | 46 | −0.55 | 1.19 | 36 | −0.66 | 1.30 | 84 | −0.58 | 1.22 |
Delayed free recall | 45 | −0.71 | 1.18 | 36 | −0.57 | 1.35 | 83 | −0.64 | 1.24 |
Draw1 | 21 | −0.56 | 1.58 | 21 | −0.43 | 1.26 | 43 | −0.50 | 1.39 |
Draw2 | 20 | −0.68 | 1.31 | 21 | −0.41 | 1.18 | 42 | −0.54 | 1.22 |
Neuropsychological standardized scores expressed in percentiles | |||||||||
D2GZF | 39 | 39.31 | 30.53 | 32 | 37.22 | 26.40 | 73 | 37.56 | 28.58 |
(c) | |||||||||
Non-Responders | Responders | Total | |||||||
N | Percentage | N | Percentage | N | Percentage | ||||
Suicidality | Yes 68 |
Yes 88.3 |
Yes 34 |
Yes 75.6 |
Yes 102 |
Yes 83.5 |
|||
Panic disorder | Yes 15 |
Yes 21.1 |
Yes 6 |
Yes 13.6 |
Yes 21 |
Yes 18.3 |
|||
Agoraphobia | Yes 15 |
Yes 20 |
Yes 7 |
Yes 17.1 |
Yes 22 |
Yes 19 |
|||
Social phobia | Yes 16 |
Yes 61.5 |
Yes 11 |
Yes 91.7 |
Yes 27 |
Yes 32.5 |
|||
PTSD | Yes 3 |
Yes 3.9 |
Yes 4 |
Yes 8.9 |
Yes 7 |
Yes 5.5 |
|||
Alcohol addiction | Yes 2 |
Yes 2.4 |
Yes 0 |
Yes 0 |
Yes 2 |
Yes 0.8 |
|||
GAD | Yes 26 |
Yes 33.8 |
Yes 10 |
Yes 22.7 |
Yes 36 |
Yes 30.2 |
|||
Antisocial personality disorder | Yes 1 |
Yes 1.3 |
Yes 0 |
Yes 0 |
Yes 1 |
Yes 0.8 |
CGI: therapeutic score index; RS: raw score; RT: Reaction Time; E: number of errors.
Descriptive nominal variables.
Non-Responders | Responders | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
V0 | V1 | V0 | V1 | |
Sex, male/female | 36/47 | 36/47 | 18/30 | 18/30 |
SSRI, n | 21 | 22 | 19 | 19 |
SNRI, n | 29 | 29 | 24 | 24 |
Other antidepressants, n | 44 | 44 | 25 | 25 |
First generation antipsychotics, n | 12 | 12 | 7 | 7 |
Second generation antipsychotics, n | 34 | 33 | 13 | 13 |
Mood stabilizers, n | 22 | 23 | 13 | 13 |
Anxiolytics, n | 51 | 51 | 24 | 24 |
Antiepileptics, n | 29 | 29 | 10 | 10 |
SSRI: selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors; SNRI: serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors.
Comparison to the norm 0.
V0 | V1 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
t | ddl | Sig. Two Sided | Cohen’s d | t | ddl | Sig. Two Sided | Cohen’s d | |
Non-responders | ||||||||
CODE | −3.26 | 68 | <0.01 | 0.40 | −3.36 | 44 | <0.01 | 0.5 |
SYMBOL | −2.82 | 68 | <0.01 | 0.34 | −0.17 | 44 | 0.86 | 0.03 |
DIGIT_SPAN | −2.37 | 67 | 0.02 | 0.28 | −2.03 | 41 | <0.01 | 0.31 |
TMTA_RT | −1.31 | 69 | 0.19 | 0.15 | 0.69 | 45 | 0.50 | 0.10 |
TMTB_RT | −3.17 | 68 | <0.01 | 0.38 | −1.12 | 45 | 0.27 | 0.16 |
FV_semantic | −4.54 | 65 | <0.01 | 0.56 | −2.71 | 50 | <0.01 | 0.37 |
FV_phonologic | 0.02 | 66 | 0.99 | <0.01 | 1.64 | 30 | 0.11 | 0.30 |
ARITMETIC | −2.10 | 65 | 0.04 | 0.26 | −0.65 | 38 | 0.05 | 0.11 |
SIMILITUDES | 2.38 | 66 | 0.02 | 0.29 | 3.08 | 39 | <0.01 | 0.49 |
Baddeley task | 1.03 | 50 | 0.03 | 0.44 | −0.37 | 29 | 0.071 | 0.14 |
Free recall 1 | −6.79 | 68 | <0.01 | 0.80 | −2.58 | 45 | 0.013 | 0.38 |
Free recall 2 | −6.22 | 68 | <0.01 | 0.75 | −3.14 | 45 | <0.01 | 0.46 |
Free recall 3 | −5.43 | 68 | <0.01 | 0.65 | −3.15 | 45 | <0.01 | 0.46 |
Delayed free recall | −6.11 | 68 | <0.01 | 0.74 | −4.03 | 44 | <0.01 | 0.60 |
Draw1 | −4.90 | 43 | <0.01 | 0.74 | −1.61 | 20 | 0.12 | 0.35 |
Draw2 | −5.31 | 40 | <0.01 | 0.83 | −2.34 | 19 | 0.03 | 0.52 |
Responders | ||||||||
CODE | −5.58 | 45 | <0.01 | 0.82 | −2.53 | 37 | 0.02 | 0.41 |
SYMBOL | −4.52 | 45 | <0.01 | 0.67 | −0.83 | 37 | 0.41 | 0.13 |
DIGIT_SPAN | −2.75 | 44 | <0.01 | 0.42 | −0.95 | 32 | 0.35 | 0.16 |
TMTA_RT | 0.49 | 44 | 0.63 | 0.07 | 0.71 | 35 | 0.48 | 0.12 |
TMTB_RT | −0.19 | 43 | 0.85 | 0.03 | −0.31 | 33 | 0.76 | 0.05 |
FV_semantic | −7.31 | 45 | <0.01 | 1.12 | −2.71 | 50 | <0.01 | 0.32 |
FV_phonologic | −3.27 | 45 | <0.01 | 0.49 | −4.09 | 27 | <0.01 | 0.78 |
ARITMETIC | −3.36 | 40 | <0.01 | 0.52 | 0.68 | 31 | 0.50 | 0.12 |
SIMILITUDES | 0.53 | 44 | 0.59 | 0.08 | 3.39 | 32 | <0.01 | 0.59 |
Baddeley task | −2.26 | 34 | 0.03 | 0.38 | −1.77 | 29 | 0.09 | 0.32 |
Free recall 1 | −5.18 | 44 | <0.01 | 0.78 | −1.50 | 35 | 0.14 | 0.25 |
Free recall 2 | −5.92 | 44 | <0.01 | 0.87 | −3.07 | 35 | <0.01 | 0.51 |
Free recall 3 | −6.04 | 44 | <0.01 | 0.91 | −3.03 | 35 | <0.01 | 0.51 |
Delayed free recall | −4.58 | 44 | <0.01 | 0.68 | −2.54 | 35 | 0.02 | 0.42 |
Draw1 | −5.98 | 27 | <0.01 | 1.14 | −1.56 | 20 | 0.13 | 0.34 |
Draw2 | −7.28 | 27 | <0.01 | 1.38 | −1.60 | 20 | 0.13 | 0.35 |
RS: raw score; SD: standard deviations from the normative mean; RT: Reaction Time; P: percentile.
Evolution of the scores between V0 and V1.
Mean | SD | Cohen’s d | t | ddl | Sig. Two Sided | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Non-responders | ||||||
Clinical information | ||||||
MADRS | 4.28 | 7.21 | 0.59 | 5.40 | 82 | <0.01 |
Neuropsychological standardized scores expressed in standard deviations | ||||||
CODE | −0.03 | 0.84 | 0.04 | −0.25 | 41 | 0.80 |
SYMBOL | −0.46 | 0.93 | 0.49 | −3.22 | 41 | <0.01 |
DIGIT_SPAN | −0.01 | 0.55 | 0.02 | −0.19 | 40 | 0.85 |
TMTA_RT | −0.22 | 0.91 | 0.24 | −1.65 | 44 | 0.11 |
TMTB_RT | −0.43 | 1.34 | 0.32 | −2.14 | 43 | 0.04 |
FV_semantic | −0.03 | 1.71 | 0.02 | −0.12 | 44 | 0.90 |
FV_phonologic | −0.42 | 1.54 | 0.02 | −1.50 | 29 | 0.14 |
ARITMETIC | −0.58 | 2.92 | 0.20 | −1.22 | 37 | 0.30 |
SIMILITUDES | −0.24 | 0.93 | 0.26 | −1.56 | 37 | 0.13 |
Baddeley task | 12.47 | 64.60 | 0.19 | 1.00 | 26 | 0.32 |
Free recall 1 | −0.39 | 1.03 | 0.38 | −2.61 | 45 | 0.01 |
Free recall 2 | −0.21 | 1.27 | 0.17 | −1.15 | 45 | 0.26 |
Free recall 3 | −0.22 | 1.11 | 0.20 | −1.33 | 45 | 0.19 |
Delayed free recall | −0.19 | 1.07 | 0.18 | −1.22 | 44 | 0.23 |
Draw1 | −0.71 | 1.52 | 0.47 | −2.11 | 19 | 0.05 |
Draw2 | −0.28 | 1.30 | 0.22 | −0.90 | 17 | 0.38 |
Neuropsychological standardized scores expressed in percentiles | ||||||
D2GZF | −5.16 | 28.09 | 0.18 | −1.09 | 34 | 0.28 |
Responders | ||||||
Clinical information | ||||||
MADRS | 20.79 | 6.71 | 3.10 | 21.46 | 47 | <0.01 |
Neuropsychological standardized scores expressed in standard deviations | ||||||
CODE | −0.38 | 0.83 | 0.46 | −2.80 | 37 | <0.01 |
SYMBOL | −0.45 | 1.08 | 0.42 | −2.54 | 37 | 0.01 |
DIGIT_SPAN | −0.30 | 0.83 | 0.36 | −2.05 | 32 | 0.05 |
TMTA_RT | −0.11 | 0.90 | 0.12 | −0.72 | 34 | 0.48 |
TMTB_RT | −0.31 | 1.33 | 0.23 | −1.36 | 32 | 0.18 |
FV_phonologic | −0.18 | 1.31 | 0.14 | −0.72 | 27 | 0.48 |
ARITMETIC | −1.43 | 2.81 | 0.51 | −2.80 | 29 | <0.01 |
SIMILITUDES | −0.84 | 1.02 | 0.82 | −4.72 | 32 | <0.01 |
Baddeley task | −22.98 | 77.37 | 0.30 | −1.54 | 26 | 0.13 |
Free recall 1 | −0.57 | 1.06 | 0.54 | −3.22 | 35 | <0.01 |
Free recall 2 | −0.29 | 1.11 | 0.26 | −1.59 | 35 | 0.12 |
Free recall 3 | −0.38 | 1.13 | 0.34 | −2.05 | 35 | 0.05 |
Delayed free recall | −0.38 | 1.40 | 0.27 | −1.62 | 35 | 0.11 |
Draw1 | −0.67 | 1.64 | 0.41 | −1.81 | 19 | 0.08 |
Draw2 | −0.98 | 1.46 | 0.67 | −3.01 | 19 | <0.01 |
Neuropsychological standardized scores expressed in percentiles | ||||||
D2GZF | −15.14 | 19.00 | 0.80 | −4.14 | 26 | <0.01 |
RS: raw score; SD: standard deviations from the normative mean; RT: Reaction Time; P: percentile.
Comparison of responders and non-responders at V1.
t | ddl | p Value | Mean Difference | Cohen’s d | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Clinical information | |||||
MADRS | 1.52 | 129 | 0.13 | 1.82 | 0.27 |
ΔMADRS | 12.94 | 129 | <0.01 | 16.51 | 2.35 |
Neuropsychological standardized scores expressed in standard deviations | |||||
CODE | 1.65 | 113 | 0.10 | 0.32 | 0.31 |
SYMBOL | 1.10 | 113 | 0.27 | 0.18 | 0.21 |
DIGIT_SPAN | 0.38 | 111 | 0.70 | 0.07 | 0.07 |
TMTA_RT | −1.21 | 113 | 0.23 | −0.23 | 0.23 |
TMTB_RT | −1.32 | 111 | 0.19 | −0.49 | 0.25 |
FV_semantic | 2.15 | 110 | 0.03 | 0.46 | 0.41 |
FV_phonologic | 2.39 | 111 | 0.02 | 0.57 | 0.46 |
ARITMETIC | 0.72 | 105 | 0.47 | 0.44 | 0.14 |
SIMILITUDES | 1.13 | 110 | 0.26 | 0.27 | 0.22 |
Baddeley task | 2.35 | 84 | 0.02 | 37.28 | 0.51 |
Free recall 1 | 0.76 | 112 | 0.45 | 0.13 | 0.15 |
Free recall 2 | 0.66 | 112 | 0.51 | 0.11 | 0.13 |
Free recall 3 | 1.21 | 112 | 0.23 | 0.25 | 0.23 |
Delayed free recall | 0.30 | 112 | 0.77 | 0.07 | 0.32 |
Draw1 | 0.59 | 70 | 0.56 | 0.19 | 0.33 |
Draw2 | 1.27 | 67 | 0.21 | 0.37 | 0.32 |
ΔCODE | −1.85 | 78 | 0.07 | −0.34 | 0.42 |
ΔSYMBOL | 0.05 | 78 | 0.96 | 0.01 | 0.01 |
ΔDIGIT_SPAN | −1.75 | 72 | 0.08 | −0.28 | 0.42 |
ΔTMTA_RT | 0.57 | 78 | 0.57 | 0.12 | 0.13 |
ΔTMTB_RT | 0.38 | 75 | 0.70 | 0.11 | 0.09 |
ΔFV_semantic | 0.64 | 56 | 0.52 | 0.24 | |
ΔFV_phonologic | −1.22 | 66 | 0.23 | −0.85 | 0.17 |
ΔARITMETIC | −2.60 | 69 | 0.01 | −0.60 | 0.30 |
Baddeley task | −1.83 | 52 | 0.07 | −35.44 | 0.50 |
ΔFree recall 1 | −0.76 | 80 | 0.50 | −0.18 | 0.17 |
ΔFree recall 2 | −0.29 | 80 | 0.77 | −0.08 | 0.06 |
ΔFree recall 3 | −0.67 | 80 | 0.50 | −0.17 | 0.15 |
ΔDelayed free recall | −0.68 | 79 | 0.50 | −0.19 | 0.15 |
ΔDraw1 | 0.01 | 38 | 0.92 | 0.05 | 0.03 |
ΔDraw2 | −1.57 | 36 | 0.13 | −0.71 | 0.51 |
Neuropsychological standardized scores expressed in percentiles | |||||
D2GZF | 1.91 | 97 | 0.60 | 10.79 | 0.39 |
ΔD2 | −1.59 | 60 | 0.12 | −9.97 | 0.41 |
RT: Reaction Time; Δ: V1 − V0.
References
1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; 5th ed. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; Available online: https://psychiatryonline.org/doi/book/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 (accessed on 8 November 2018).
2. Sartorius, N. The economic and social burden of depression. J. Clin. Psychiatry; 2001; 62, (Suppl. 15), pp. 8-11. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11444765]
3. de Santé, H.A. Affections Psychiatriques de Longue durée Troubles Dépressifs Récurrents ou Persistants de l’Adulte. 2009; Available online: http://www.has-sante.fr/portail/upload/docs/application/pdf/2009-04/gm_ald23_troubles_depressifs_webavril2009.pdf (accessed on 10 April 2017).
4. Inserm. Dépression. Inserm—La Science Pour la Santé. 2021; Available online: https://www.inserm.fr/information-en-sante/dossiers-information/depression (accessed on 15 February 2021).
5. Morvan, Y.; Prieto, A.; Briffault, X.; Blanchet, A.; Dardennes, R.; Rouillon, F.; Lamboy, B. La dépression en France: Prévalence, facteurs associés et consommation de soins. Baromètre Santé; 2007; pp. 459-485. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/50298912_La_depression_en_France_Prevalence_facteurs_associes_et_consommation_de_soins (accessed on 15 July 2023).
6. Lam, R.W.; Kennedy, S.H.; McIntyre, R.S.; Khullar, A. Cognitive Dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder: Effects on Psychosocial Functioning and Implications for Treatment. Can. J. Psychiatry; 2014; 59, pp. 649-654. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/070674371405901206] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25702365]
7. Kindermann, S.S.; Brown, G.G. Depression and memory in the elderly: A meta-analysis. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol.; 1997; 19, pp. 625-642. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688639708403749] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9408794]
8. Rock, P.L.; Roiser, J.P.; Riedel, W.J.; Blackwell, A.D. Cognitive impairment in depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med.; 2014; 44, pp. 2029-2040. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713002535]
9. Snyder, H.R. Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: A meta-analysis and review. Psychol. Bull.; 2013; 139, pp. 81-132. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028727]
10. Burt, D.B.; Zembar, M.J.; Niederehe, G. Depression and memory impairment: A meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychol. Bull.; 1995; 117, pp. 285-305. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.285] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7724692]
11. Gallagher, D.; Savva, G.M.; Kenny, R.; Lawlor, B.A. What predicts persistent depression in older adults across Europe? Utility of clinical and neuropsychological predictors from the SHARE study. J. Affect. Disord.; 2013; 147, pp. 192-197. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.10.037]
12. López-Solà, C.; Subirà, M.; Serra-Blasco, M.; Vicent-Gil, M.; Navarra-Ventura, G.; Aguilar, E.; Acebillo, S.; Palao, D.J.; Cardoner, N. Is cognitive dysfunction involved in difficult-to-treat depression? Characterizing resistance from a cognitive perspective. Eur. Psychiatry J. Assoc. Eur. Psychiatr.; 2020; 63, e74. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2020.65]
13. Pimontel, M.A.; Rindskopf, D.; Rutherford, B.R.; Brown, P.J.; Roose, S.P.; Sneed, J.R. A Meta-Analysis of Executive Dysfunction and Antidepressant Treatment Response in Late-Life Depression. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Off. J. Am. Assoc. Geriatr. Psychiatry; 2016; 24, pp. 31-41. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2015.05.010]
14. Sanders, M. Psychologue Bordeaux TECC » Thérapie Emotionnelle Cognitive et Comportementale (TECC). 2014; Available online: http://www.psychologue-bordeaux-tecc.fr/therapie-emotionnelle-cognitive-et-comportementale-tecc/ (accessed on 6 April 2016).
15. Lee, R.S.C.; Hermens, D.F.; Porter, M.A.; Redoblado-Hodge, M.A. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode Major Depressive Disorder. J. Affect. Disord.; 2012; 140, pp. 113-124. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.10.023]
16. Baune, B.T.; Miller, R.; McAfoose, J.; Johnson, M.; Quirk, F.; Mitchell, D. The role of cognitive impairment in general functioning in major depression. Psychiatry Res.; 2010; 176, pp. 183-189. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.12.001]
17. Bhalla, R.K.; Butters, M.A.; Mulsant, B.H.; Begley, A.E.; Zmuda, M.D.; Schoderbek, B.; Pollock, B.G.; Reynolds, C.F.; Becker, J.T. Persistence of neuropsychologic deficits in the remitted state of late-life depression. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Off. J. Am. Assoc. Geriatr. Psychiatry; 2006; 14, pp. 419-427. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.JGP.0000203130.45421.69] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16670246]
18. Hasselbalch, B.J.; Knorr, U.; Kessing, L.V. Cognitive impairment in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder: A systematic review. J. Affect. Disord.; 2011; 134, pp. 20-31. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.011] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21163534]
19. Cambridge, O.R.; Knight, M.J.; Mills, N.; Baune, B.T. The clinical relationship between cognitive impairment and psychosocial functioning in major depressive disorder: A systematic review. Psychiatry Res.; 2018; 269, pp. 157-171. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.033] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30149273]
20. Brewster, G.S.; Peterson, L.; Roker, R.; Ellis, M.L.; Edwards, J.D. Depressive Symptoms, Cognition, and Everyday Function Among Community-Residing Older Adults. J. Aging Health; 2017; 29, pp. 367-388. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0898264316635587]
21. McIntyre, R.S.; Cha, D.S.; Soczynska, J.K.; Woldeyohannes, H.O.; Gallaugher, L.A.; Kudlow, P.; Alsuwaidan, M.; Baskaran, A. Cognitive deficits and functional outcomes in major depressive disorder: Determinants, substrates, and treatment interventions. Depress. Anxiety; 2013; 30, pp. 515-527. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.22063]
22. Knight, M.J.; Air, T.; Baune, B.T. The role of cognitive impairment in psychosocial functioning in remitted depression. J. Affect. Disord.; 2018; 235, pp. 129-134. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.04.051]
23. Fava, M. Diagnosis and definition of treatment-resistant depression. Biol. Psychiatry; 2003; 53, pp. 649-659. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00231-2]
24. Rao, D.; Xu, G.; Lu, Z.; Liang, H.; Lin, K.; Tang, M. Comparative Study of Cognitive Function Between Treatment-Resistant Depressive Patients and First-Episode Depressive Patients. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat.; 2019; 15, pp. 3411-3417. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S226405]
25. Vancappel, A.; Dansou, Y.; Godin, O.; Haffen, E.; Yrondi, A.; Stéphan, F.; Richieri, R.; Molière, F.; Horn, M.; Allauze, E. et al. Cognitive impairments in treatment-resistant depression: Results from the French cohort of outpatients (FACE-DR). J. Affect. Disord. Rep.; 2021; 6, 100272. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadr.2021.100272]
26. Priyamvada, R.; Ranjan, R.; Chaudhury, S. Cognitive rehabilitation of attention and memory in depression. Ind. Psychiatry J.; 2015; 24, pp. 48-53. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.4103/0972-6748.160932] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26257483]
27. Yrondi, A.; Bennabi, D.; Haffen, E.; Garnier, M.; Bellivier, F.; Bourgerol, T.; Camus, V.; D’amato, T.; Doumy, O.; Haesebaert, F. et al. Significant Need for a French Network of Expert Centers Enabling a Better Characterization and Management of Treatment-Resistant Depression (Fondation FondaMental). Front. Psychiatry; 2017; 8, 244. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00244] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29225582]
28. Thase, M.E.; Rush, A.J. When at first you don’t succeed: Sequential strategies for antidepressant nonresponders. J. Clin. Psychiatry; 1997; 58, (Suppl. 13), pp. 23-29.
29. Montgomery, S.A.; Asberg, M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br. J. Psychiatry J. Ment. Sci.; 1979; 134, pp. 382-389. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.134.4.382] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/444788]
30. Bondolfi, G.; Jermann, F.; Rouget, B.W.; Gex-Fabry, M.; McQuillan, A.; Dupont-Willemin, A.; Aubry, J.-M.; Nguyen, C. Self- and clinician-rated Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale: Evaluation in clinical practice. J. Affect. Disord.; 2010; 121, pp. 268-272. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.06.037]
31. Hobden, B.; Schwandt, M.L.; Carey, M.; Lee, M.R.; Farokhnia, M.; Bouhlal, S.; Oldmeadow, C.; Leggio, L. The Validity of the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale in an Inpatient Sample with Alcohol Dependence. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res.; 2017; 41, pp. 1220-1227. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/acer.13400]
32. Rosa, A.R.; Sánchez-Moreno, J.; Martínez-Aran, A.; Salamero, M.; Torrent, C.; Reinares, M.; Comes, M.; Colom, F.; Van Riel, W.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L. et al. Validity and reliability of the Functioning Assessment Short Test (FAST) in bipolar disorder. Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Ment. Health CP EMH; 2007; 3, 5. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1745-0179-3-5]
33. Claire, D.; Raust, A.; Fouques, D.; Barbato, A.; Etain, B.; Henry, C. Validation of the French version of the functioning assessment short test (FAST) in patients with bipolar disorder. A study from the french bipolar expert centers network. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol.; 2012; 28, pp. e62-e63. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.yic.0000423355.40365.cb]
34. Buschke, H. Cued recall in amnesia. J. Clin. Neuropsychol.; 1984; 6, pp. 433-440. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688638408401233]
35. Brickenkamp, R. D2 Test of Attention; Hogrefe & Huber: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999.
36. Reitan, R.M. The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J. Consult Psychol.; 1955; 19, pp. 393-394. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0044509]
37. Wechsler, D.; Saklofske, D. WAIS-IV Echelle d’Intelligence de Wechsler Pour Adultes: Manuel d’Administration et de Cotation; 4th ed. Coédition ECPA: Montreuil, France, 2011.
38. Godefroy, O. Fonctions Exécutives et Pathologies Neurologiques et Psychiatriques: Evaluation en Pratique Clinique; De Boeck Universite: Marseille, France, 2008.
39. Repovs, G.; Baddeley, A. The multi-component model of working memory: Explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuroscience; 2006; 139, pp. 5-21. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.12.061] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16517088]
40. Landrø, N.I.; Stiles, T.C.; Sletvold, H. Neuropsychological function in nonpsychotic unipolar major depression. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol.; 2001; 14, pp. 233-240. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11725217]
41. Kriesche, D.; Woll, C.F.J.; Tschentscher, N.; Engel, R.R.; Karch, S. Neurocognitive deficits in depression: A systematic review of cognitive impairment in the acute and remitted state. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci.; 2022; 273, pp. 1105-1128. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00406-022-01479-5] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36048295]
42. Kaiser, R.H.; Andrews-Hanna, J.R.; Wager, T.D.; Pizzagalli, D.A. Large-Scale Network Dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder: A Meta-analysis of Resting-State Functional Connectivity. JAMA Psychiatry; 2015; 72, pp. 603-611. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0071]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Previous studies set out profound cognitive impairments in subjects with treatment-resistant depression (TRD). However, little is known about the course of such alterations depending on levels of improvement in those patients followed longitudinally. The main objective of this study was to describe the course of cognitive impairments in responder versus non-responder TRD patients at one-year follow-up. The second aim was to evaluate the predictive aspect of cognitive impairments to treatment resistance in patients suffering from TRD. We included 131 patients from a longitudinal cohort (FACE-DR) of the French Network of Expert TRD Centers. They undertook comprehensive sociodemographic, clinical, global functioning, and neuropsychological testing (TMT, Baddeley task, verbal fluencies, WAIS-4 subtests, D2 and RLRI-16) at baseline (V0) and one-year follow-up (V1). Most patients (n = 83; 63.36%) did not respond (47 women, 49.47 ± 12.64 years old), while one-third of patients responded (n = 48, 30 women, 54.06 ± 12.03 years old). We compared the cognitive performances of participants to average theoretical performances in the general population. In addition, we compared the cognitive performances of patients between V1 and V0 and responder versus non-responder patients at V1. We observed cognitive impairments during the episode and after a therapeutic response. Overall, each of them tended to show an increase in their cognitive scores. Improvement was more prominent in responders at V1 compared to their non-responder counterparts. They experienced a more marked improvement in code, digit span, arithmetic, similarities, and D2 tasks. Patients suffering from TRD have significant cognitive impairments that persist but alleviate after therapeutic response. Cognitive remediation should be proposed after therapeutic response to improve efficiency and increase the daily functioning.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details









1 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
2 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
3 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
4 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
5 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
6 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
7 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
8 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
9 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
10 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
11 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
12 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
13 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;
14 Fondation FondaMental, 94000 Créteil, France;