Correspondence to Dr Elisa Frisaldi; [email protected]
STRENGTHS AND LIMITATIONS OF THIS STUDY
The umbrella review was reported according to the PRISMA guidelines.
By only analysing placebo and nocebo effects associated with pharmacological interventions, it was possible to circumscribe the area of investigation and reduce the degree of methodological variability between studies.
Systematic reviews were appraised by using the Assessment of Multiple Systematic Reviews 2 tool, which has demonstrated satisfactory reliability and construct validity.
The database search was conducted by one author, whereas two authors independently reviewed the full text of potentially eligible studies against the inclusion and exclusion criteria.
While the umbrella review methodology allows for a comprehensive summary of the findings, it does not permit to overcome the single study limitations, which include publication biases, and the lack of information about unpublished data and the grey literature.
Introduction
Placebo and nocebo effects are the effects of patients’ positive and negative expectations, respectively, about their health status and they can occur during treatment with a placebo or an active agent, either in clinical practice or in clinical trials. While placebo effects result in beneficial outcomes, nocebo effects result in patient harms.1–5
Over the past 30 years, there has been a surge of research on the placebo and nocebo effects in the fields of neuroscience, medicine, psychology and genetics. What has emerged is that there are many placebo and nocebo effects, not just one. They occur through specific mechanisms in many clinical conditions and in the domain of physical and cognitive performance.6 Furthermore, it has been shown that many biological mechanisms triggered by placebos and nocebos resemble those modulated by drugs, suggesting a possible interaction between psychological factors and drug action.6
In 2018, a consensus of experts emphasised the importance of distinguishing placebo effects from placebo responses.7 This need comes from the pharmacological definitions of drug effect and drug response, whereby the former is the specific pharmaco-dynamic effect of a drug, whereas the latter is the global response to drug administration.6 Accordingly, while the placebo and nocebo effects specifically refer to the changes attributable to placebo and nocebo mechanisms, which are the ‘actual’ psychobiological phenomena, the placebo and nocebo responses include all trial outcome changes resulting from the administration of an inactive treatment, including natural history and regression to the mean.7
Besides classical placebo/nocebo effects, today we can also differentiate between placebo/nocebo effects and placebo-related and nocebo-related effects. Although the psychosocial context around the treatment plays a key role in both cases, in the former case, an inert treatment is administered, while in the latter case, it is not.8 These strict definitions remind us that it is not always necessary to administer a placebo to obtain a therapeutic effect, as sometimes doctor’s or healthcare professionals’ words, their attitudes and the therapeutic rituals are enough.8
Another important term used in clinical research is the Hawthorne effect, which refers to changes in baseline conditions that occur in response to a participant’s awareness of being under study. Improvements that occur after recruitment but before the start of treatment could be attributable to several factors, including increased expectations of health benefits, better observation, better compliance and treatment adherence.9
With the exponential increase in the placebo and nocebo literature,10 novel interpretative approaches have arisen by both Ongaro and Kaptchuk11 and Pagnini et al,12 along with the concept of open-label placebos (OLPs), in which patients are informed that they have been prescribed inert treatments.13
It is, therefore, important to incorporate new insights with the existing knowledge. Umbrella reviews provide a unique approach to knowledge integration in circumstances where multiple systematic reviews and meta-analyses have already been published on a specific research topic. In fact, they provide a bird eye’s view of the currently available evidence on broad research topics, explore the consistency of findings, and indicate potential priorities for future research.14 15 This umbrella review aims to present an up-to-date overview of neurobiological basis of both placebo/nocebo effects and placebo-related/nocebo-related effects associated with pharmacological interventions. Our threefold goal was to present findings regarding: (1) what are the conditions, that is, clinical or physiological, in which robust placebo/nocebo effects or placebo-related/nocebo-related effects have been documented to date; (2) what are the contexts/circumstances, that is, clinical or laboratory setting, in which they occur and (3) what do we know about the biological underpinnings of these effects.
Methods
Review selection
The study was reported according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines,16 with methods established prior to conducting the umbrella review. The protocol was registered on the international prospective register for systematic reviews PROSPERO (record no. CRD42023392281, see online supplemental appendix 1A). The objective was to capture systematic reviews, with or without meta-analyses, and narrative reviews aimed at mapping placebo and nocebo effects, or related effects, associated with pharmacological interventions. These studies were then to be informative in terms of biological mechanisms and/or effect sizes.
The electronic bibliographic databases MEDLINE/PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, PsycINFO and Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL) were searched in September 2022, according to the search equation provided in online supplemental appendix 1B. The search was conducted applying the Population, Intervention, Comparison, Outcomes and Study (PICOS) criteria reported in table 1, and no time restrictions were set.
Table 1Description of PICOS components of umbrella review
P | Human population, across different clinical conditions and beyond the healing context. |
I | Placebo and nocebo effects: inert treatments undistinguishable from the matched active pharmacological interventions, administered with suggestions of improvement/worsening or according to conditioning procedures. Placebo-related and nocebo-related effects: suggestions of improvement/worsening without administration of inert treatments or difference between expected (open) and unexpected (hidden) active pharmacological interventions. |
C | No-treatment condition or control group, waiting list, pharmacological placebo not associated with expectation for symptoms improvement/worsening, baseline condition (told placebo, get placebo) according to the balanced-placebo design. |
O | Biological mechanisms of placebo/nocebo effects and of placebo/nocebo-related effects, along with their effect sizes. |
S | Peer-reviewed studies, published in English, informative in terms of biological mechanisms and/or effect sizes. Specifically:
|
AEs, adverse events; OLPs, open-label placebos; PICOS, Population, Intervention, Comparison, Outcomes and Study; RCTs, randomised clinical trials.
Regarding the interventions, we excluded the investigation of placebo/nocebo effects and placebo-related/nocebo-related effects in non-pharmacological interventions (eg, psychotherapy, acupuncture, surgery, neuromodulation, physical therapies, hypnosis, mindfulness training, biofeedback, neurofeedback, music) in order to circumscribe the area of investigation and reduce the degree of methodological variability among studies.
The randomised clinical trials (RCTs) and OLPs clinical trials included in the present umbrella review were required to have a three-arm design (ie, genuine treatment, placebo and no-treatment arms). The latter design allows participants receiving placebo treatment to be compared with those left untreated, and thus to disentangle placebo/nocebo effects from placebo/nocebo responses.2
To provide additional information on the biological mechanisms of placebo/nocebo effects, a first deviation from the original protocol was made for those meta-analyses based on rigorous placebo-controlled RCTs without a no-treatment group, which examined: (1) different routes of placebo administration and reported improvements not attributable to spontaneous remission or regression to the mean; (2) different likelihoods of receiving active treatment or placebo; (3) the type of adverse events (AEs) occurring in both the active and placebo arms. A second deviation was made for original research articles informative about mechanisms and effect sizes that: (1) addressed an underinvestigated topic in the field of placebo research that missed to be included in systematic or narrative reviews and (2) were too recent to be included in systematic or narrative reviews.
Screening process and data extraction
The database search was conducted by one author (EF), who removed duplicates and screened the titles and abstracts. Two authors (EF and FP) independently reviewed the full text of potentially eligible studies (systematic review, narrative reviews and original research articles) against the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Any disagreements were resolved through discussion among all the authors. The references of the surveyed systematic and narrative reviews, and those of books or book chapters on placebo and nocebo mechanisms, were screened for potentially suitable publications. Narrative review articles were included to verify that database search had been exhaustive. If not, they were used as a valuable source of citations. In addition, they provided useful comparative material regarding the arguments brought by the authors on cutting-edge issues related to placebo and nocebo effects.
Very recent informative studies (systematic reviews and original research articles) were found through literature search. The same two authors (EF and FP) progressively entered the data into a spreadsheet preset to record biological mechanisms and effect sizes.
Critical appraisal
EF and FP independently appraised the captured systematic reviews using the Assessment of Multiple Systematic Reviews (AMSTAR) 2 tool, which has demonstrated satisfactory reliability and construct validity.17 In assessing the overall quality of individual studies, more weight was given to the AMSTAR 2 critical domains (ie, 7 out 16 items).17 About the protocol domain, an explicit statement was required that the methods had been established prior to conducting the systematic review and/or that PRISMA guidelines16 or those for meta-analyses and systematic reviews of observational studies18 had been adhered to, and/or that any deviations from protocol had been reported.
In online supplemental appendix 2, the full assessment according to AMSTAR 2 tool was provided for each of the examined systematic reviews, including the seven critical domains marked in yellow and the final positive or negative rating.
Because of the real heterogeneity in the examined conditions and in studies design included in each systematic review, we did not use funnel plots and we choose to summarise the umbrella review results mainly through narrative synthesis and tables.
Statistical analysis
The total number of participants in systematic reviews and original articles was calculated. Since for some systematic reviews only a subset of studies met the inclusion criteria, we took just such studies into account in the overall calculation.
Results of critical appraisal were summarised as: (1) the percentage of all surveyed systematic reviews that received a positive final overall assessment and (2) the percentage of systematic reviews, distinguishing between those with and without meta-analysis, that received a positive final overall assessment.
Regarding the effect sizes expressed as Cohen’s d, Hedges’ g or standardised mean difference, they were summarised as a range with the smallest and largest placebo or nocebo effects, along with their respective 95% CI.
Patient and public involvement
No patient involved.
Results
Umbrella review outcomes
As shown in figure 1, the main search returned a total of 6215 records, which were reduced to 3725 after the exclusion of duplicates. After records were screened for title and abstract, and 3353 records were excluded, a total of 372 full text papers were retrieved, from which 357 met full inclusion criteria. Fifteen additional studies (5 systematic reviews and 10 original research articles) were identified from citations or literature search, for a total of 372 studies included in the umbrella review and 158 312 participants. In particular, the pool of eligible studies includes 41 systematic reviews, 312 narrative reviews and 19 original articles, with all the examined systematic reviews and original articles published in the last 30 years.
Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart. Trial flow of the selection process, showing both the number of events and reasons for the exclusion of most of the 6215 initially selected records. PRISMA, Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses.
Characteristics of the 41 systematic reviews, 30 with and 11 without meta-analyses, are presented in online supplemental appendix 3.19–59 Furthermore, as documented in online supplemental appendix 2, 73% of the eligible systematic reviews were rated as overall high quality, 77% for those with meta-analysis and 64% for those without.
Online supplemental appendix 4 contains the list of both narrative reviews (1, A) and original articles (1, B) included in the umbrella review, together with the list of systematic reviews identified from citation or literature search (1, C). Online supplemental appendix 5 contains the list of studies excluded after being read in their full length, with reason for the exclusion.
General concepts and mechanisms
Although placebos are not expected to work uniformly in all clinical conditions, a series of meta-analyses were conducted between 2001 and 2013 on three-arm RCTs across all clinical conditions (comprising mainly pharmacological interventions).21–25 In particular, Hróbjartsson and Gøtzsche focused on the comparison between placebo and no-treatment groups. They found little evidence in general that placebo interventions had clinically important effects.24 25 Placebos had no significant effects on continuous objective outcomes and subjective or objective binary outcomes, while they had possible small benefits in studies with continuous subjective outcomes, especially in the settings of pain and nausea.22 To facilitate quick comprehension for readers, examples of subjective continuous outcomes were the pain intensity measured on 11-point Numeric Rating Scale or the Rhodes Inventory of Nausea and Vomiting for pain and nausea, respectively. An example of objective continuous outcomes for both settings was the dose of rescue medication. Consistently, the incidence of pain or nausea based on specific cutpoints of the adopted clinical scales represented an example of subjective binary outcomes, while the administration or not of rescue medication represented an example of objective binary outcomes. Results obtained from Hróbjartsson and Gøtzsche’s meta-analyses were inevitably constrained by the studies selected and the sensitivity of their measures. For example, binary outcomes have less power to detect effects generally than do continuous outcomes. Moreover, the authors used very broad inclusion criteria (ie, RCTs with a placebo group and a no-treatment group, employing both parallel or crossover designs), and the surveyed studies used 40 different outcome measures, some more reliable than others and some more likely to exhibit a response to placebo than others.60
Since the assessment of the clinical utility of placebos requires a comparison with an active treatment, in 2013 Howick et al21 extracted data about treatment effects from the last meta-analysis conducted by Hróbjartsson and Gøtzsche.22 They showed that placebos often had a great benefit compared with no-treatment as active treatments had over placebos.21 In trials with binary outcomes, active treatment effects were usually greater than placebo effects (n=37, ratio of risk ratios=0.72 (95% CI 0.61 to 0.86) p<0.001). In trials with continuous outcomes (n=115), placebo effects were found to be higher than active treatment effects when the analysis was restricted to studies with a low risk of bias (n=8, mean difference=1.59 (95% CI 0.40 to 2.77) p=0.009).21
Starting from the same pool of studies used by Hróbjartsson and Gøtzsche in 2004,24 and selecting studies that used peripherally measured parameters as outcomes, a subsequent meta-analysis showed that placebo interventions can improve physical disease processes of peripheral organs (n=20, Hedges’ pooled effect size=0.22 (95% CI 0.07 to 0.36) p=0.003) more easily and effectively than biochemical processes (n=6, g=−0.17 (95% CI −0.31 to –0.02) p=0.02).23
Regarding nocebo effects, manipulation of expectation, conditioning or both has been shown to successfully evoke nocebo effects in domains such as those of pain sensation, skin dryness, nausea and cognitive performance. For example, regarding the manipulation of expectation in pain, it has been shown that pain intensity increases in healthy participants who were informed that during a painful stimulation they would have receive a cream with a hyperalgesic effect. With regard to Pavlovian conditioning of nausea in healthy volunteers (rotation paired with cinnamon breath strips), it has been shown to significantly induce both a decrease in reaction time (stopping the rotation in rotation chair) and an increase in symptom reporting. Conversely, nocebo effects have not been shown to occur in the domains of satiety and dizziness.26
Despite their proven effectiveness in many conditions, prescribing placebos is considered unethical because it entails deception.61 Yet, this idea has been challenged recently by the use of OLPs.3 62 A positive effect for non-deceptive placebos compared with no-treatment (standardised mean difference 0.88 (95% CI 0.62 to 1.14) p<0.001) was recently reported in meta-analysis in which the clinical conditions analysed were depression, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), irritable bowel syndrome (IBS), allergic rhinitis.20
The effect size of choice on the placebo effect has also recently been examined in a pool of studies that compared placebo treatment with any form of choice on its administration against placebo treatment without choice.19 The 15 eligible studies, which assessed a range of conditions including pain, discomfort, sleep difficulty and anxiety, showed that choice did significantly enhance the placebo effect, even if with a small effect size (Hedges’ g=0.298). Also, the magnitude of the placebo effect without choice (ie, placebo without choice vs no-treatment) was identified as the only reliable moderator of the choice effect, according to the role that larger placebo effect without choice produced smaller choice effects (ie, placebo with choice vs placebo without choice). Therefore, treatment choice can effectively facilitate the placebo effect, but this effect appears more pronounced in contexts where the placebo effect without choice is not prominent.19
From a psychobiological perspective, most knowledge about the mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects comes from the field of pain. It shows that expectation and learning are the main mediators. Expectation is a conscious event, whereby the subject expects a future outcome. The link between expectation and clinical outcomes is twofold. First, positive expectations may reduce anxiety. Second, expectation of a positive event (ie, a therapeutic benefit), may activate reward mechanisms, in which reward is the therapeutic benefit itself. Learning mechanisms, ranging from classical or behavioural conditioning to social learning, are crucial because prior experience towards effective treatments leads to substantial placebo effects. It is important to emphasise that expectation and learning are not mutually exclusive, since learning can lead to the reinforcement of expectations or can even create de novo expectations.4 6 8
A central role in placebo effects seems also to be played by the interactions between associative learning systems and appraisals, which are flexible cognitive evaluations of the personal meaning of events and situations. While learning can occur in many neural circuits, appraisal appears to be supported by a specialised system—a collection of midline cortical and temporoparietal regions associated with the so-called ‘default mode network’. This network, involved in emotion generation, social and self-referential cognition, and value-based learning and decision-making, allows individuals to simulate potential outcomes and to develop expectations about future events.63
In terms of predictive factors, it should be emphasised that many reasons exist why some people respond to placebos (placebo responders) while others do not (placebo non-responders). Learning is certainly an important factor, as people who have had prior positive therapeutic experiences show larger placebo effects than those who have not had any.1–3 6 Other important determinants are: personality traits, genetic variants, gender, individual differences in the efficiency of the neural mechanisms of reward, whereby the ventral striatum—that is, the nucleus accumbens—is involved in motivation and reward anticipation; prefrontal functioning and connectivity.4 64 65 Regarding the latter factor, its importance in the placebo component of the analgesic treatments was demonstrated in studies on Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients, while the individual placebo analgesic effect was found to be correlated with the white matter integrity in the descending pain control system in normal subjects. Therefore, the potential disruption of placebo mechanisms should be considered in all those conditions where the prefrontal regions are involved, as occurs in vascular and frontotemporal dementia as well as in any lesion of the prefrontal cortex.4 Regarding sex differences, males have been found to respond more strongly to placebo treatments, while females to nocebo treatments.27 Furthermore, males respond with larger placebo effects induced by verbal information, whereas females respond with larger nocebo effects induced by conditioning procedures. The observed sex differences in placebo responding are probably due to larger stress reduction in males compared with females. Furthermore, endogenous opioid transmission has been reported to be more effective in males compared with females and may, therefore, explain the observed sex differences in placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia.27
Mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects across conditions
The retrieved psychobiological mechanisms of placebo/nocebo effects and placebo-related/nocebo-related effects associated with pharmacological interventions, together with their effect sizes, are reported in online supplemental appendix 6. In summary, meaningful results have been found for the following clinical conditions: pain,2 4 6 8 20 29–40 62 66–75 non-noxious somatic sensation,76 Parkinson’s disease (PD),2 6 41 77–79 migraine,42–44 sleep,45 80 intellectual disability (ID),46 depression,2 6 20 47 48 62 69 74 81–83 anxiety,2 6 8 74 dementia,2 4 49 84 addiction,2 4 50 51 63 79 85 86 gynaecological disorders,87 88 ADHD,20 89 immune and endocrine systems,2 4 20 79 90–92 cardiovascular system,2 52 79 93 94 respiratory system,2 79 95–97 gastrointestinal disorders,6 20 53 62 74 98–100 skin diseases,26 54 62 87 96 101–103 influenza and related vaccines,55 104 oncology20 26 53 62 96 and obesity.9 105 106 Beyond the healing context, meaningful results have also been found for physical2 56–59 107–109 and cognitive performance.26 108 110
Regarding placebo and nocebo effect sizes, they were found to vary from small to large depending on the condition under investigation: from 0.08 to 2.01 (95% CI 0.37 to 0.89) in the case of placebo effects, and from 0.32 to 0.90 (95% CI 0.24 to 1.00) in the case of nocebo effects. Consistently, table 2 lists the clinical and non-clinical conditions according to the effect sizes of the placebo/nocebo effects, and for each of them indicates the outcome measures adopted (subjective and/or objective).
Table 2Magnitude of placebo and nocebo effects across conditions
Magnitude of the effect size | Type of effect | Condition | Values | Outcome measures |
Large | Placebo | Nociceptive, idiopathic and neuropathic pain in placebo mechanism studies | Nociceptive pain Cohen’s d=1.0166 Idiopathic pain Cohen’s d=1.6366 Neuropathic pain Cohen’s d=2.0166 | Validated clinical scales of pain relief, filled in by patients (subjective self-reported measures) |
Placebo | Chronic migraine prevention trials: strictly dependent by route of placebo administration (application to the head being superior to the other routes) | Seventy-five percent of the therapeutic gain42 | Reduction in the no of days with migraine in the month (subjective self-reported measure) | |
Placebo | Acute sadness in female depressed patients | Hedge’s g=0.9281 | Validated clinical scale for major depression, filled in by patients (subjective measure) | |
Placebo | Respiratory system: cough | 50% reduction in cough frequency95 | Reduction in cough frequency, recorded by means of a microphone (objective measure) | |
Placebo | Sport performance assuming purported anabolic steroids or an erythropoietin like substance | Purported anabolic steroids Cohen’s d=1.4458 Erythropoietin like substance Cohen’s d=0.8158 | Direct measure of performance, for example, power output, speed or time to completion (objective measures) | |
Moderate to large | Nocebo | Nociceptive and idiopathic pain, where nocebo effects were induced by verbal suggestions | Cohen’s d around 0.66 to 0.9035 | Validated clinical scales of pain relief, filled in by patients (subjective self-reported measures) |
Moderate | Placebo | Addiction: alcohol-challenge studies whereby the experimental setting consists of a natural environment (both less tension and experimental reactivity than in experimental lab situations) | Cohen’s d=0.65851 | Self-reported measures (subjective measures); physiological or behavioural measures (objective measures) |
Placebo related | Intellectual disability: effect associated to the certainty of receiving the active treatment | Hedges’ g=0.6546 | Validated clinical scales filled in by patients (subjective measures) | |
Nocebo | Motor performance | Cohen’s d=0.6056 | Rotor task performance, sprint time, alertness reaction time, biceps curl total repetitions (objective measures) | |
Small to moderate | Placebo | Sleep | Sleep onset latency Hedges’ g=0.27245 Total sleep time Hedges’ g=0.32245 Perceived global sleep quality Hedges’ g=0.5845 | Global sleep quality, total sleep time, sleep onset latency (patients’ subjective self-reported measures) |
Placebo | Addition: alcohol-challenge studies conducted according to the balanced-placebo design | Behavioural Cohen’s d=0.22151 Self-report Cohen’s d=0.34851 Physiological Cohen’s d=0.39451 | Self-report variables (subjective); behavioural and physiological variables (objective) | |
Placebo | Sport performance assuming placebo described as amino acids or caffeine | Amino acids Cohen’s d=0.3658 Caffeine Cohen’s d=0.4058 | Direct measure of performance, for example, power output, speed or time to completion (objective measures) | |
Placebo | Acute migraine treatment (small for oral placebo administration, moderate for subcutaneous placebo administration) | Oral placebo administration, 25.7% of patients44 Subcutaneous placebo administration, 32.4% of patients44 | Headache relief rate (patients’ subjective self-reported measure) | |
Nocebo | Sport performance assuming a fictitious sport supplement thought to be detrimental to performance | Cohen’s d=0.3258 | Sprint time (objective measure) | |
Small | Placebo | Pain | Hedges’ g=0.0830 | Activation of neurologic pain signature (objective measure) |
Placebo | Depression | Standardised Mean Difference 0.2247 | Validated clinical scale for major depression, filled in by patients (subjective measure); no of relapses (objective measure) | |
Placebo | Sport performance assuming a fictitious sport supplement | Cohen’s d=0.2158 | Direct measure of performance, for example, power output, speed or time to completion (objective measures) | |
Placebo | Sport performance assuming the active nutritional supplements caffeine and extracellular buffers | Hedges’ g=0.0957 | Total work done, means: power output, mean velocity, mean height and time to completion (ie, performance test)/time to exhaustion (ie, capacity test). |
Interpreting the evidence
Some results about the magnitude or mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects require interpretation and an in-depth analysis. Different settings and mechanisms present peculiarities that should be individually considered.
In the field of pain, the difference in magnitude of placebo analgesia observed between those studies aimed at investigating placebo mechanism compared with those using placebos as control condition appears to result from different suggestions given for pain relief.37 Moreover, magnitudes of placebo and nocebo effects in both nociceptive and idiopathic pain conditions appear to be roughly similar, supporting the hypothesis that similar mechanisms are involved in the opposite effects.35 Regarding the difference in placebo analgesic effects according to the population type, patients show to benefit from placebo treatment to a greater extent than healthy participants do.31 Consistently, the analysis of neurotransmitter systems involved in placebo/nocebo effects in healthy participants and chronic pain patients suggests that knowledges obtained in the former population may not necessarily be transferred to the latter.28
Major advances in the neuroanatomical viewpoint of placebo analgesia have also been made in the last decade. Placebos administered along with positive verbal suggestions activate and deactivate different brain regions. Many of these regions show anticipatory increases prior to pain, predicting the strength of an individual’s placebo analgesic effect, and suggesting that their role in placebo analgesia may not be pain-specific but rather may be tied to broader appraisal and expectation processes.36 70 Consistently, very small effects are elicited by placebo on the neurological pain signature, which is a brain-based pattern that can reliably distinguish between responses to painful and non-painful stimuli, and is sensitive and specific to pain.30 This finding suggests that placebos might modulate non-specific affective and cognitive processes rather than affecting nociception.30 70
The neuroanatomy of nocebo hyperalgesia has been characterised as well.33 Cortical systems implicated in the experience of pain have been shown to be involved in pain anticipation. Their involvement suggests that these activations have a preparatory function, whereby potentially threatening stimuli receive more attention and are reliably detected.33 75
In antimigraine clinical trials, adequate control groups are lacking. Nevertheless, the placebo-controlled RCTs in both chronic migraine prevention and acute migraine treatment trials, which examined the efficacy of different routes of drug and placebo administration, proved to be informative about placebo effects.42 44 Indeed, as Swerts et al) state,42 although their meta-analysis evaluated the placebo response deriving from different routes of administration, the methodology of the eligible trials was kept the same (all of which were double-blinded RCTs, with the natural history being kept constant). Therefore, the differences in the placebo response emerged from statistical analysis actually reflect a difference in the placebo effect, and provides a starting point for the investigation of the underlying mechanisms.42
The neuroanatomy of placebo effects in depression has also begun to be disclosed. It involves the activity in the ventral striatum, rostral anterior cingulate cortex and other default mode network regions, orbitofrontal cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, with overlap with some of the areas involved in placebo analgesia.48
Dementia deserves special attention because its pathophysiology is complex and varies across the different types of dementia, of which AD is by far the most common. AD patients in moderate and later stages of the disease have shown to not benefit from certainty of receiving genuine treatment (100% certainty) compared with the uncertainty of receiving treatment or placebo (50% certainty).49
This could be due to the nature/progression of the disease, but it could also be related to an order effect in the practice of running AD trials, where RCTs are conducted prior to open-label trials. These findings have implications for the understanding of non-specific treatment effects in AD patients as well as for the design of clinical trials that test pharmacological treatments in AD.49
Regarding respiratory system, expectation-induced dyspnoea in the laboratory setting by using classical conditioning shows important therapeutic perspective.79 97 Since expectation of dyspnoea can be manipulated by an external intervention, it becomes of major importance not only to interfere with acute brain mechanisms, but also to reverse chronic conditioning to free the patient’s mind from negative respiratory anticipation.97
In oncology, the experimental tradition in placebo and nocebo effects originated in the study of anticipatory nausea in chemotherapy. The latter refers to the phenomenon whereby patients develop such strong learning between their chemotherapy context and the nausea that they begin to feel nauseous purely when they re-enter this context.53 96 There is promising preliminary evidence that latent inhibition and overshadowing procedures can be used to prevent or diminish anticipatory nausea.53 Also, these procedures do not involve deception, so if confirmed as effective in large-scale studies they could be applied and ethically translated into practice.53
Placebo and nocebo effects in sport performance involve a variety of factors, such as fatigue endurance, pain tolerance, motivation and muscle strength. Motor performance is instead a broader term, incorporating not only the execution of sport specific movements, but also including skills that are essential to normal everyday functioning, such as simple reaction time or vigilance.56 According to the model of central command, motor performance is not limited by a failure of homeostasis in key organs, but rather it is regulated at early stages in order to ensure that exercise is completed before harm develops.107 Consistently, placebos and nocebos might act in motor performance on the balance between an inhibitory and a facilitatory system, by altering the individual evaluation of the ongoing muscles performance. On one hand, placebos could act to increase fatigue threshold with the consequent increase of motor output and decrease of perceived fatigue; on the other hand, nocebos could act to decrease fatigue threshold.107 108
Discussion
This umbrella review attested the significant progress made in the past 30 years in the investigation of placebo/nocebo effects and placebo/nocebo-related effects, and it offered an up-to-date overview on the topic. The overall high quality of the examined systematic reviews supported the reliability of both the obtained qualitative and quantitative results. Furthermore, even if overlapping meta-analyses on the same topic were found, especially in pain, each of them made specific contributions to the whole picture.
Many biological mechanisms were rigorously characterised in both clinical and non-clinical contexts, as extensively described in online supplemental appendix 6. Moreover, the magnitude of placebo effects, ranging from small to large, was calculated for nociceptive, idiopathic and neuropathic pain,30 66 migraine,42 44 sleep,45 depression,47 81 addiction,51 respiratory system95 and physical performance.57–59 Moderate placebo-related effect was calculated for ID.46 The magnitude of nocebo effects, ranging from small to moderate and moderate to large, was calculated for nociceptive and idiopathic pain35 and for physical performance.56 58
Cough and asthma showed to undergo powerful placebo effects, measured as cough frequency and airway reactivity, respectively. However, their magnitudes have not yet been quantified in pools of eligible studies.95 96
Significant responses to OLPs administration were documented for: pain (low back pain and ischaemic arm pain),20 62 72 depression,20 62 menopausal hot flushes,87 ADHD,20 89 allergic rhinitis,20 IBS,20 62 psoriasis62 and cancer-related fatigue.20 62 Also, the Hawthorne effect was documented in both dementia84 and obesity.9
Indications regarding which outcome measures were assessed for each condition were also provided, including: validated clinical scales of pain relief in the case of pain; reduction in the number of migraine days per month in the case of chronic migraine or headache relief rate in the case of acute migraine treatment; global sleep quality, total sleep time, sleep-onset latency in the case of sleep.
With the intention to provide a list of strategies for better future research in clinical practice and clinical trials, table 3 was prepared from our results and from what has been proposed in previous literature.3 9 79 111 112 Regarding clinical practice, whereby placebo, nocebo and Hawthorne effects are powerful, pervasive and common, and produce uncertainty in the measurement of therapeutic outcomes,3 9 the outlined strategies should be considered a priority, also given their numerous benefits at no cost.113 Our considerations for better future trial design were outlined as well, which do not include the current strategy to artificially reduce placebo responses. Indeed, the double-blind placebo run-in (or lead-in) period for identifying placebo responders and excluding them from further random assignment9 should be interpreted with caution, as should the elimination of placebo responders based on genetic screening.9 In fact, these procedures create an ideal and strictly controlled conditions (efficacy studies), which do not represent the real world (effectiveness studies). Furthermore, the degree of responsiveness to placebo could vary over time within the same individual, while random assignment of non-responders to both the placebo and active treatment arms could lead to low placebo effects in both groups, with no real benefit.
Table 3Strategies for better future research in clinical practice and clinical trials
Clinical practice | Clinical trials |
Communication style and verbal information | |
Enhance the physician-patient relationship by adopting an authentic and empathetic communication style. Provide adequate information regarding disease, diagnoses and treatments. Present patients with realistic possible effects of the intervention, balancing the presentation of desired treatment effects, adverse effects, and frame information about side effects. Provide patients with an introduction to the mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects as a basis for promoting healing processes. Ask patients to summarise the treatment information they were provided with, to prevent negative biases and misunderstandings. Favour positive associations and minimise negative associations between the therapeutic intervention and contextual factors. Refer to sources that provide evidence-based information about the ongoing treatment, instead of unproven and/or anxiogenic comments. Use communication strategies to reduce the likelihood of non-adherence to the treatment regimen or discontinuation of the drug. Teach and train strategies to cope with adverse effects. | Standardise the language used to present the benefit–risk profile of the intervention under investigation. Standardise framing strategies used to present information about side effects. Standardise questions and use structured checklists to collect data on side effects. Standardise the duration and no of therapeutic visits across study sites. |
Expectations | |
Encourage patients to recount their previous positive or negative experiences with interventions. Regularly assess and address patients’ treatment expectations. Optimise treatment expectations and adverse effects expectations, but avoid violations of expectations. Regularly assess and address possible factors that may influence patients’ treatment expectations, especially anxiety. Provide ‘open-medication’ (ie, administer the pharmacological agent in full view of the patient) together with positive instructions about its potential benefits. | Ask patients at baseline how much improvement they would expect from the active treatment. All trials should assess patients’ perceived assignment by asking participants which group they believe they belong to. Adverse events in placebo arms, namely nocebo effects, might depend on the adverse events of the active medication against which the placebo is compared; such comparisons could provide important information on the role of patients’ expectations. |
Conditioning | |
Provide multisensory treatment cues (eg, sight, smell and taste stimulations) associated with the active medication to promote conditioning. Use placebo-controlled drug tapering, if applicable; it consists of starting treatment with repeated full doses to establish associative learning processes and replacing drugs with placebo at a later time. When pretreatments are allowed or required, they should be designed to be highly effective and the patient should receive feedback on their positive effects. | Different placebos use different mechanisms, which in turn might lead to different outcomes; thus, the careful selection of placebos (pills, injections, delivery systems, etc) and outcome measures is crucial. Longer and larger trials can produce large placebo responses; thus, shorter and smaller trials are sometimes preferable to longer, larger, multicentre trials. |
Social learning | |
Promote social learning of the positive effects of drugs: patients starting a new treatment could talk to other patients who have received the same treatment successfully or observe their response through video clips. | Social interactions among trial participants should be avoided to prevent possible effects on baseline clinical and biological variables. |
Hawthorne effect | |
The effect of being under study should be considered and investigated in detail. | The effect of being under study should be considered in any clinical trial and investigated in detail. |
An additional strength of our study is that it allowed us to identify which research areas presented findings that are ready to be implemented in clinical practice. They are: nociceptive, idiopathic and neuropathic pain, non-noxious somatic sensation (with implications for conditions characterised by a pathological lack of sensation, eg, stroke), PD, chronic migraine, ID, depression, AD, addiction, ADHD disorder, allergic diseases, type 2 diabetes, cough, dyspnoea, IBS, itch, COVID-19 vaccination and management of influenza or influenza-like symptoms, physical performance, the latter with important implications for all diseases which have fatigue and/or dyspnoea as cardinal symptoms.
Many other clinical conditions exist that may contribute to the discovery of new placebo and nocebo effects in the near future. These are mainly chronic diseases in which placebos, administered in the context of classic RCTs, have been shown to induce significant improvements. These responses, however, would require the inclusion of an untreated control group in the trial to be accounted for as placebo/nocebo effects. Some of these clinical conditions include myasthenia gravis (MG)114 and painful diabetic neuropathy (PDN).115 Placebo and drug responses in MG trials, as assessed by means of the Quantitative Myasthenia Gravis scores assigned by neurologists, have been shown to be small and moderate, respectively.114 In PDN trials, the placebo response, as assessed by patients-perceived pain relief, showed moderate effect size (with the year of study initiation as the only significant moderator), whereas the nocebo response substantially accounted for patients’ reported AEs.115
Despite the exponential growth of research into placebo and nocebo effects, these phenomena remain complex and far from being fully understood. First of all, meta-analyses rigorously quantifying the magnitude of placebo and nocebo effects are lacking for several of the clinical conditions examined: PD, anxiety, immune, endocrine and cardiovascular systems, gastrointestinal disorders, and oncology. Furthermore, while some studies provided answers to certain questions, they also raised new ones, thus identifying research gaps. For example, the magnitude of placebo and nocebo effects can be modulated through conditioning and instructional strategies? What kind of interaction exists between placebo and nocebo effects, that is, is it possible for placebos to act, in part or entirely, on a pre-existing nocebo effect under certain conditions? How do placebo and nocebo effects modulate subjective/patient-reported and objective (physiological/behavioural) outcomes in different clinical conditions? In addition, further investigations are needed both to study the factors predicting the magnitude of placebo and nocebo responses, for example, by screening for genetic polymorphisms among individuals, and to pursue the mapping of the conditions under which OLPs work, accompanied by the investigation of the underlying mechanisms.
Focusing instead on the therapist-patient encounter, the biggest challenges for future research include: (1) the identification of those elements, psychological and social, that may lead to a good relationship; (2) in-depth experiments with brain imaging techniques to understand complex functions such as hope, trust, empathy, compassion and admiration; (3) the development of questionnaires and psychometric measurements able to identify patient’s needs.
This study should be interpreted in the context of its limitations. In fact, while the umbrella review methodology allows for a comprehensive summary of the findings, it does not permit to overcome the single study limitations, which include publication biases, and the lack of information about unpublished data and the grey literature. In addition, as the value of a second reviewer throughout the entire screening process of systematic reviews has been documented,116 the use of a single reviewer in the database search represent a further potential limitation of this study.
In conclusion, this umbrella review was intended to raise awareness among clinicians and researchers of the application of clear evidence on the benefits and harms of placebo and nocebo effects. Depending on the contexts, specific tools were provided to best harness, develop, and implement strategies that enhance placebo effects and prevent or minimise potential nocebo effects associated with pharmacological interventions. In addition, this study identified which findings are ready to be implemented in clinical practice and highlighted research gaps that need to be addressed in the near future.
Data availability statement
Extracted data are available on reasonable request to the corresponding author.
Ethics statements
Patient consent for publication
Not applicable.
Contributors EF, FP, FB and AS are guarantors and responsible for the study design and protocol development. EF and FP analysed and interpreted the data with the support of FB and AS. All authors drafted the paper and read, commented on, and approved the final draft. The corresponding author attests that all listed authors meet authorship criteria and that no others meeting the criteria have been omitted.
Funding This project was funded by Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Milan, Italy, with the 'Finanziamento Ponte 2022' grant.
Competing interests None declared.
Patient and public involvement Patients and/or the public were not involved in the design, or conduct, or reporting, or dissemination plans of this research.
Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Supplemental material This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.
1 Benedetti F. Placebo effects: from the neurobiological paradigm to translational implications. Neuron 2014; 84: 623–37. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.023
2 Finniss DG, Kaptchuk TJ, Miller F, et al. Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects. Lancet 2010; 375: 686–95. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61706-2
3 Ropper AH, Colloca L, Barsky AJ. Placebo and nocebo effects. N Engl J Med 2020; 382: 554–61. doi:10.1056/NEJMra1907805
4 Benedetti F. Placebo and the new physiology of the doctor-patient relationship. Physiol Rev 2013; 93: 1207–46. doi:10.1152/physrev.00043.2012
5 Benedetti F, Frisaldi E, Piedimonte A. The need to investigate Nocebo effects in more detail. World Psychiatry 2019; 18: 227–8. doi:10.1002/wps.20627
6 Benedetti F, Frisaldi E, Shaibani A. Thirty years of neuroscientific investigation of placebo and nocebo: the interesting, the good, and the bad. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 2022; 62: 323–40. doi:10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-052120-104536
7 Evers AWM, Colloca L, Blease C, et al. Implications of placebo and nocebo effects for clinical practice: expert consensus. Psychother Psychosom 2018; 87: 204–10. doi:10.1159/000490354
8 Benedetti F. Mechanisms of placebo and placebo-related effects across diseases and treatments. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 2008; 48: 33–60. doi:10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.48.113006.094711
9 Benedetti F, Carlino E, Piedimonte A. Increasing uncertainty in CNS clinical trials: the role of placebo, nocebo, and hawthorne effects. Lancet Neurol 2016; 15: 736–47. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(16)00066-1
10 Weimer K, Buschhart C, Broelz EK, et al. Bibliometric properties of placebo literature from the JIPS database: a descriptive study. Front Psychiatry 2022; 13: 853953. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2022.853953
11 Ongaro G, Kaptchuk TJ. Symptom perception, placebo effects, and the Bayesian brain. Pain 2019; 160: 1–4. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001367
12 Pagnini F, Barbiani D, Cavalera C, et al. Placebo and nocebo effects as bayesian-brain phenomena: the overlooked role of likelihood and attention. Perspect Psychol Sci 2023; 18: 1217–29. doi:10.1177/17456916221141383
13 Kaptchuk TJ, Miller FG. Open label placebo: can honestly prescribed placebos evoke meaningful therapeutic benefits? BMJ 2018; 363: k3889. doi:10.1136/bmj.k3889
14 Belbasis L, Bellou V, Ioannidis JPA. Conducting umbrella reviews. BMJ Med 2022; 1: e000071. doi:10.1136/bmjmed-2021-000071
15 Fusar-Poli P, Radua J. Ten simple rules for conducting umbrella reviews. Evid Based Ment Health 2018; 21: 95–100. doi:10.1136/ebmental-2018-300014
16 Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021; 372: n71. doi:10.1136/bmj.n71
17 Shea BJ, Reeves BC, Wells G, et al. AMSTAR 2: a critical appraisal tool for systematic reviews that include randomised or non-randomised studies of healthcare interventions, or both. BMJ 2017: j4008. doi:10.1136/bmj.j4008
18 Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, et al. Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for reporting. In: Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA 283. 2000: 2008–12. doi:10.1001/jama.283.15.2008
19 Tang B, Barnes K, Geers A, et al. Choice and the placebo effect: a meta-analysis. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 2022; 56: 977–88. doi:10.1093/abm/kaab111
20 Charlesworth JEG, Petkovic G, Kelley JM, et al. Effects of placebos without deception compared with no treatment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Evid Based Med 2017; 10: 97–107. doi:10.1111/jebm.12251
21 Howick J, Friedemann C, Tsakok M, et al. Are treatments more effective than placebos? a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2013; 8: e62599. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0062599
22 Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC. Placebo interventions for all clinical conditions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2010; 2010: CD003974. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003974.pub3
23 Meissner K, Distel H, Mitzdorf U. Evidence for placebo effects on physical but not on biochemical outcome parameters: a review of clinical trials. BMC Med 2007; 5: 3. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-5-3
24 Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC. Is the placebo powerless? update of a systematic review with 52 new randomized trials comparing placebo with no treatment. J Intern Med 2004; 256: 91–100. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01355.x
25 Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC. Is the placebo powerless? an analysis of clinical trials comparing placebo with no treatment. N Engl J Med 2001; 344: 1594–602. doi:10.1056/NEJM200105243442106
26 Bagarić B, Jokić-Begić N, Sangster Jokić C. The nocebo effect: a review of contemporary experimental research. Int J Behav Med 2022; 29: 255–65. doi:10.1007/s12529-021-10016-y
27 Vambheim SM, Flaten MA. A systematic review of sex differences in the placebo and the nocebo effect. J Pain Res 2017; 10: 1831–9. doi:10.2147/JPR.S134745
28 Skyt I, Lunde SJ, Baastrup C, et al. Neurotransmitter systems involved in placebo and nocebo effects in healthy participants and patients with chronic pain: a systematic review. Pain 2020; 161: 11–23. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001682
29 Daniali H, Flaten MA. A qualitative systematic review of effects of provider characteristics and nonverbal behavior on pain, and placebo and nocebo effects. Front Psychiatry 2019; 10: 242. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00242
30 Zunhammer M, Bingel U, Wager TD, et al. Placebo effects on the neurologic pain signature: a meta-analysis of individual participant functional magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA Neurol 2018; 75: 1321–30. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.2017
31 Forsberg JT, Martinussen M, Flaten MA. The placebo analgesic effect in healthy individuals and patients: a meta-analysis. Psychosom Med 2017; 79: 388–94. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000432
32 Peerdeman KJ, van Laarhoven AIM, Keij SM, et al. Relieving patients’ pain with expectation interventions: a meta-analysis. Pain 2016; 157: 1179–91. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000540
33 Palermo S, Benedetti F, Costa T, et al. Pain anticipation: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain imaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 2015; 36: 1648–61. doi:10.1002/hbm.22727
34 Atlas LY, Wager TD. A meta-analysis of brain mechanisms of placebo analgesia: consistent findings and unanswered questions. Handb Exp Pharmacol 2014; 225: 37–69. doi:10.1007/978-3-662-44519-8_3
35 Petersen GL, Finnerup NB, Colloca L, et al. The magnitude of nocebo effects in pain: a meta-analysis. Pain 2014; 155: 1426–34. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2014.04.016
36 Amanzio M, Benedetti F, Porro CA, et al. Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain correlates of placebo analgesia in human experimental pain. Hum Brain Mapp 2013; 34: 738–52. doi:10.1002/hbm.21471
37 Vase L, Petersen GL, Riley JL, et al. Factors contributing to large analgesic effects in placebo mechanism studies conducted between 2002 and 2007. Pain 2009; 145: 36–44. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.04.008
38 Sauro MD, Greenberg RP. Endogenous opiates and the placebo effect: a meta-analytic review. J Psychosom Res 2005; 58: 115–20. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.07.001
39 Vase L, Riley JL, Price DD. A comparison of placebo effects in clinical analgesic trials versus studies of placebo analgesia. Pain 2002; 99: 443–52. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00205-1
40 Ter Riet G, de Craen AJM, de Boer A, et al. Is placebo analgesia mediated by endogenous opioids? a systematic review. Pain 1998; 76: 273–5. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00057-8
41 Quattrone A, Barbagallo G, Cerasa A, et al. Neurobiology of placebo effect in Parkinson’s disease: what we have learned and where we are going. Mov Disord 2018; 33: 1213–27. doi:10.1002/mds.27438
42 Swerts DB, Benedetti F, Peres MFP. Different routes of administration in chronic migraine prevention lead to different placebo responses: a meta-analysis. Pain 2022; 163: 415–24. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002365
43 Amanzio M, Corazzini LL, Vase L, et al. A systematic review of adverse events in placebo groups of anti-migraine clinical trials. Pain 2009; 146: 261–9. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.07.010
44 de Craen AJ, Tijssen JG, de Gans J, et al. Placebo effect in the acute treatment of migraine: subcutaneous placebos are better than oral placebos. J Neurol 2000; 247: 183–8. doi:10.1007/s004150050560
45 Yeung V, Sharpe L, Glozier N, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of placebo versus no treatment for insomnia symptoms. Sleep Med Rev 2018; 38: 17–27. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2017.03.006
46 Jensen KB, Kirsch I, Pontén M, et al. Certainty of genuine treatment increases drug responses among intellectually disabled patients. Neurology 2017; 88: 1912–8. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000003934
47 Fernández-López R, Riquelme-Gallego B, Bueno-Cavanillas A, et al. Influence of placebo effect in mental disorders research: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Clin Invest 2022; 52: e13762. doi:10.1111/eci.13762
48 Huneke NTM, Aslan IH, Fagan H, et al. Functional neuroimaging correlates of placebo response in patients with depressive or anxiety disorders: a systematic review. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2022; 25: 433–47. doi:10.1093/ijnp/pyac009
49 Matthiesen ST, Rosenkjær S, Pontén M, et al. Does certainty of genuine treatment increase the drug response in alzheimer’s disease patients: a meta-analysis and critical discussion. JAD 2021; 84: 1821–32. doi:10.3233/JAD-210108
50 Galindo MN, Navarro JF, Cavas M. The influence of placebo effect on craving and cognitive performance in alcohol, caffeine, or nicotine consumers: a systematic review. Front Psychiatry 2020; 11: 849. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00849
51 McKay D, Schare ML. The effects of alcohol and alcohol expectancies on subjective reports and physiological reactivity: a meta-analysis. Addict Behav 1999; 24: 633–47. doi:10.1016/s0306-4603(99)00021-0
52 Daniali H, Flaten MA. Placebo analgesia, nocebo hyperalgesia, and the cardiovascular system: a qualitative systematic review. Front Physiol 2020; 11: 549807. doi:10.3389/fphys.2020.549807
53 Quinn VF, Colagiuri B. Placebo interventions for nausea: a systematic review. Ann Behav Med 2015; 49: 449–62. doi:10.1007/s12160-014-9670-3
54 Meeuwis SH, van Middendorp H, van Laarhoven AIM, et al. Placebo and nocebo effects for itch and itch-related immune outcomes: a systematic review of animal and human studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2020; 113: 325–37. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.025
55 Amanzio M, Mitsikostas DD, Giovannelli F, et al. Adverse events of active and placebo groups in SARS-Cov-2 vaccine randomized trials: a systematic review. Lancet Reg Health Eur 2022; 12: 100253. doi:10.1016/j.lanepe.2021.100253
56 Horváth Á, Köteles F, Szabo A. Nocebo effects on motor performance: a systematic literature review. Scand J Psychol 2021; 62: 665–74. doi:10.1111/sjop.12753
57 Marticorena FM, Carvalho A, Oliveira LFD, et al. Nonplacebo controls to determine the magnitude of ergogenic interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2021; 53: 1766–77. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000002635
58 Hurst P, Schipof-Godart L, Szabo A, et al. The placebo and nocebo effect on sports performance: a systematic review. Eur J Sport Sci 2020; 20: 279–92. doi:10.1080/17461391.2019.1655098
59 Bérdi M, Köteles F, Szabó A, et al. Placebo effects in sport and exercise: a meta-analysis. EJMH 2011; 6: 196–212. doi:10.5708/EJMH.6.2011.2.5
60 Spiegel D, Kraemer H, Carlson RW. Is the placebo powerless N Engl J Med 2001; 345: 1276–9. doi:10.1056/NEJM200110253451712
61 Blease C, Colloca L, Kaptchuk TJ. Are open-label placebos ethical? informed consent and ethical equivocations. Bioethics 2016; 30: 407–14. doi:10.1111/bioe.12245
62 Colloca L, Howick J. Placebos without deception: outcomes, mechanisms, and ethics. Int Rev Neurobiol 2018; 138: 219–40. doi:10.1016/bs.irn.2018.01.005
63 Ashar YK, Chang LJ, Wager TD. Brain mechanisms of the placebo effect: an affective appraisal account. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2017; 13: 73–98. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093015
64 Benedetti F, Frisaldi E. Creating placebo responders and nonresponders in the laboratory: boons and banes. Pain Manag 2014; 4: 165–7. doi:10.2217/pmt.14.11
65 Frisaldi E, Shaibani A, Benedetti F. Placebo responders and nonresponders: what’s new Pain Manag 2018; 8: 405–8. doi:10.2217/pmt-2018-0054
66 Vase L, Skyt I, Hall KT. Placebo, nocebo, and neuropathic pain. Pain 2016; 157 Suppl 1 (Suppl 1): S98–105. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000445
67 Carlino E, Frisaldi E, Benedetti F. Pain and the context. Nat Rev Rheumatol 2014; 10: 348–55. doi:10.1038/nrrheum.2014.17
68 Peciña M, Zubieta J-K. Molecular mechanisms of placebo responses in humans. Mol Psychiatry 2015; 20: 416–23. doi:10.1038/mp.2014.164
69 Pecina M, Zubieta J-K. Expectancy modulation of opioid neurotransmission. Int Rev Neurobiol 2018; 138: 17–37.: S0074-7742(18)30016-3. doi:10.1016/bs.irn.2018.02.003
70 Atlas LY. A social affective neuroscience lens on placebo analgesia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2021; 25: 992–1005. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2021.07.016
71 Wai-lanYeung V, Geers A, Kam SM. Merely possessing a placebo analgesic reduced pain intensity: preliminary findings from a randomized design. Curr Psychol 2019; 38: 194–203. doi:10.1007/s12144-017-9601-0
72 Benedetti F, Shaibani A, Arduino C, et al. Open-label nondeceptive placebo analgesia is blocked by the opioid antagonist naloxone. Pain 2023; 164: 984–90. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002791
73 Wrobel N, Fadai T, Sprenger C, et al. Are children the better placebo analgesia responders. An Experimental Approach J Pain 2015; 16: 1005–11. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2015.06.013
74 Hall KT, Loscalzo J, Kaptchuk TJ. Genetics and the placebo effect: the Placebome. Trends Mol Med 2015; 21: 285–94.: S1471-4914(15)00043-X. doi:10.1016/j.molmed.2015.02.009
75 Amanzio M, Palermo S. Pain anticipation and nocebo-related responses: a descriptive mini-review of functional neuroimaging studies in normal subjects and precious hints on pain processing in the context of neurodegenerative disorders. Front Pharmacol 2019; 10: 969. doi:10.3389/fphar.2019.00969
76 Fiorio M, Recchia S, Corrà F, et al. Enhancing non-noxious perception: behavioural and neurophysiological correlates of a placebo-like manipulation. Neuroscience 2012; 217: 96–104. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.04.066
77 Murray D, Stoessl AJ. Mechanisms and therapeutic implications of the placebo effect in neurological and psychiatric conditions. Pharmacol Ther 2013; 140: 306–18. doi:10.1016/j.pharmthera.2013.07.009
78 Frisaldi E, Carlino E, Lanotte M, et al. Characterization of the thalamic-subthalamic circuit involved in the placebo response through single-neuron recording in parkinson patients. Cortex 2014; 60: 3–9. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2013.12.003
79 Enck P, Bingel U, Schedlowski M, et al. The placebo response in medicine: minimize, maximize or personalize Nat Rev Drug Discov 2013; 12: 191–204. doi:10.1038/nrd3923
80 Laverdure-Dupont D, Rainville P, Montplaisir J, et al. Relief expectation and sleep. Rev Neurosci 2010; 21: 381–95. doi:10.1515/revneuro.2010.21.5.381
81 Haas JW, Rief W, Glombiewski JA, et al. Expectation-induced placebo effect on acute sadness in women with major depression: an experimental investigation. J Affect Disord 2020; 274: 920–8.: S0165-0327(20)30250-0. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2020.05.056
82 Colagiuri B, Schenk LA, Kessler MD, et al. The placebo effect: from concepts to genes. Neuroscience 2015; 307: 171–90.: S0306-4522(15)00740-X. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.017
83 Rutherford BR, Wall MM, Brown PJ, et al. Patient expectancy as a mediator of placebo effects in antidepressant clinical trials. Am J Psychiatry 2017; 174: 135–42. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16020225
84 McCarney R, Warner J, Iliffe S, et al. The Hawthorne effect: a randomised, controlled trial. BMC Med Res Methodol 2007; 7: 30. doi:10.1186/1471-2288-7-30
85 Bailey RC, Baillie AJ. The relationship between placebo alcohol and affect: motives for drinking. Drug Alcohol Rev 2013; 32: 162–9. doi:10.1111/j.1465-3362.2012.00500.x
86 Dar R, Stronguin F, Etter J-F. Assigned versus perceived placebo effects in nicotine replacement therapy for smoking reduction in swiss smokers. J Consult Clin Psychol 2005; 73: 350–3. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.350
87 Pardo-Cabello AJ, Manzano-Gamero V, Puche-Cañas E. Placebo: a brief updated review. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 2022; 395: 1343–56. doi:10.1007/s00210-022-02280-w
88 Van Ree JM, Schagen Van Leeuwen JH, Koppeschaar HP, et al. Unexpected placebo response in premenstrual dysphoric disorder: implication of endogenous opioids. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005; 182: 318–9. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0090-8
89 Sandler AD, Glesne CE, Bodfish JW. Conditioned placebo dose reduction: a new treatment in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? J Dev Behav Pediatr 2010; 31: 369–75. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181e121ed
90 Hadamitzky M, Lückemann L, Pacheco-López G, et al. Pavlovian conditioning of immunological and neuroendocrine functions. Physiol Rev 2020; 100: 357–405. doi:10.1152/physrev.00033.2018
91 Park C, Pagnini F, Langer E. Glucose metabolism responds to perceived sugar intake more than actual sugar intake. Sci Rep 2020; 10: 15633. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-72501-w
92 Park C, Pagnini F, Reece A, et al. Blood sugar level follows perceived time rather than actual time in people with type 2 diabetes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2016; 113: 8168–70. doi:10.1073/pnas.1603444113
93 Malani A, Houser D. Expectations mediate objective physiological placebo effects. Adv Health Econ Health Serv Res 2008; 20: 311–27.
94 Olliges E, Schneider S, Schmidt G, et al. Placebo and nocebo effects in patients with takotsubo cardiomyopathy and heart-healthy controls. Front Psychiatry 2019; 10: 549. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00549
95 Eccles R. The powerful placebo effect in cough: relevance to treatment and clinical trials. Lung 2020; 198: 13–21. doi:10.1007/s00408-019-00305-5
96 Wolters F, Peerdeman KJ, Evers AWM. Placebo and nocebo effects across symptoms: from pain to fatigue, dyspnea, nausea, and itch. Front Psychiatry 2019; 10: 470. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00470
97 Vinckier F, Betka S, Nion N, et al. Harnessing the power of anticipation to manage respiratory-related brain suffering and ensuing dyspnoea: insights from the neurobiology of the respiratory nocebo effect. Eur Respir J 2021; 58: 2101876. doi:10.1183/13993003.01876-2021
98 Elsenbruch S, Enck P. Placebo effects and their determinants in gastrointestinal disorders. Nat Rev Gastroenterol Hepatol 2015; 12: 472–85. doi:10.1038/nrgastro.2015.117
99 Enck P, Horing B, Weimer K, et al. Placebo responses and placebo effects in functional bowel disorders. Eur J Gastroenterol Hepatol 2012; 24: 1–8. doi:10.1097/MEG.0b013e32834bb951
100 Price DD, Finniss DG, Benedetti F. A comprehensive review of the placebo effect: recent advances and current thought. Annu Rev Psychol 2008; 59: 565–90. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.113006.095941
101 Sondermann W, Reinboldt-Jockenhöfer F, Dissemond J, et al. Effects of patients’ expectation in dermatology: evidence from experimental and clinical placebo studies and implications for dermatologic practice and research. Dermatology 2021; 237: 857–71. doi:10.1159/000513445
102 Bartels DJP, van Laarhoven AIM, van de Kerkhof PCM, et al. Placebo and nocebo effects on itch: effects, mechanisms, and predictors. Eur J Pain 2016; 20: 8–13. doi:10.1002/ejp.750
103 Sölle A, Worm M, Benedetti F, et al. Targeted use of placebo effects decreases experimental itch in Atopic dermatitis patients: a randomized controlled trial. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2021; 110: 486–97. doi:10.1002/cpt.2276
104 Pagnini F, Cavalera C, Volpato E, et al. Illness expectations predict the development of influenza-like symptoms over the winter season. Complement Ther Med 2020; 50: 102396. doi:10.1016/j.ctim.2020.102396
105 Tippens KM, Purnell JQ, Gregory WL, et al. Expectancy, self-efficacy, and placebo effect of a sham supplement for weight loss in obese adults. J Evid Based Complementary Altern Med 2014; 19: 181–8. doi:10.1177/2156587214528513
106 Fontaine KR, Williams MS, Hoenemeyer TW, et al. Placebo effects in obesity research. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2016; 24: 769–71. doi:10.1002/oby.21456
107 Shaibani A, Frisaldi E, Benedetti F. Placebo response in pain, fatigue, and performance: possible implications for neuromuscular disorders: placebo response. Muscle Nerve 2017; 56: 358–67. doi:10.1002/mus.25635
108 Beedie C, Benedetti F, Barbiani D, et al. Incorporating methods and findings from neuroscience to better understand placebo and nocebo effects in sport. Eur J Sport Sci 2020; 20: 313–25. doi:10.1080/17461391.2019.1675765
109 Davis AJ, Hettinga F, Beedie C. You don’t need to administer a placebo to elicit a placebo effect: social factors trigger neurobiological pathways to enhance sports performance. Eur J Sport Sci 2020; 20: 302–12. doi:10.1080/17461391.2019.1635212
110 Clifasefi SL, Garry M, Harper DN, et al. Psychotropic placebos create resistance to the misinformation effect. Psychon Bull Rev 2007; 14: 112–7. doi:10.3758/bf03194037
111 Bingel U. Placebo 2.0: the impact of expectations on analgesic treatment outcome. Pain 2020; 161 Suppl 1: S48–56. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001981
112 Frisaldi E, Shaibani A, Benedetti F. Why we should assess patients’ expectations in clinical trials. Pain Ther 2017; 6: 107–10. doi:10.1007/s40122-017-0071-8
113 Evers AWM, Colloca L, Blease C, et al. What should Clinicians tell patients about placebo and Nocebo effects. Practical Considerations Based on Expert Consensus Psychother Psychosom 2021; 90: 49–56. doi:10.1159/000510738
114 Frisaldi E, Shaibani A, Vollert J, et al. The placebo response in myasthenia gravis assessed by quantitative myasthenia gravis score: a meta‐analysis. Muscle Nerve 2019; 59: 671–8. doi:10.1002/mus.26469
115 Frisaldi E, Vollert J, Al Sultani H, et al. Placebo and nocebo responses in painful diabetic neuropathy: systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain 2023. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000003000
116 Stoll CRT, Izadi S, Fowler S, et al. The value of a second reviewer for study selection in systematic reviews. Res Synth Methods 2019; 10: 539–45. doi:10.1002/jrsm.1369
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2023 Author(s) (or their employer(s)) 2023. Re-use permitted under CC BY-NC. No commercial re-use. See rights and permissions. Published by BMJ. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited, appropriate credit is given, any changes made indicated, and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ . Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Objectives
This review aimed to summarise the existing knowledge about placebo and nocebo effects associated with pharmacological interventions and their mechanisms.
Design
Umbrella review, adopting the Assessment of Multiple Systematic Reviews 2 tool for critical appraisal.
Data sources
MEDLINE/PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, PsycINFO, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trial were searched in September 2022, without any time restriction, for systematic reviews, narrative reviews, original articles. Results were summarised through narrative synthesis, tables, 95% CI.
Outcome measures
Mechanisms underlying placebo/nocebo effects and/or their effect sizes.
Results
The databases search identified 372 studies, for a total of 158 312 participants, comprising 41 systematic reviews, 312 narrative reviews and 19 original articles. Seventy-three per cent of the examined systematic reviews were of high quality.
Our findings revealed that mechanisms underlying placebo and/or nocebo effects have been characterised, at least in part, for: pain, non-noxious somatic sensation, Parkinson’s disease, migraine, sleep disorders, intellectual disability, depression, anxiety, dementia, addiction, gynaecological disorders, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, immune and endocrine systems, cardiovascular and respiratory systems, gastrointestinal disorders, skin diseases, influenza and related vaccines, oncology, obesity, physical and cognitive performance. Their magnitude ranged from 0.08 to 2.01 (95% CI 0.37 to 0.89) for placebo effects and from 0.32 to 0.90 (95% CI 0.24 to 1.00) for nocebo effects.
Conclusions
This study provides a valuable tool for clinicians and researchers, identifying both results ready for clinical practice and gaps to address in the near future.
Funding
Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Milan, Italy with the ‘Finanziamento Ponte 2022’ grant.
PROSPERO registration number
CRD42023392281.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details




1 Department of Neuroscience "Rita Levi Montalcini", University of Turin, Turin, Italy
2 Muscle and Nerve Center, Houston, Texas, USA; Department of Medicine, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA
3 Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Milan, Italy