JH and QX are joint first authors.
WHAT IS ALREADY KNOWN ON THIS TOPIC
Synaptic loss and deficits have recently been found in various mental disorders.
18F-labelled difluoro-analogue of UCB-J (18F-SynVesT-1) is a new synaptic vesicle glycoprotein 2A positron emission tomography (PET) radiotracer and a potentially sensitive biomarker indicating synaptic density in vivo.
WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS
Using 18F-SynVesT-1 PET imaging, synaptic deficits in the bilateral putamen, right pregenual anterior cingulate cortex and right Rolandic operculum were closely linked to the severity of internet gaming disorder.
HOW THIS STUDY MIGHT AFFECT RESEARCH, PRACTICE OR POLICY
Our novel in vivo findings provide new insights into the neuropathological mechanisms of internet gaming disorder and may suggest possibilities for developing new treatments targeting synaptic function.
Introduction
Internet gaming disorder (IGD) is an emerging and increasing problem worldwide that leads to clinically significant impairment or distress.1 2 As a specific behavioural addictive disorder, IGD involves excessive or poorly controlled preoccupations, impulses or behaviours regarding persistent and recurrent online gameplay. The prevalence of IGD ranges from 1.2% to 8.5% in the USA and Europe3–5 and from 6% to 12% in Asian countries,6 with a particularly higher prevalence among men and young people. As a behavioural addiction unique to humans that has received recognition in the past decade, IGD has been observed in the clinic. However, understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms related to this disorder remains in its early stages. With the current lack of evidence-based treatments for IGD, neurobiological studies may be needed to inform the development of novel treatments.
Synaptic deficits are evident in different behavioural addictions. For example, enduring impairments in transmission and synaptic plasticity were shown at glutamatergic synapses in the nucleus accumbens subcompartment in rats with diet-induced obesity and food addiction.7 8 The effect of synaptic γ-aminobutyric acid release induced by dopamine rises was reduced or even reversed in problem gamblers.9 Furthermore, dopamine transporter binding ratios in the bilateral caudate and putamen were inversely correlated with days spent gambling and reward-based decision-making in subjects with gambling disorders.10 In terms of IGD, dysregulation of postsynaptic dopamine D2 receptors in the orbitofrontal cortex and striatum was found.11 These findings provide evidence of aberrant synaptic connectivity and plasticity in individuals engaging in excessive and problematic non-substance use behaviours. However, abnormalities in synaptic density have not yet been elucidated in any behavioural addiction.
Synaptic vesicle glycoprotein 2A (SV2A) is an integral protein ubiquitously present in the presynaptic terminals of all synapses across the brain12 and is a suitable marker of synaptic density. 11C-UCB-J, a radioligand that has high specificity for SV2A,13 has recently been shown to be sensitive to region-specific decreases in synaptic density in cocaine and cannabis use disorders,14 15 depression, and post-traumatic stress disorder.16 A new SV2A positron emission tomography (PET) radiotracer, the 18F-labelled difluoro-analogue of UCB-J (18F-SynVesT-1), showed similar useful imaging properties, a longer radioactive half-life and a superior signal to noise ratio compared with 11C-UCB-J in healthy humans.17 Combined with PET data, these techniques allow the change in synaptic density in the brain to be evaluated in real time, dynamically and in vivo. We hypothesised that the region-specific abnormal synaptic density might also occur in IGD.
The current study aimed to investigate the behavioural performance of individuals with IGD and the neural correlates of cognitive function that underlie this disorder. We assessed the differences in synaptic density by using 18F-SynVesT-1 and PET imaging approaches in subjects with IGD and healthy controls (HCs). The Stroop Colour-Word Test (SCWT),18 stop-signal paradigms19 and N-back tasks20 are widely used to examine cognitive function in IGD and other addictions, and we also examined potential correlations between synaptic density and severity of IGD and specific cognitive impairment. We hypothesised that patients with IGD would exhibit lower synaptic density in executive function or reward-seeking brain regions compared with HCs and that there is a relationship between synaptic density and the severity of gaming addiction.
Methods
Participants
The study included unmedicated clinical subjects diagnosed with current IGD according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5)21 and 16 age-matched and sex-matched HC subjects who participated in the study. Participants were recruited by word of mouth, posters and flyers. All study participants provided written informed consent before inclusion in the study. They were screened with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5, Clinician Version, within 3 days of the PET scan to determine the presence of major psychiatric disorders. All subjects were medication-naive during the assessment. The flow chart of this study is shown in figure 1.
Figure 1. Flowchart of study enrolment. 18 F-SynVesT-1, 18 F-labelled difluoro-analogue of UCB-J; BIS-11, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11; DSM-5, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition; HAMA, Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale; HAMD, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; HC, healthy control; IGD, internet gaming disorder; IGDS9-SF, Internet Gaming Disorder Scale-Short Form; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; PET, positron emission tomography; SCWT, Stroop Colour-Word Test; SST, stop-signal task.
The inclusion criteria for participants with IGD were as follows: (1) primary use of the internet to play games and regular internet game use over the past 12 months (daily internet game use >7 hours/day or >30 hours/week), (2) an Internet Gaming Disorder Scale-Short Form (IGDS9-SF) score ≥3222 and (3) an IGD diagnosis based on the DSM-5 criteria. The inclusion criteria for HCs were as follows: (1) regular use of a smartphone, laptop or other electronic device but not diagnosed with IGD based on the DSM-5 criteria and (2) IGDS9-SF score <21. The exclusion criteria for IGD and HCs included (1) a lifetime or current diagnosis of a major psychiatric disorder (eg, met the DSM-5 criteria for gambling disorders, nicotine or alcohol use disorders, neurodevelopmental disorders, schizophrenia spectrum disorders, bipolar disorders, or related disorders), (2) clinically significant medical conditions or laboratory abnormalities, (3) other illicit substance use or nicotine use in the past 12 months, (4) pregnancy or lactation before any scan, and (5) contraindications to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI).
The custom-designed questionnaire included age, sex, education level and game usage and was used to collect sociodemographic data. The participants were evaluated to determine internet use patterns and were screened for IGD using the Chinese version of the IGDS9-SF.22 23 The IGDS9-SF has been validated for use among Chinese participants and has high internal consistency (Cronbach’s α was 0.91).24 We used the Chinese version of the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAMD) (reliability coefficient of 0.714 and validity coefficient of 0.92),25 26 the Chinese version of the Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAMA) (reliability coefficient of 0.88)27 28 and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11 (BIS-11)29 to test impulsivity. The BIS-11 has been translated into Chinese with good internal consistency and test–retest reliability.30 The BIS-11 consists of three subscales: cognitive impulsivity, motor impulsivity and non-planning impulsivity.
Cognitive function measures
Stop-signal task
Before entering the PET scanner, participants performed a manual version of the stop-signal task (SST)31 (online supplemental figure S1). During the SST, subjects were instructed to press the ‘F’ button in response to a circle and the ‘J’ button in response to a fork with the index finger of their left or right hand as rapidly and accurately as possible. In a minority of trials, a ‘Stop’ signal (in which the green box turned red unpredictably) was presented at varying delays after the ‘Go’ stimulus signals, indicating that the subject should withhold that motor response. A fixation cross was presented for a period of 1–5 s and the ‘Go’ reaction time (Go RT) was shorter than 1 s. For each run, the ‘Stop’ signal delay (SSD) time was initially set at 250 ms and then either increased or decreased by 50 ms after a successful or failed ‘Stop’ response, respectively. The staircase algorithm adjusted the temporal delay between ‘Go’ and ‘Stop’ stimuli in 50 ms increments to achieve a 50% target ‘Stop’ inhibition rate in stop trials. The stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) was calculated by subtracting the critical SSD from the mean RT in all correct-response go trials. The Go RT and SSRT were the primary performance measures, representing response initiation and inhibitory control, respectively. A shorter SSRT reflects a faster ‘stop’ process and represents better inhibitory control ability. Subjects with impaired motor inhibition are less able to inhibit their motor response and thus have longer SSRTs.32
Stroop Colour-Word Test
We administered the SCWT,33 consisting of three parts: word reading (WR), colour naming (CN) and colour word (CW). In each part, subjects were requested to read WR and name the colour of the ink of each item (a series of Xs in CN and the name of a colour different from that of the ink in CW) in a list of 100 elements. In each part, we recorded the number of elements correctly completed in 45 s and the interference score (IS). The IS, considered a measure of inhibitory control, was calculated from the other three scores [IS=CW−(WR×CN/WR+CN)].
Two-back and three-back tests
Subjects had to indicate whether the graphic presented in the centre of a computer screen was the same as the graphic presented two or three positions before (two-back or three-back).34 Participants responded by pressing the ‘F’ button of a keyboard with their left index finger when the graphic was the same (target) or the ‘J’ button with their right index finger when the graphic was different (non-target). In each test, a total of 63 trials were performed, but the first three non-target trials were not included in the analyses. The number of target trials was 20 (ie, ~32% of the total set). The stimulus duration was 500 ms, with a 2500 ms interstimulus interval. Accuracies on the whole test were recorded.
Magnetic resonance imaging
High-resolution MRIs were collected on a General Electric (GE) Healthcare 3.0 T MRI scanner. To register with PET images and check for structural abnormalities, a sagittal three-dimensional (3D) T1 bravo sequence was employed (echo time (TE)=3.2 ms, repetition time (TR)=8.5 ms, matrix=256×256 mm2, phase field of view (FOV)=1, voxel size 1.0×1.0×1.0 mm3, slice thickness=1.0 mm). All MRI data were quality-checked by a skilled neuroradiologist.
Positron emission tomography
18F-SynVesT-1 was synthesised using a previously described method with radiochemical purity >99%.17 35 None of the participants took drugs targeted at SV2A for at least 1 day before the PET scans. The PET/computed tomography (CT) was carried out with a GE PET/CT scanner (Discovery 690 Elite; GE Healthcare, Waukesha, Wisconsin). 18F-SynVesT-1 PET/CT imaging was performed 60 min after the radiotracer was intravenously injected at a dose of 3.7–4.44 MBq (0.1–0.12 mCi) per kilogram of body weight. Static PET images were acquired in three dimensions for 30 min. First, a low-dose CT scan (120 kV; automatic mAs; pitch, 1:1; slice thickness, 3.75 mm; matrix, 512×512) was performed for attenuation correction. Next, all PET images were reconstructed as a 256×256 transaxial matrix (35 cm field of view) using the 3D VUE Point (GE Healthcare) ordered-subset expectation-maximisation algorithm with six iterations and six subsets, which produced 47 transaxial images at 3.25 mm intervals. Participants were also monitored on-site for other signs of adverse effects for 90 min after injection of 18F-SynVesT-1 and were asked to report any ensuing adverse effects.
MRI and PET analysis
The structural MRIs were standardised with automatic anatomical marker templates in the Montréal Neurological Institute space using statistical parametric mapping software (SPM V.12; Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). Then the transformation parameters determined by MRI spatial normalisation were applied to the coregistered 18F-SynVesT-1 PET image for PET spatial normalisation. The standardised uptake values (SUVs) were calculated for all regions of interest. The SUV ratio (SUVR) with the centrum semiovale (CS) as a reference region was calculated for interpatient comparisons using SPM, as the CS has been reported to be free of SV2A and thus can be used as a reference region.36 The height threshold for synaptic density changes was set at p<0.001 with a cluster size of at least 50. After data preprocessing with SPM, the xjView MATLAB toolbox was used to visualise and anatomically label significant clusters.
Demographic and clinical data analysis
Differences between the IGD and HC groups in demographic, clinical and radiotracer characteristics were assessed using independent-sample t-tests for continuous measures and χ2 tests for categorical variables. The Mann-Whitney U test was applied to compare non-normally distributed variables. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov one-sample test was used to determine whether variables were normally distributed. The Bonferroni correction was adopted to adjust for multiple comparisons. Furthermore, group differences in the SUVR of 18F-SynVesT-1 between patients with IGD and the HCs were analysed in various brain regions using a two-sample t-test as a confounding covariate. However, given the exploratory nature of these analyses and the limited sample size, no adjustments for multiple comparisons were made. The relationship between the SUVR of 18F-SynVesT-1 and clinical/cognitive parameters was explored with a scatter plot in subjects with IGD. Pearson correlation analyses were used to examine the relationship between the SUVR of 18F-SynVesT-1 and clinical/cognitive parameters. SPSS V.23.0 was used to perform the analysis, and GraphPad Prism V.7.0 (GraphPad Software, Boston, Massachusetts, USA) was used to generate figures. The two-tailed p values had a significance level of α=0.05.
Results
In total, 18 patients were enrolled in the IGD group and 16 in the HC group. The demographics were well matched between the two groups. All participants enrolled in the study were right-handed. The mean (SD) age was 20.53 (3.69) for the IGD group and 22.44 (5.44) for the HC group. There were four women enrolled in the IGD group and six in the HC group. None of the participants reported previous or current neurological or psychiatric disorders, illegal drug use, or nicotine or alcohol use disorders. The 18F-SynVesT-1 injection was well tolerated, with no subjective or objective adverse effects detected. In addition, all the participants were free of depressive and anxiety disorders. However, a minority of subjects with IGD had mild to moderate anxiety or depression symptoms, as assessed by the HAMA and HAMD, respectively (table 1).
Table 1Demographic and clinical characteristics
Variable | IGD Mean (SD) | HC Mean (SD) | t | P value |
Age (years) | 20.53 (3.69) | 22.44 (5.44) | −1.305 | 0.201 |
Male:female | 14:4 | 10:6 | 0.456 | |
Education (years) | 13.12 (1.87) | 13.38 (1.94) | −0.382 | 0.705 |
Daily gaming (hours) | 11.47 (2.83) | N/A | N/A | N/A |
Duration of IGD (years) | 3.61 (1.64) | N/A | N/A | N/A |
Frequency of IG usage after midnight per week | 4.29 (2.31) | N/A | N/A | N/A |
Frequency of IG usage overnight per week | 2.81 (2.60) | N/A | N/A | N/A |
IGDS9-SF | 35.33 (2.00) | 13.00 (5.56) | 15.753 | <0.001 |
HAMA | 11.00 (6.67) | 3.1 (3.16) | 3.999 | <0.001 |
HAMD | 18.89 (10.08) | 3.46 (3.52) | 5.268 | <0.001 |
BIS-11 cognitive | 44.06 (15.10) | 33.27 (11.96) | −2.634 | 0.035 |
BIS-11 motor | 48.89 (15.56) | 27.50 (14.03) | −3.472 | <0.001 |
BIS-11 non-planning | 55.58 (20.58) | 32.11 (15.37) | −2.580 | 0.002 |
BIS-11 total | 49.54 (12.66) | 30.22 (10.81) | −1.008 | <0.001 |
Two-back test accuracy | 0.73 (0.19) | 0.87 (0.09) | −2.419 | 0.022 |
Three-back test accuracy | 0.62 (0.20) | 0.74 (0.13) | −1.842 | 0.076 |
SCWT | ||||
WR | 89.06 (11.97) | 98.08 (3.33) | −3.039 | 0.006 |
CN | 67.39 (17.45) | 86.46 (10.91) | −3.735 | 0.001 |
CW | 39.50 (12.20) | 49.69 (8.60) | −2.728 | 0.011 |
IS | 1.43 (6.48) | 3.94 (7.31) | −0.988 | 0.333 |
SST | ||||
Go RT, ms | 682.06 (106.57) | 619.79 (120.25) | 1.739 | 0.091 |
SSD, ms | 382.28 (84.53) | 337.95 (100.13) | 1.235 | 0.226 |
SSRT, ms | 300.36 (35.94) | 274.34 (26.90) | 2.434 | 0.021 |
BIS-11, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11; CN, colour naming; CW, colour word; Go RT, reaction time in correct go trials; HAMA, Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale; HAMD, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; HC, healthy control; IG, internet gaming; IGD, internet gaming disorder; IGDS9-SF, Internet Gaming Disorder Scale-Short Form; IS, interference score; N/A, not applicable; SCWT, Stroop Colour-Word Test; SD, standard deviation; SSD, stop-signal delay; SSRT, stop-signal reaction time; SST, stop-signal task; WR, word reading.
The IGD group scored higher on the IGDS9-SF than the HC group (p<0.001). The mean (SD) daily internet gaming (IG) usage reached 11.47 (2.83) hours, and IG usage after midnight/overnight each week reached 4.29 (2.31)/2.81 (2.60) times in the IGD group. The details of the demographic and clinical characteristics of the subgroups are summarised in table 1. The results also indicated the presence of significant group differences on the BIS-11 cognitive (t=−2.634, p=0.035), motor (t=−3.472, p<0.001) and non-planning subscales (t=−2.580, p=0.002), in addition to the BIS-11 total scores (t=−1.008, p<0.001).
HCs performed better on the two-back test (t=−2.419, p=0.022), but not on the three-back test (t=−1.842, p=0.076), than patients with IGD. On the Stroop test, the accuracies for WR (t=−3.039, p=0.006), CN (t=−3.735, p=0.001) and CW (t=−2.728, p=0.011) were significantly higher in HCs than in patients with IGD, but no difference in IS scores on the SCWT was found between the groups (t=−0.988, p=0.333). The staircase algorithm in the SST guaranteed that the participants had an approximately 50% success rate in stop trials. Both the IGD and HC groups had accuracy rates higher than 50% in the stop trials despite a group difference, indicating that the present task was conducted successfully. Relative to HCs, patients with IGD had significantly longer SSRTs (t=2.434, p=0.021). Regarding the other indices of the SST (Go RT and SSD), no significant group difference between the IGD and HC groups was found.
Compared with HCs, patients with IGD showed lower uptake of 18F-SynVesT-1 in the right pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), bilateral putamen and right Rolandic operculum (height threshold at p<0.001, with a level above 50 contiguous voxels; table 2, figures 2 and 3, online supplemental table S1). The associations between synaptic density (SUVRs of 18F-SynVesT-1) and clinical characteristics are summarised in online supplemental table S2. Significant linear relationships between higher IGDS9-SF scores or longer daily gaming hours and lower synaptic density (SUVR of 18F-SynVesT-1) of the left putamen (r=−0.476, p=0.046; r=−0.573, p=0.015) and the right putamen (r=−0.479, p=0.044; r=−0.479, p=0.010) were observed in patients with IGD (figure 4). SSRTs were negatively correlated with the SUVR of 18F-SynVesT-1 in the right pregenual ACC (r=−0.573, p=0.013) and the right Rolandic operculum (r=−0.527, p=0.025). There were no associations between SV2A availability and frequency of IG usage, BIS-11 subscales and total scores, accuracies for WR, CN and CW, and IS scores for SCWT.
Table 2Location and peaks of significant synaptic density reduction in patients with IGD compared with HC
Location | Cluster level | Peak level | Coordinates | |||||
Cluster KE | P value (uncorrected) | t | Z-score | P value (uncorrected) | x (mm) | y (mm) | z (mm) | |
Left lenticular nucleus, putamen | 77 | 0.002 | 5.371 | 4.501 | <0.001 | −28 | 2 | 8 |
3.527 | 3.217 | <0.001 | −32 | −4 | 4 | |||
3.442 | 3.151 | <0.001 | −24 | 16 | 0 | |||
Right lenticular nucleus, putamen | 130 | <0.001 | 5.308 | 4.462 | <0.001 | 24 | 10 | 4 |
4.716 | 4.079 | <0.001 | 28 | 4 | 8 | |||
Right Rolandic operculum | 53 | 0.008 | 5.048 | 4.297 | <0.001 | 56 | −16 | 18 |
3.612 | 3.283 | <0.001 | 44 | −10 | 14 | |||
Right anterior cingulate cortex, pregenual | 59 | 0.005 | 4.477 | 3.916 | <0.001 | 2 | 38 | −6 |
Left anterior cingulate cortex, pregenual | 77 | 0.002 | 3.788 | 3.417 | <0.001 | −2 | 40 | 10 |
HC, healthy control; IGD, internet gaming disorder; KE, cluster size.
Figure 2. Comparison of the clusters of synaptic changes by 18 F-SynVesT-1 PET with patients with IGD versus HCs was performed in (A) render view and (B) slice view (p<0.001 with cluster thresholding, k=50 voxels). Voxels with significantly low uptake are shown in red. 18 F-SynVesT-1, 18 F-labelled difluoro-analogue of UCB-J; HCs, healthy controls; IGD, internet gaming disorder; PET, positron emission tomography.
Figure 3. Voxel-wise analysis results of group comparison with 18 F-SynVesT-1 SUVR in IGD and HCs using a two-sample t-test (**p<0.001). 18 F-SynVesT-1, 18 F-labelled difluoro-analogue of UCB-J; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; HC, healthy control; IGD, internet gaming disorder; SUVR, standardised uptake value ratio.
Figure 4. Significant relationships between synaptic density with severity of IGD and inhibitory control in subjects with IGD. The SUVR of 18 F-SynVesT-1 PET was negatively correlated with IGDS9-SF and daily gaming hours in the bilateral putamen (A and B) and also negatively correlated with SSRT in the right pregenual ACC and right Rolandic operculum (C and D). 18 F-SynVesT-1, 18 F-labelled difluoro-analogue of UCB-J; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; IGD, internet gaming disorder; IGDS9-SF, Internet Gaming Disorder Scale-Short Form; SSRT, stop-signal reaction time; SUVR, standardised uptake value ratio.
Discussion
Main findings
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to measure SV2A synaptic density using SV2A in IGD in vivo. Using the new radiotracer (18F-SynVesT-1), patients with IGD had significant reductions in synaptic density compared with HCs in three prespecified brain areas: right ACC, bilateral putamen and right Rolandic operculum. Lower synaptic density in the bilateral putamen, which is part of the reward system, is correlated with the severity of IGD. Moreover, a correlation between worse inhibitory control and lower synaptic density in the right ACC and Rolandic operculum was found in the IGD group. Therefore, the pattern of associations between altered synaptic integrity and behavioural measures of IGD suggests that such alterations may be disorder-specific and a potentially novel candidate for a brain-based functional biomarker of this disorder.
The findings of this study add to the evidence indicating that synaptic density changes occur in addictive disorders. To date, a relationship between synaptic density and addictive behaviour has been demonstrated only in substance use disorder (SUD). Two recent studies using 11C-UCB-J PET found lower levels of the synaptic terminal protein SV2A in the frontal and temporal cortices of patients with cocaine and cannabis use disorders.14 15 Based on the above research, we believe that IGD and SUDs likely share at least a subset of similar underlying neurobiological mechanisms. Furthermore, results from recent studies suggest that reduced metabolism, volume and synaptic deficits in brain regions involved in executive function, reward-related processing, decision-making and impulse inhibition occur in these disorders, according to in vivo PET11 37 and functional MRI studies,38–40 which is in line with our findings.
Consistent with our hypothesis, lower SV2A availability in reward circuit-related brain regions was found in patients with IGD. The putamen, along with the globus pallidus, forms the lentiform nucleus; it and the caudate nucleus compose the striatum, a subcortical limbic structure. The putamen is involved in reward-related processing and the development of addiction.41 42 In addition, the ACC, particularly the pregenual component, and the adjacent medial prefrontal cortex have been implicated in the craving processes related to cocaine and gambling.43 The cingulate gyrus is part of the limbic system, which activates in response to addiction-related cues, resulting in immediate, strong emotional reactions. Studies assessing reward processing in individuals with IGD have shown reduced putamen functional connectivity of the putamen with the posterior insula-parietal operculum44 and decreased bidirectional interactions between the putamen and ACC.45 A PET study using 11C-raclopride found that striatal dopamine release is reduced in subjects with internet addiction.46 Another 18F-fluoro-2-deoxyglucose PET study reported that the regional cerebral metabolic rate of glucose in the right ACC was associated with the severity of IGD.47 Notably, a longitudinal study of recovered subjects with IGD showed increased ACC lentiform connectivity at 1 year relative to onset.45 Together with our observations, these results suggest that patients with IGD exhibit hypometabolism, synaptic loss and lower connectivity in the ACC and putamen.
In addition, we also found significant reductions in synaptic density in the right Rolandic operculum (ie, postcentral operculum) in patients with IGD compared with the HCs. The Rolandic operculum is implicated in the semantic processing of language, emotional processing of facial expressions, sensorimotor integration and bodily self-consciousness. Our result is consistent with those of previous voxel-based morphometry studies showing decreased grey matter volume in the bilateral Rolandic operculum in individuals with addiction-related disorders compared with HCs (p<0.005).48 Resting-state functional MRI data from subjects with IGD revealed lower static functional connectivity between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the Rolandic operculum.49 However, the exact mechanism of Rolandic operculum abnormalities in IGD remains unclear, and more research is needed to explain this in the future.
Similar to those with SUD, patients with IGD might display faulty inhibitory control mechanisms. Consistent with findings in this field,50–52 we observed greater impulsivity and worse performance on the gaming working memory task, attentional inhibition and prepotent motor inhibitory control in patients with IGD than in HCs. Regarding the effect of synaptic density on SST performance, decreased synaptic density of the right pregenual ACC and right Rolandic operculum was associated with a more negative SSRT change. Previous work has established that lower activation in the ACC19 53 and Rolandic operculum54 is associated with worse behavioural inhibition and execution (longer Go RTs and SSRTs). Additionally, we also demonstrated that the severity of IGD was associated with synaptic density changes in the putamen, providing a possible molecular explanation for synaptic density alterations that may underlie behavioural and cognitive impairments in IGD. This hypothesis needs to be evaluated with further studies exploring the longitudinal trajectories of these measures and investigating the synaptogenic effects of interventions, such as transcranial direct current stimulation,55 cognitive–behavioural therapy, family therapy or electronic-based intervention,56 to reduce cravings and promote recovery in patients with IGD.
Limitations
Several limitations of this study should be noted. First, although the sample size of this study was appropriate for the standards of previous similar PET/CT studies,14–16 57 it was relatively small. Therefore, the presence of false-negative results in the frontal and temporal lobes cannot be ruled out. We readily acknowledge that this is a preliminary analysis, and replication in a larger sample is needed to support our interpretations. Second, this is a cross-sectional study. Therefore, we cannot determine whether the lower synaptic density in patients with IGD represents a cause or a consequence of IGD. Further longitudinal investigations measuring synaptic density after a period of gaming abstinence could help to determine whether the lower synaptic density increases after withdrawal, enabling us to better understand the relationship between synaptic density changes and the course of IGD.
Implications
To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to provide in vivo evidence of synaptic alterations in patients with IGD using 18F-SynVesT-1-PET imaging. This investigation provided intriguing preliminary translational support for relatively low uptake distribution in the right ACC, bilateral putamen and right Rolandic operculum in patients with IGD compared with HCs. These findings also imply that the synaptic deficits in these regions are closely linked to the severity of addiction and impairment of inhibitory control in IGD, although further work is needed to clarify the nature of this association. Such work can advance our understanding of the role of synaptic function in IGD and identify more effective and novel treatment approaches based on synapse-related mechanisms.
The authors thank SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, United Kingdom), xjView toolbox for freely providing software analysis tools. The authors thank professor Yiyun Huang (PET Center, Department of Radiology and Biomedical Imaging, Yale University) for drug synthesis.
Data availability statement
Data are available upon reasonable request. The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author.
Ethics statements
Patient consent for publication
All participants provided informed consent to take part in this study.
Ethics approval
This study involves human participants and was approved by the Institutional Ethics Review Committee of Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, for research purposes only (no: 202106134). The study conformed to the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). The procedures were conducted in accordance with the approved guidelines. Participants gave informed consent to participate in the study before taking part.
Correction notice This article has been corrected since it was first published. Figure 4 has been updated.
Contributors HD and JH designed the task and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. HD and QX collected and analysed the data and prepared the figures. JH collected the image data, contributed to analysis of data and prepared the figures. MZ undertook the drug synthesis and quality control. LX was responsible for data curation. MY, JL and TL contributed to collecting and preparing the data. NZ contributed to editing, interpretation and revision processes. SH and HD were mainly responsible for proposing the concepts, revising the manuscripts, project management and providing funds. SH and HD are the guarantors for the manuscript.
Funding This work was supported by the STI2030-Major Projects (grant number 2022ZD0211100; NZ), Foundation of Shanghai Mental Health Center (grant number 2021-YJ15; JL), National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number 82272045; SH), National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number 82201702; QX), Science and Technology Innovation Program of Hunan Province (grant number 2021RC4056; SH), Key Program of Ministry of Industry and Information Technology of China (grant number CEIEC-2022-ZM02-0219; SH), National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number 82001405; HD), and Hunan Natural Science Foundation Youth Program (grant number 2021JJ40979; HD).
Competing interests None declared.
Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Supplemental material This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.
1 Zhao M, Hao W. Challenges of gaming disorder: suggestions from a public health perspective. Gen Psychiatr 2019; 32: e100086. doi:10.1136/gpsych-2019-100086
2 Ballou N, Zendle D. "Clinically significant distress” in internet gaming disorder: an individual participant meta-analysis. Comput Hum Behav 2022; 129: 107140. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2021.107140
3 Gao Y-X, Wang J-Y, Dong G-H. The prevalence and possible risk factors of internet gaming disorder among adolescents and young adults: systematic reviews and meta-analyses. J Psychiatr Res 2022; 154: 35–43. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.06.049
4 Kim HS, Son G, Roh E-B, et al. Prevalence of gaming disorder: a meta-analysis. Addict Behav 2022; 126: 107183. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.107183
5 Stevens MW, Dorstyn D, Delfabbro PH, et al. Global prevalence of gaming disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2021; 55: 553–68. doi:10.1177/0004867420962851
6 Liao Z, Chen X, Huang Q, et al. Prevalence of gaming disorder in East Asia: a comprehensive meta-analysis. J Behav Addict 2022; 11: 727–38. doi:10.1556/2006.2022.00050
7 Brown RM, Kupchik YM, Spencer S, et al. Addiction-like synaptic impairments in diet-induced obesity. Biol Psychiatry 2017; 81: 797–806. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.11.019
8 Becchi S, Hood J, Kendig MD, et al. Food for thought: diet-induced impairments to decision-making and amelioration by N-Acetylcysteine in male rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2022; 239: 3495–506. doi:10.1007/s00213-022-06223-4
9 Møller A, Rømer Thomsen K, Brooks DJ, et al. Attenuation of dopamine-induced GABA release in problem gamblers. Brain Behav 2019; 9: e01239. doi:10.1002/brb3.1239
10 Pettorruso M, Martinotti G, Cocciolillo F, et al. Striatal presynaptic dopaminergic dysfunction in gambling disorder: A (123) I-FP-CIT SPECT study. Addict Biol 2019; 24: 1077–86. doi:10.1111/adb.12677
11 Tian M, Chen Q, Zhang Y, et al. PET imaging reveals brain functional changes in internet gaming disorder. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 2014; 41: 1388–97. doi:10.1007/s00259-014-2708-8
12 Bajjalieh SM, Frantz GD, Weimann JM, et al. Differential expression of synaptic vesicle protein 2 (SV2) isoforms. J Neurosci 1994; 14: 5223–35. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-09-05223.1994
13 Finnema SJ, Nabulsi NB, Mercier J, et al. Kinetic evaluation and test-retest reproducibility of [11C]UCB-J, a novel radioligand for positron emission tomography imaging of synaptic vesicle glycoprotein 2A in humans. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2018; 38: 2041–52. doi:10.1177/0271678X17724947
14 Angarita GA, Worhunsky PD, Naganawa M, et al. Lower prefrontal cortical synaptic vesicle binding in cocaine use disorder: an exploratory (11) C-UCB-J positron emission tomography study in humans. Addict Biol 2022; 27: e13123. doi:10.1111/adb.13123
15 D’Souza DC, Radhakrishnan R, Naganawa M, et al. Preliminary in vivo evidence of lower hippocampal synaptic density in cannabis use disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2021; 26: 3192–200. doi:10.1038/s41380-020-00891-4
16 Holmes SE, Scheinost D, Finnema SJ, et al. Lower synaptic density is associated with depression severity and network alterations. Nat Commun 2019; 10: 1529. doi:10.1038/s41467-019-09562-7
17 Naganawa M, Li S, Nabulsi N, et al. First-in-human evaluation of (18)F-Synvest-1, a radioligand for PET imaging of synaptic vesicle glycoprotein 2A. J Nucl Med 2021; 62: 561–7. doi:10.2967/jnumed.120.249144
18 Wang Z, Wu L, Yuan K, et al. Cortical thickness and volume abnormalities in internet gaming disorder: evidence from comparison of recreational internet game users. Eur J Neurosci 2018. doi:10.1111/ejn.13987. [Epub ahead of print: 2018 Jun 8]
19 Wang L, Yang G, Zheng Y, et al. Neural substrates of deficient cognitive control in individuals with severe internet gaming disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical 2021; 32: 102828. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102828
20 Nystrom LE, Braver TS, Sabb FW, et al. Working memory for letters, shapes, and locations: fMRI evidence against stimulus-based regional organization in human prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 2000; 11: 424–46. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0572
21 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 22 May 2013. doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
22 Qin L, Cheng L, Hu M, et al. Clarification of the cut-off score for nine-item Internet Gaming Disorder Scale-Short Form (IGDS9-SF) in a Chinese context. Front Psychiatry 2020; 11: 470. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00470
23 Severo RB, Barbosa A, Fouchy DRC, et al. Development and psychometric validation of Internet Gaming Disorder Scale-Short-Form (IGDS9-SF) in a Brazilian sample. Addict Behav 2020; 103: 106191. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106191
24 Chen IH, Strong C, Lin YC, et al. Time invariance of three ultra-brief internet-related instruments: Smartphone Application-Based Addiction Scale (SABAS), Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS), and the nine-item Internet Gaming Disorder Scale- Short Form (IGDS-SF9) (Study Part B). Addict Behav 2020; 101: 105960. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.04.018
25 Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23: 56–62. doi:10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56
26 Zheng YP, Zhao JP, Phillips M, et al. Validity and reliability of the Chinese Hamilton Depression Rating Scale. Br J Psychiatry 1988; 152: 660–4. doi:10.1192/bjp.152.5.660
27 Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. Br J Med Psychol 1959; 32: 50–5. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1959.tb00467.x
28 Lee YJ, Lee QY, Zhang Y, et al. Reliability of HDRS and HARS telepsychiatric assessment of anxiety and depression. Taiwan J Psychiatry 1998; 12: 236–42.
29 Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol 1995; 51: 768–74. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1
30 Li CR, Chen S-H. Obsessive-compulsiveness and impulsivity in a non-clinical population of adolescent males and females. Psychiatry Res 2007; 149: 129–38. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2006.05.001
31 Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1984; 10: 276–91. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.10.2.276
32 Lipszyc J, Schachar R. Inhibitory control and psychopathology: a meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010; 16: 1064–76. doi:10.1017/S1355617710000895
33 Golden CJ. Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Chicago, IL: Stoelting Co, 1978.
34 Gevins A, Cutillo B. Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working memory. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1993; 87: 128–43. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(93)90119-g
35 Tang Y, Liu P, Li W, et al. Detection of changes in synaptic density in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis patients using (18) F-Synvest-1 positron emission tomography. Eur J Neurol 2022; 29: 2934–43. doi:10.1111/ene.15451
36 Rossano S, Toyonaga T, Finnema SJ, et al. Assessment of a white matter reference region for (11)C-UCB-J PET quantification. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2020; 40: 1890–901.
37 Park HS, Kim SH, Bang SA, et al. Altered regional cerebral glucose metabolism in internet game overusers: a 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography study. CNS Spectr 2010; 15: 159–66. doi:10.1017/s1092852900027437
38 Lee D, Namkoong K, Lee J, et al. Dorsal striatal functional connectivity changes in internet gaming disorder: a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Addict Biol 2021; 26: e12868. doi:10.1111/adb.12868
39 Schettler L, Thomasius R, Paschke K. Neural correlates of problematic gaming in adolescents: a systematic review of structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Addict Biol 2022; 27: e13093. doi:10.1111/adb.13093
40 Weinstein A. Problematic internet usage: brain imaging findings. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2022; 47: 101209. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101209
41 Balodis IM, Kober H, Worhunsky PD, et al. Diminished frontostriatal activity during processing of monetary rewards and losses in pathological gambling. Biol Psychiatry 2012; 71: 749–57. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.006
42 Wang M, Dong H, Zheng H, et al. Inhibitory neuromodulation of the putamen to the prefrontal cortex in internet gaming disorder: how addiction impairs executive control. J Behav Addict 2020; 9: 312–24. doi:10.1556/2006.2020.00029
43 Kober H, Lacadie CM, Wexler BE, et al. Brain activity during cocaine craving and gambling urges: an fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016; 41: 628–37. doi:10.1038/npp.2015.193
44 Hong S-B, Harrison BJ, Dandash O, et al. A selective involvement of putamen functional connectivity in youth with internet gaming disorder. Brain Res 2015; 1602: 85–95. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2014.12.042
45 Dong GH, Wang M, Zhang J, et al. Functional neural changes and altered cortical-subcortical connectivity associated with recovery from internet gaming disorder. J Behav Addict 2019; 8: 692–702. doi:10.1556/2006.8.2019.75
46 Kim SH, Baik SH, Park CS, et al. Reduced striatal dopamine D2 receptors in people with internet addiction. Neuroreport 2011; 22: 407–11. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e328346e16e
47 Kim SK, Jeong H, Im JJ, et al. PET hypometabolism of the prefrontal-cingulate cortices in internet gaming disorder. Front Psychiatry 2020; 11: 566518. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2020.566518
48 Zhang M, Gao X, Yang Z, et al. Shared gray matter alterations in subtypes of addiction: a voxel-wise meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2021; 238: 2365–79. doi:10.1007/s00213-021-05920-w
49 Han X, Wu X, Wang Y, et al. Alterations of resting-state static and dynamic functional connectivity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in subjects with internet gaming disorder. Front Hum Neurosci 2018; 12: 41. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2018.00041
50 Weinstein A, Lejoyeux M. Neurobiological mechanisms underlying internet gaming disorder. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2020; 22: 113–26. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2020.22.2/aweinstein
51 Zheng H, Hu Y, Wang Z, et al. Meta-analyses of the functional neural alterations in subjects with internet gaming disorder: similarities and differences across different paradigms. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2019; 94: 109656. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109656
52 Zhang P, Pan Y, Zha R, et al. Impulsivity-related right superior frontal gyrus as a biomarker of internet gaming disorder. Gen Psychiatr 2023; 36: e100985. doi:10.1136/gpsych-2022-100985
53 Hu S, Li C-SR. Neural processes of preparatory control for stop signal inhibition. Hum Brain Mapp 2012; 33: 2785–96. doi:10.1002/hbm.21399
54 Chevrier A, Schachar RJ. Error detection in the stop signal task. Neuroimage 2010; 53: 664–73. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.056
55 Wu L-L, Potenza MN, Zhou N, et al. Efficacy of single-session transcranial direct current stimulation on addiction-related inhibitory control and craving: a randomized trial in males with internet gaming disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2021; 46: E111–8. doi:10.1503/jpn.190137
56 Fu R, Yuan C, Sun W, et al. Effectiveness of E-SBIRT intervention in community healthcare institution in China: study proposal for a randomised controlled trial. Gen Psychiatr 2021; 34: e100486. doi:10.1136/gpsych-2021-100486
57 Onwordi EC, Halff EF, Whitehurst T, et al. Synaptic density marker SV2A is reduced in schizophrenia patients and unaffected by antipsychotics in rats. Nat Commun 2020; 11: 246. doi:10.1038/s41467-019-14122-0
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2023. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Background
Internet gaming disorder (IGD) is an ideal model to study the mechanisms underlying synaptic deficits in addiction as it eliminates the confounding effects of substance use. Synaptic loss and deficits are hypothesised to underlie the enduring maladaptive behaviours and impaired cognitive function that contribute to IGD.
Aims
This study aimed to determine whether subjects with IGD have lower synaptic density than control subjects and the relationship between synaptic density and IGD severity.
Methods
Eighteen unmedicated subjects diagnosed with current IGD according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition criteria and 16 demographically matched healthy controls (HCs) participated in the study and underwent 18F-labelled difluoro-analogue of UCB-J (18F-SynVesT-1) positron emission tomography scans to assess the density of synaptic vesicle glycoprotein 2A (SV2A). The Internet Gaming Disorder Scale-Short Form (IGDS9-SF), Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAMD), Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAMA), Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11 (BIS-11), Stroop Colour-Word Test (SCWT), stop-signal paradigms and N-back tasks were administered to all subjects.
Results
Patients with IGD had significantly higher scores on the IGDS9-SF, HAMD, HAMA and BIS-11 than HCs. HCs performed better on the two-back and SCWT tests as well as in terms of stop-signal reaction times (SSRTs) in the stop-signal paradigms than patients with IGD. Lower uptake was found in the bilateral putamen, right pregenual anterior cingulate cortex and Rolandic operculum of patients with IGD compared with HCs. Furthermore, in the IGD group, IGDS9-SF scores and daily gaming hours were negatively correlated with the standardised uptake value ratios of 18F-SynVesT-1 in the bilateral putamen. Longer SSRTs were significantly associated with lower SV2A density in the right pregenual anterior cingulate cortex and right Rolandic operculum.
Conclusions
The in vivo results in this study suggest that lower synaptic density contributes to the severity and impairments in inhibitory control of IGD. These findings may provide further incentive to evaluate interventions that restore synaptic transmission and plasticity to treat IGD.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details


1 Department of Nuclear Medicine, Central South University, Changsha, Hunan, China
2 Mental Health Center of Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, Hunan, China
3 Department of Applied Psychology, Hunan University of Chinese Medicine, Changsha, Hunan, China
4 Department of Substance Use and Addictive Behaviors Disorders, Shanghai Mental Health Center, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China
5 Shanghai Mental Health Center, Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine, Shanghai, China; Department of Internal Medicine, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, USA
6 Department of Psychology, The First Affiliated Hospital of Nanchang University, Nanchang, Jiangxi, China
7 Department of Nuclear Medicine, Central South University, Changsha, Hunan, China; National Clinical Research Center for Geriatric Disorders (Xiangya), Changsha, Hunan, China