It appears you don't have support to open PDFs in this web browser. To view this file, Open with your PDF reader
Abstract
Many songbirds learn to produce songs through vocal practice in early life and continue to sing daily throughout their lifetime. While it is well-known that adult songbirds sing as part of their mating rituals, the functions of singing behavior outside of reproductive contexts remain unclear. Here, we investigated this issue in adult male zebra finches by suppressing their daily singing for two weeks and examining the effects on song performance. We found that singing suppression decreased the pitch, amplitude, and duration of songs, and that those song features substantially recovered through subsequent free singing. These reversible song changes were not dependent on auditory feedback or the age of the birds, contrasting with the adult song plasticity that has been reported previously. These results demonstrate that adult song structure is not stable without daily singing, and suggest that adult songbirds maintain song performance by preventing song changes through physical act of daily singing throughout their life. Such daily singing likely functions as vocal training to maintain the song production system in optimal conditions for song performance in reproductive contexts, similar to how human singers and athletes practice daily to maintain their performance.
This study demonstrates that adult zebra finches maintain song performance by preventing song changes through physical act of daily singing throughout their life.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details



1 Korea Brain Research Institute, Sensory and Motor Systems Research Group, Daegu, Republic of Korea (GRID:grid.452628.f) (ISNI:0000 0004 5905 0571)
2 Korea Brain Research Institute, Sensory and Motor Systems Research Group, Daegu, Republic of Korea (GRID:grid.452628.f) (ISNI:0000 0004 5905 0571); Johns Hopkins University, Department of Biomedical Engineering, School of Medicine, Baltimore, USA (GRID:grid.21107.35) (ISNI:0000 0001 2171 9311)
3 University of Tokyo, Department of Life Sciences, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, Tokyo, Japan (GRID:grid.26999.3d) (ISNI:0000 0001 2151 536X); Teikyo University, Faculty of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Department of Life and Health Sciences, Tokyo, Japan (GRID:grid.264706.1) (ISNI:0000 0000 9239 9995)
4 Hokkaido University, Department of Biological Sciences, Faculty of Science, Sapporo, Japan (GRID:grid.39158.36) (ISNI:0000 0001 2173 7691)