Introduction
Late childhood and its transition toward adolescence is a period marked by decreasing parental influence alongside increasing peer influence. It is a period critical for social interaction, during which friendships are especially important (Blakemore and Mills, 2014). During this period, the social brain is still undergoing significant development, in parallel with changes in social cognition (Mills et al., 2014). Meanwhile, evidence suggests that psychiatric disorders often have an onset in adolescence (Kessler et al., 2005), which may be partly influenced by the concurrent changes in the social environment and brain (Paus et al., 2008). Therefore, understanding the relationship between friendship, mental health, and cognition during this period, and the underlying brain mechanisms, is of considerable clinical and public health importance.
It has been well established that positive social relationships such as close friendships are essential for mental health and cognition in children and adolescents (Marion et al., 2013; Narr et al., 2019; Wentzel et al., 2018). However, it remains unclear whether having more close friends is necessarily better. Cognitive constraints and time resources limit the number of close social ties that an individual can maintain simultaneously (Dunbar, 2018). The innermost layer of the friendship group with the highest emotional closeness is around five close friends (the so-called Dunbar’s number) (Zhou et al., 2005). For now, only a few empirical studies have examined the nonlinear association between social relationships, mental health, and cognition in children and adolescents. For instance, a large study of a nationally representative sample in the United States reported that adolescents with either too many or too few friends had higher levels of depressive symptoms (Falci and McNeely, 2009). Two large-scale studies reported that the benefits of social interactions for well-being were nearly negligible once the quantity reached a moderate level (Kushlev et al., 2018; Ren et al., 2022). Additionally, a significant U-shaped effect was detected between positive relations with others and cognitive performance (Brown et al., 2021). Overall, the assumption of linearity still dominates studies of social relationships, and the effect of the friendship network size at the high end remains largely unexplored.
Despite a large body of evidence linking friendships to mental health and cognition, we know relatively little about the underlying mechanisms involved (Pfeifer and Allen, 2021). The social brain hypothesis proposes that the evolution of brain size is driven by complex social selection pressures (Dunbar and Shultz, 2007). Animal studies have shown that social network size can predict the volume of the mid-superior temporal sulcus (Sallet et al., 2011; Testard et al., 2022), a region in which neurons respond to socially relevant stimuli such as face expression and head movement to make or break social contact (Hasselmo et al., 1989a; Hasselmo et al., 1989b). In human neuroimaging studies, several key brain regions, including the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC, i.e. orbitofrontal [OFC] and anterior cingulate cortex [ACC]), the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus (STS), the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), amygdala, and the anterior insula, have been implicated in social cognitive processes (Frith and Frith, 2007). Moreover, there has been an increasing number of studies dedicated to investigating the social brain in children and adolescents over the past decade (Andrews et al., 2021; Burnett et al., 2011).
At the molecular level, the μ-opioid receptor is widely distributed in the brain, particularly in regions associated with social pain such as the ACC and anterior insula (Baumgärtner et al., 2006). Recent studies have identified the crucial role of μ-opioid receptors in forming and maintaining friendships (Dunbar, 2018), and variations in the μ-opioid receptor gene have been related to individual differences in rejection sensitivity (Way et al., 2009). In addition, other neurotransmitters, including dopamine, serotonin, GABA, and noradrenaline, may interact with the opioids, and are involved in social affiliation and social behavior (Machin and Dunbar, 2011). Dysregulation of the social brain and neurotransmitter systems is also implicated in the pathophysiology of major psychiatric disorders (Porcelli et al., 2019). Taken together, it is suggested that changes in the social brain might explain the relationship between social connections and mental health (Lamblin et al., 2017). However, the empirical evidence on this topic is limited in late childhood and adolescence.
In this study, we aimed to investigate the relationship between the number of close friends, mental health, and cognitive outcomes, with a focus on potential nonlinear associations. We used data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study (Karcher and Barch, 2021) and an independent social network dataset (Paluck et al., 2016). These datasets provided reliable measures of close friend quantity, mental health, and cognition, and included a combined total of more than 23,000 participants (Figure 1a). To evaluate the potential nonlinear relation between friendship quantity (predictor) and mental health and cognition (outcome), two different analytic approaches were utilized. Specifically, we examined the presence of a significant quadratic term as an indicator of nonlinearity, and subsequently conducted a two-lines test (Simonsohn, 2018) to estimate an interrupted regression and identify the breakpoint (Figure 1b). To explore the underlying neurobiological mechanisms, we further tested the nonlinear association between the number of close friends and brain structure. We then correlated the related brain differences with the density of eight neurotransmitter systems, as well as the expression of the μ-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) and the κ-opioid receptor gene (OPRK1) (Figure 1c). Finally, longitudinal and mediation analyses were conducted to uncover the direction and direct association between the number of close friends, mental health, cognition, and brain structure (Figure 1d). Based on the existing literature, we hypothesized that the number of close friends was nonlinearly related to mental health, cognition, and the social brain; and that this relationship could potentially be explained by brain differences and molecular mechanisms.
Figure 1.
The study workflow.
(a) Study datasets and key measures used in the present study. (b) A two-step approach to evaluate the nonlinear association. The number of close friends is used as the independent variable in quadratic regression models. Once a significant squared term (‘b’) is found, a two-lines test is conducted to estimate the breakpoint. Then participants are classified into two groups according to the breakpoint. (c) Correlation of brain differences related to the number of close friends with neurotransmitter density and gene expression level. (d) Longitudinal and mediation analysis of the number of close friends, ADHD symptoms, crystalized intelligence, and the significant surface areas.
Results
Demographic characteristics
In the ABCD study, 7512 participants (3625 [48.3%] females, aged 9.91 ± 0.62 y) provided self-reported number of close friends, a broad range of mental health and cognitive measures, and quality-controlled MRI data at baseline (Table 1), and 4290 of them (2044 [47.7%] females, aged 11.49 ± 0.66 y) had 2-year follow-up data available (Table 2). In the social network dataset, 16,065 subjects from 48 middle schools (8065 [50.3%] female, aged 12.00 ± 1.03 y) who had complete key variables were included (Table 3).
Table 1.
Characteristics of the study population in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study at baseline*.
≤5 close friends(N = 4863) | >5 close friends(N = 2649) | p-value† | |
---|---|---|---|
Age | 9.91 ± 0.61 | 9.93 ± 0.63 | 0.08 |
Sex | 0.02 | ||
Female | 2299 (47.3%) | 1326 (50.1%) | |
Male | 2564 (52.7%) | 1323 (49.9%) | |
Race | 0.001 | ||
White | 2672 (54,9%) | 1497 (56.5%) | |
Black | 561 (11,5%) | 370 (14.0%) | |
Hispanic | 1015 (20.9%) | 485 (18.3%) | |
Asian | 109 (2.2%) | 45 (1.7%) | |
Other | 506 (10.4%) | 252 (9.5%) | |
Family size ‡ | 4.69 ± 1.82 | 4.58 ± 1.84 | 0.01 |
Family income | 7.28 ± 2.34 | 7.4 ± 2.35 | 0.03 |
Parental education | 16.82 ± 2.62 | 16.97 ± 2.53 | 0.02 |
Body mass index | 18.71 ± 4.11 | 18.73 ± 4.11 | 0.77 |
Puberty | 1.73 ± 0.86 | 1.78 ± 0.88 | 0.02 |
Urbanization § | 0.12 | ||
Rural | 395 (8.5%) | 220 (8.7%) | |
Urban clusters | 167 (3.6%) | 68 (2.7%) | |
Urbanized area | 4,074 (87.9%) | 2,229 (88.6%) | |
Total close friends | 3.04 ± 1.36 | 12.19 ± 13.14 | <0.001 |
Same-sex close friends | 2.41 ± 1.22 | 9.13 ± 9.82 | <0.001 |
Opposite-sex close friends | 0.63 ± 0.79 | 3.05 ± 5.79 | <0.001 |
*
Values are mean ± SDor N (%).
†
For continuous data,
‡
4780 and 2624 participants in ≤5 and >5 close friends groups have family size data, respectively.
§
4636 and 2517 participants in ≤5 and >5 close friends groups have urbanization data, respectively.
Table 2.
Characteristics of the study population in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study at 2-year follow-up (N = 4290).
Value* | |
---|---|
Age | 11.49 ± 0.66 |
Sex | |
Female | 2044 (47.7%) |
Male | 2246 (52.4%) |
Race | |
White | 2612 (60.9%) |
Black | 385 (9.0%) |
Hispanic | 791 (18.4%) |
Asian | 89 (2.1%) |
Other | 413 (9.6%) |
Family income | 7.83 ± 2.04 |
Parental education | 17.11 ± 2.44 |
Body mass index | 20.35 ± 4.63 |
Puberty | 2.53 ± 1.05 |
Total close friends | 6.82 ± 8.37 |
Same-sex close friends | 4.99 ± 5.92 |
Opposite-sex close friends | 1.83 ± 3.66 |
*
Values are mean ± SDor N (%).
Table 3.
Characteristics of the study population in the social network dataset (N = 16,056).
Variable | Value * |
---|---|
Age | 12.00 ± 1.03 |
Sex | |
Female | 8068 (50.3%) |
Male | 7988 (49.8%) |
Grade | |
5th grade | 1107 (6.9%) |
6th grade | 4190 (26.1%) |
7th grade | 5279 (32.9%) |
8th grade | 5480 (34.1%) |
New to the school | |
New to school | 4315 (26.9%) |
Returning to school | 11,741 (73.1%) |
Most friends go to this school | |
Yes | 14,429 (89.9%) |
No | 1627 (10.1%) |
Outdegree | 8.08 ± 2.43 |
Indegree | 7.83 ± 4.42 |
Reciprocal degree | 3.82 ± 2.14 |
Well-being | 0.86 ± 0.24 |
Grade point average | 3.17 ± 0.61 |
*
Values are mean ± SD or N (%).
Nonlinear association between the number of close friends, mental health, and cognition
The number of close friends was significantly associated with 12 out of 20 mental health measures, and 7 out of 10 cognitive scores at baseline (the total F-value of the linear and quadratic terms, p<0.05/30; Figure 2a–g). For these 18 outcomes except the withdrawn/depressed, all quadratic terms reached significance after Bonferroni corrections (p<0.05/60), and all quadratic models provided a significantly better fit than the corresponding linear models (F = [13.25, 55.53], all p<0.001). For mental health, the greatest effect sizes of the quadratic terms were observed for social problems (
Figure 2.
Results of behavior-level nonlinear association analyses in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study at baseline.
(a) Results of quadratic regression models. The total
Figure 2—figure supplement 1.
Effect sizes of linear and quadratic terms of close friend number in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study at baseline.
Mental health and cognitive outcomes with a significant
Figure 2—figure supplement 2.
Behavior-level results of quadratic regression models by random choice of the siblings in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study at baseline.
Results of quadratic regression models by randomly selecting siblings once (a) and twice (b). (c) Correlations of
Figure 2—figure supplement 3.
Results of behavior-level nonlinear association analyses in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study at baseline in girls and boys, respectively.
(a) Results of quadratic regression models in girls (N = 3625). (b) Results of quadratic regression models in boys (N = 3887). (c) Results of two-lines tests in girls. (d) Results of two-lines tests in boys.
Figure 2—figure supplement 4.
Nonlinear association of the number of same-sex and opposite-sex close friends with mental health and cognition in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study at baseline.
Results of mental health and cognitive outcomes quadratically regressed on the number of same-sex close friends (a) and opposite-sex close friends (b). (c) Effect sizes of linear and quadratic terms of same-sex close friend number. (d) Results of two-lines tests of same-sex close friend number.
Figure 2—figure supplement 5.
Results of behavior-level nonlinear association analyses in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study at 2-year follow-up.
(a) Results of quadratic regression models using cross-sectional 2-year follow-up data. An asterisk indicates statistical significance after Bonferroni correction (i.e., p<0.05/26 for
The average breakpoint of the number of close friends for the mental health and cognitive outcomes with significant quadratic terms was 4.89 ± 0.68 (Figure 2h). Both mental health and cognition were positively associated with close friend quantity, with an ideal number of around 5. These nonlinear associations were consistent in males and females (Figure 2—figure supplement 3). However, the number of same-sex close friends, but not of opposite-sex close friends, was significantly related to mental health and cognition (26 out of 30 measures with a significant
Finally, the same analyses were performed using the cross-sectional data collected at 2 y later (Figure 2—figure supplement 5). The number of close friends was significantly associated with 10 out of 20 mental health measures, and 3 out of 6 cognitive scores. Significant nonlinear associations were observed between close friend quantity and five measures, with an average breakpoint of 4.60 ± 0.55 close friends. The greatest effect sizes of the quadratic terms were observed for attention problems (
The number of close friends was quadratically associated with brain structure
In the ABCD study, the number of close friends was significantly associated with the total cortical area (
Figure 3.
Nonlinear association between the number of close friends and cortical area in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study at baseline.
(a) Cortical areas significantly associated with the number of close friends after FDR correction (i.e., 360 regions) based on the total
Figure 3—figure supplement 1.
Nonlinear association between the number of close friends and cortical areas by random choice of the siblings.
Cortical areas significantly associated with the number of close friends by randomly selecting siblings once (a) and twice (b). (c) Correlations of
Figure 3—figure supplement 2.
Nonlinear association between the number of close friends and cortical volumes.
(a) Cortical volumes significantly associated with the number of close friends after FDR correction (i.e., 360 regions) based on the total
Figure 3—figure supplement 3.
Relationship between cortical area and cortical volume.
(a) Association between the
Figure 3—figure supplement 4.
Results of two-lines tests for significant cortical areas.
The breakpoint of close friend quantity and the estimated coefficients with 95% confidence intervals of linear regressions in each group separated by the breakpoint are reported.
Figure 3—figure supplement 5.
Results of linear association analyses between close friend quantity and cortical area in ≤5 and >5 groups, respectively.
(a) Unthresholded
After false discovery rate (FDR) correction (
Further, two-lines tests suggested that participants with around five close friends (breakpoint = 5.30 ± 0.85) had the largest areas in these cortical regions (Figure 3—figure supplement 4). To illustrate the patterns of nonlinear relationships, we performed linear regression models in participants with ≤5 and >5 close friends, respectively. Similar regions to those found with quadratic models including the OFC, insula, the ACC, and temporal cortex were significant after FDR correction in the ≤5 group (Figure 3—figure supplement 5a and b), and the largest effect size was observed in the OFC (Figure 3—figure supplement 5c). However, the number of close friends was not related to cortical area in the >5 group (Figure 3—figure supplement 5d). Moreover, the cortical area associative patterns of close friend quantity in the two groups were not correlated (
Relationship to molecular architecture
As the number of close friends was nonlinearly associated with cortical area and the significant regions were only found in participants with no more than five close friends, we focused on the brain associative pattern for the number of close friends in the ≤5 group. We found that the correlations between the spatial pattern of cortical area related to the number of close friends and densities of neurotransmitters were not significant except for the μ-opioid receptor (Spearman’s rho = 0.44, Bonferroni corrected
Figure 4.
Spatial correlation between cortical area differences related to the number of close friends in children with ≤5 close friends and density of neurotransmitters and gene expression level.
(a) Bootstrapped Spearman correlations (10,000 times) between
Longitudinal and mediation results
As the nonlinear association between the number of close friends and ADHD symptoms is relatively strong and robust, and for cognitive outcomes, only crystalized intelligence was collected at 2-year follow-up in the ABCD study, we focused on these two measures in longitudinal and mediation analyses. The cross-lagged panel model (CLPM) revealed that participants having closer to five close friends had fewer ADHD symptoms 2 y later (
Figure 5.
Results of longitudinal and mediation analysis in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ABCD) study.
(a) Cross-lagged panel model (CLPM) of the absolute value of close friendship quantity to 5 and ADHD symptoms (N = 6013). Comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.996, Tucker–Lewis index (TFI) = 0.97, standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR) = 0.002, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.015. (b) CLPM of the absolute value of close friendship quantity to 5 and crystalized intelligence (N = 6013). CFI = 0.994, TFI = 0.96, SRMR = 0.003, RMSEA = 0.025. (c) Mediation analysis of close friendship quantity, the total area of significant regions, and ADHD symptoms. (d) The effect of individual significant cortical areas that mediated the association between close friendship quantity and ADHD symptoms after FDR correction. (e) Mediation analysis of close friendship quantity, the total area of significant regions and crystalized intelligence. (f) The effect of individual significant cortical areas that mediated the association between close friendship quantity and crystalized intelligence after FDR correction.
Figure 5—figure supplement 1.
Cross-lagged panel models (CLPMs) of close friend number and Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental (ADHD) symptoms, and crystalized intelligence in ≤5 and >5 groups, respectively.
CLPMs of close friend number and ADHD symptoms in the ≤5 group (a) and in the >5 group (b). CLPM of close friend number and crystalized intelligence in the ≤5 group (c) and in the >5 group (d).
Mediation analyses were used to determine whether and the extent to which the association between the number of close friends, ADHD symptoms, and crystalized intelligence could be explained by the identified cortical areas in the ≤5 group. The total identified cortical area partly mediated the association between the number of close friends and ADHD symptoms (6.5%, 95% CI [3.4%, 14%]; path a*b: –0.005, 95% CI [-0.008,–0.003]; Figure 5c), and the mediation effects of individual significant regions ranged from 1.52 to 4.68% (Figure 5d). Similarly, the association between the number of close friends and crystalized intelligence was partly mediated by the total identified cortical area (13.5%, 95% CI [8.1%, 27%]; path a*b: 0.008, 95% CI [0.005, 0.01]; Figure 5e), ranging from 2.58 to 8.52% for each significant region (Figure 5f).
Findings in an independent social network dataset
Utilizing the social network dataset allowed us to extend findings in the ABCD study, as it is an independent and large dataset, a directed friendship network was generated by nomination, and different measures of mental health and cognition were collected (i.e., well-being and grade point average [GPA]). Three indicators of friendship network size (i.e., outdegree, indegree, and reciprocal degree; Figure 6) were significantly related to well-being (indegree:
Figure 6.
Distribution of outdegree, indegree, and reciprocal degree in the social network dataset.
(a) Distribution of outdegree which is the number of outward nominations. (b) Distribution of indegree which is the number of inward nominations. (c) Distribution of reciprocal degree which is the number of reciprocal nominations. Relationship of well-being with outdegree (d), indegree (e), and reciprocal degree (f). Relationship of grade point average (GPA) with outdegree (g), indegree (h), and reciprocal degree (i). In each bin, the mean (i.e., black dot) and standard error (i.e., error bar) of the dependent variable are shown. The red line is the fitted quadratic model. For outdegree, sample sizes of bins 1-11 are 92, 87, 208, 519, 803, 1230, 1326, 1286, 1235, 1151 and 8119. For indegree, sample sizes of bins 1-14 are 617, 728, 1116, 1341, 1526, 1563, 1532, 1462, 1273, 1095, 870, 1281, 716 and 936. For reciprocal degree, sample sizes of bins 1-11 are 797, 1561, 2356, 2774, 2765, 2260, 1685, 1039, 524, 220 and 75.
Figure 6—figure supplement 1.
Results of nonlinear association analysis in the social network dataset.
(a) Results of well-being quadratically regressed on outdegree, indegree, and reciprocal degree, respectively. (b) Results of grade point average (GPA) quadratically regressed on outdegree, indegree, and reciprocal degree, respectively. The total
Discussion
The present study showed that close friendship quantity was associated with better mental health and higher cognitive functions in late childhood, and that the beneficial association diminished or reversed when increasing the number of close friends beyond a moderate level. The results also support the hypothesis that a quadratic association exists between the number of close friends and the areas of social brain regions such as the OFC, the ACC, insula, the anterior temporal cortex, and the TPJ. These regions mediated the nonlinear association between close friendship quantity and behavior. Furthermore, the brain differences related to the number of close friends were correlated with measures of the endogenous opioid involvement of the brain regions.
Social relationships play a double-edged role for mental health. Previous research has primarily focused on the positive aspects of social relationships, while the negative effects have received comparatively less attention (Song et al., 2021). In our study, we identified a robust nonlinear association of close friend quantity with various mental health and cognitive outcomes in the ABCD study at baseline and 2-year follow-up, and an independent social network dataset. This result demonstrates the persistence of the findings. The findings are in line with past studies, which showed that too large a social network size or too frequent social contacts were not positively correlated with well-being in adults (Kushlev et al., 2018; Ren et al., 2022; Stavrova and Ren, 2021) and were even negatively correlated with mental health in adolescents (Falci and McNeely, 2009). One explanation is that an individual’s cognitive capacity and time limit the size of the social network that an individual can effectively maintain (Dunbar, 2018). People devote about 40% of their total social efforts (e.g., time and emotional capital) to just their five most important people (Bzdok and Dunbar, 2020). In a phone-call dataset of almost 35 million users and 6 billion calls, a layered structure was found with the innermost layer having an average of 4.1 people (Mac Carron et al., 2016). There is a trade-off between the quantity and quality of friendships, with an increased number of close friends potentially leading to less intimacy. Meanwhile, spending too much time on social activities may lead to insufficient time for study and thereby to lower academic performance. Second, adolescents are particularly susceptible to peer influence (Berndt, 1979). Researchers have found that the presence of a peer may increase risk-taking behaviors that can be detrimental to mental health (Chein et al., 2011) and reduce cognitive performance (Wolf et al., 2015). Having more close friends may increase the possibility of this kind of influence.
Our study revealed a significant link between the number of close friends and the cortical areas of social brain regions in the largest sample of children to date. Studies suggest that two major systems in the brain related to social behavior include the affective system of the ACC, the anterior insula, and the OFC, and the mentalizing system typically involving the TPJ (Güroğlu, 2022; Schmälzle et al., 2017). The dorsal ACC and anterior insula play an important role in social pain (i.e., painful feelings associated with social disconnection) (Eisenberger, 2012). The OFC receives information about socially relevant stimuli such as face expression and gesture from the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus (Hasselmo et al., 1989a; Pitcher and Ungerleider, 2021), and is involved in social behavior by representing social stimuli in terms of their reward value (Rolls, 2019b; Rolls, 2019a; Rolls et al., 2006). The volume of the OFC is associated with social network size, partly mediated by mentalizing competence (Powell et al., 2012). Previous meta-analysis studies report an overlap in brain activation between all mentalizing tasks in the mPFC and posterior TPJ (Schurz et al., 2014). Notably, in our study, the positive relationship at the brain level only held for the children with no more than approximately five close friends, which is consistent with the behavioral findings. Furthermore, in these children, the areas of social brain regions partly mediated the relationship of the close friend quantity with ADHD symptoms and crystalized intelligence. Research also indicates that the brain regions regulating social behavior undergo structural development during adolescence (Blakemore, 2008; Lamblin et al., 2017; Mills et al., 2014). Animal studies provide evidence for the causal effect of social relationships on brain development. For instance, adolescent rodents with deprivation of peer contacts showed brain level changes including reduced synaptic pruning in the prefrontal cortex (Orben et al., 2020).
Moreover, the brain associative pattern of close friend quantity in children with no more than five close friends was correlated with the density of the μ-opioid receptor, as well as the expression of OPRM1 and OPRK1 genes. It is known that the endogenous opioid system has a vital role in social affiliative processes (Machin and Dunbar, 2011). Positron emission tomography studies in human revealed that μ-opioid receptor regulation in brain regions such as the amygdala, anterior insula, and the ACC may preserve and promote emotional well-being in the social environment (Hsu et al., 2013). Variation in the OPRM1 gene was associated with individual differences in rejection sensitivity, which was mediated by dorsal ACC activity in social rejection (Way et al., 2009). OPRM1 variation was also related to social hedonic capacity (Troisi et al., 2011). Pain tolerance, which is associated with activation of the μ-opioid receptor, was correlated with social network size in humans (Johnson and Dunbar, 2016). Social behaviors like social laughter and social touch increase pleasurable sensations and triggered endogenous opioid release to maintain social relationships (Dunbar, 2010; Manninen et al., 2017; Nummenmaa et al., 2016). Additionally, the opioid system has found to be associated with major psychiatric disorders especially depression (Peciña et al., 2019), which may help explain the association between social relationships and mental health problems.
Several issues should be taken into account when considering our findings. First, as an association study, no causal conclusion should be made in this study. It is unclear whether the number of close friends drives the social brain development or whether children with larger social brains tend to have more close friends. A bidirectional relationship has been reported in the literature (Dunbar and Shultz, 2007). Second, it is worth noting that the measures used in the ABCD study and the social network dataset differed, and the breakpoints identified in each dataset were not equivalent. However, relative to the optimal number of close friends, the primary objective of the current study was to examine the nonlinear relationship between the number of close friend and different behavioral outcomes and brain structure. In this sense, the findings from both datasets were similar, and the social network dataset provided valuable information regarding friendship measures and objective cognitive index that extended the results obtained from the ABCD study. Third, the quality of close friendships was not considered in the ABCD study. However, reciprocal degree is an indirect measure of friendship quality, which was found to be linearly associated with well-being and nonlinearly related to the GPA in the social network dataset. It has been reported that the relationship between having more friends and fewer depressive symptoms in adolescence is mediated by a sense of belonging (Ueno, 2005). Although current findings on the relative importance of friendship quantity and quality are inconsistent (Bruine de Bruin et al., 2020; Platt et al., 2014), it is essential for future studies to incorporate measures of close friendship quality and to test the potential interaction between quantity and quality. Finally, the interpretation of this study should be limited to the particular age range of late childhood and early adolescence, as well as Western culture. Further research is needed to explore whether the nonlinear relationship between the number of close friends and mental health and cognition, and the idea of having around five close friends as a breakpoint, can be generalized to other age ranges and cultures.
In conclusion, this study provides new evidence going beyond previous research that a larger number of close friends up to a moderate level in late childhood is associated with better mental health and higher cognitive functions, and that this can be partly explained by the size of the social brain including the OFC and TPJ, and the endogenous opioid system. This study may have implications for targeted friendship interventions in the transition from late childhood to early adolescence.
Materials and methods
Participants and behavioral measures
The ABCD study
The ABCD study is tracking the brain development and health of a nationally representative sample of children aged 9–11 y from 21 centers throughout the United States (https://abcdstudy.org). Parents’ full written informed consent and all children’s assent were obtained by each center. Research protocols were approved by the institutional review board of the University of California, San Diego (no. 160091), and the institutional review boards of the 21 data collection sites (Auchter et al., 2018). The current study was conducted on the ABCD Data Release 4.0. At baseline, 8835 individuals from 7512 families (6225 [82.9%] with a child, 1252 [16.7%] with two children, 34 [0.5%] with three children, and 1 [0.01%] with four children) had complete behavioral and structural MRI data. To avoid the influence of family relatedness, we randomly picked only one child in each family, finally resulting in 7512 children, of whom 4290 had 2-year follow-up data.
Close friendships are characterized by enjoying spending time together, having fun, and trust. Participants were asked how many close friends that are boys and girls they have, respectively. Mental health problems were rated by the parent using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), which contains 20 empirically based subscales spanning emotional, social and behavioral domains in subjects aged 6–18 (Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001). The CBCL has high inter-interviewer reliability, test–retest reliability, internal consistency, and criterion validity, and therefore is widely utilized by child psychiatrists, developmental psychologists, and other mental health professionals for clinical and research purposes (Achenbach et al., 1987). Raw scores were used in analyses, higher scores indicating more severe problems. Cognitive functions were assessed by the NIH Toolbox (Luciana et al., 2018), which has good reliability and validity in children (Akshoomoff et al., 2013). The toolbox consists of seven different tasks covering episodic memory, executive function, attention, working memory, processing speed, and language abilities, and also provides three composites of crystalized, fluid, and total intelligence (Weintraub et al., 2013). Uncorrected standard scores were used in analyses. All 10 cognitive scores were available at baseline, but only crystalized intelligence was collected 2 y later.
Social network dataset
In order to extend the findings in the ABCD study, we utilized a publicly available dataset of a social network experiment, conducted among students in 56 middle schools in New Jersey, USA (Paluck et al., 2016) (https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/web/civicleads/studies/37070). All parents and students provided informed consent for the survey, and the research protocol was approved by the Princeton University Institutional Review Board. Participants were asked to report which other students (up to 10) in their school they chose to spend time with in the last few weeks, allowing us to generate a directed friendship network. Three kinds of network measures were created for each participant: (1) outdegree is a measure of sociability and refers to the number of friendship nominations that a participant made to other participants, (2) indegree is a measure of popularity and refers to the number of friendship nominations received from others, and (3) reciprocal degree refers to the number of outward nominations that are reciprocated by an inward nomination from the same person and to some extent reflects the quality of friendship. Well-being was assessed by three questions: ‘I feel like I belong at this school,’ ‘I have stayed home from school because of problems with other students,’ and ‘During the past month, I have often been bothered by feeling sad and down’ (Ren et al., 2022). Cognitive function was indirectly measured by the GPA on a 4.0 scale, obtained from school administrative records.
Structural MRI data
In the ABCD study, 3D T1- and T2-weighted structural images were collected using 3T scanners at 21 data collecting sites (Casey et al., 2018). The detailed preprocessing pipeline has been described elsewhere (Gong et al., 2021). In brief, we used FreeSurfer v6.0 to preprocess the minimal preprocessed T1- and T2-weighted images downloaded from the ABCD study, including cortical surface reconstruction, subcortical segmentation, smoothed by a Gaussian kernel (FWHM = 10 mm), and estimation of morphometric measures (i.e., cortical area, thickness, and volume). Then, the cortical surface of each subject was registered to a standard fsaverage space and parcellated into 180 cortical regions per hemisphere as defined in the Human Connectome Project multimodal parcellation (HCP-MMP) atlas (Glasser et al., 2016). Volumetric reconstructions of subcortical structures were also obtained based on the Aseg atlas (Fischl et al., 2002).
Neurochemical data
Fourteen receptors and transporters across eight different neurotransmitter systems (serotonin: 5HT1a, 5HT1b, 5HT2a, 5HT4, and 5HTT; dopamine: D1, D2, and DAT; GABA: GABAa; glutamate: mGluR5; norepinephrine: NAT; cannabinoid: CB1; opioid: MOR; acetylcholine: VAChT) were investigated. Density estimates were derived from average group maps of healthy volunteers scanned in prior PET and SPECT studies (Supplementary file 1). All density maps were downloaded online (https://github.com/juryxy/JuSpace/tree/JuSpace_v1.3/JuSpace_v1.3/PETatlas; Dukart et al., 2021), which had been registered and normalized into the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, and linearly rescaled to 0–100 (Dukart et al., 2021). For comparability, the HCP atlas in fsaverage space was converted to individual surface space (‘mri_surf2surf’) of the MNI brain template ch2 (Rorden and Brett, 2000) which was preprocessed by Freesurfer (‘recon-all’), and then was projected to volume (‘mri_label2vol’). The density maps were parcellated into the 360 cortical regions as the structural MRI data according to the volume-based HCP-MMP atlas. Specifically, for the μ-opioid receptor, occipital cortex served as the reference region (Kantonen et al., 2020) and was therefore excluded in analysis.
Transcriptomic data
Gene expression data was from six neurotypical adult brains in the Allen Human Brain Atlas (Hawrylycz et al., 2012). We focused on the opioid receptor genes (i.e., OPRM1 and OPRK1). The preprocessed transcriptomic data were imported from Arnatkeviciute et al., 2019 (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.6852911), including probe-to-gene re-annotation, intensity-based data filtering, and probe selection using RNA-seq data as a reference. Then, samples were assigned to brain regions according to the volume-based HCP-MMP atlas, and expression values were averaged within each region. Since right hemisphere data were only available for two donors, analyses were conducted on the left hemisphere only, finally resulting in 177 brain regions.
Statistical analysis
Nonlinear association analysis
The nonlinear associations of close friendship quantity with mental health, cognition, and brain structure were investigated. The quantity of close friendship was log-transformed [log10(x + 1)] in analyses as it has a skewed distribution (Hobbs et al., 2016). Two different analytic approaches were used to robustly evaluate the nonlinear relationships. First, we fitted a quadratic regression model (y = bx2+ ax + c) with close friendship quantity as the independent variable. Close friendship quantity was mean-centered to ensure that the linear (a) and quadratic (b) terms were orthogonal. Three statistical parameters were of interest: a total
Several sensitivity analyses were performed. To examine the potential sex influence, we conducted nonlinear association analyses in male and female, respectively. The effect of the sex of close friends was tested by separating close friends into same-sex and opposite-sex ones. To validate the findings from data at baseline and to test the hypothesis of stationarity for cross-lagged panel models (CLPM), we replicated the same analyses using the cross-sectional data collected at 2 y later. For neuroimaging analyses, if significant nonlinear associations were detected, we also conducted linear regression models in two groups split by the average breakpoint, respectively.
Spatial correlation with neurotransmitter density and gene expression
Unthresholded
Cross-lagged panel analysis
Longitudinal relationships of close friendship quantity with ADHD symptoms (i.e., cbcl_scr_syn_attention) and crystalized intelligence were investigated using classic two-wave CLPMs implemented by Mplus 7.0. Firstly, we conducted CLPMs using the absolute value of the difference between close friendship quantity and the breakpoint, and then established CLPMs for participants with the quantity of close friendship ≤breakpoint and >breakpoint at baseline, respectively. We controlled for several stable (i.e., sex, parent education level, ethnicity, and site) and time-variant variables (i.e., age, household income, and puberty) in these models. The CLPMs met important assumptions such as synchronicity and stationarity (Baribeau et al., 2022; Kenny, 1975). The model parameters were estimated by the full information maximum likelihood method (Muthén et al., 1987). The model fit was evaluated by the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), comparative fit index (CFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR), and interpreted using common thresholds of good fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). All CLPMs reported in the current study have a good model fit.
Mediation analysis
We used the baseline data in the ABCD study to test the associations between close friendship quantity, ADHD symptoms, crystalized intelligence, and brain structure. The total area of the significant brain regions was used as the mediator. Variables were normalized and then entered into the model. Sex, age, parent education level, household income, ethnicity, pubertal status, BMI, handedness, head motion, MRI manufacturer, and site were used as covariates of no interest. In addition to the total area, the mediation effect of individual significant regions was also evaluated, the p-values of the mediation effect corrected by FDR correction. Total, direct, and indirect associations were estimated by bootstrapping 10,000 times, and the 95% bias-corrected and accelerated confidence interval (CI) was reported. Analyses were performed using the R mediation package.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2023, Shen, Rolls et al. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Close friendships are important for mental health and cognition in late childhood. However, whether the more close friends the better, and the underlying neurobiological mechanisms are unknown. Using the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Developmental study, we identified nonlinear associations between the number of close friends, mental health, cognition, and brain structure. Although few close friends were associated with poor mental health, low cognitive functions, and small areas of the social brain (e.g., the orbitofrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, the anterior insula, and the temporoparietal junction), increasing the number of close friends beyond a level (around 5) was no longer associated with better mental health and larger cortical areas, and was even related to lower cognition. In children having no more than five close friends, the cortical areas related to the number of close friends revealed correlations with the density of μ-opioid receptors and the expression of OPRM1 and OPRK1 genes, and could partly mediate the association between the number of close friends, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms, and crystalized intelligence. Longitudinal analyses showed that both too few and too many close friends at baseline were associated with more ADHD symptoms and lower crystalized intelligence 2 y later. Additionally, we found that friendship network size was nonlinearly associated with well-being and academic performance in an independent social network dataset of middle-school students. These findings challenge the traditional idea of ‘the more, the better,’ and provide insights into potential brain and molecular mechanisms.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer