Introduction
Personality remains a salient and pervasive construct that can be helpful in understanding consistent individual differences in humans and other animals [1–8]. Despite the interest in nonhuman primate personality [4], 61% of studies prior to 2010 were only representative of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) or chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) [4]. After 2010, still only 52% of studies were represented by five species [7]. Additionally, the environmental condition of primate personality study subjects is predominantly captive, with wild subjects represented in only 9% of studies prior to 2010 [4], with some improvement to 25% since 2010 [7]. Addressing these gaps in phylogenetic diversity and environmental condition is important for understanding the evolution, biobehavioral expression, and function of personality. This deeper understanding of comparative personality could address questions about the processes of social evolution [6] (i.e., how does individual variation differ across species, and along which axes?), but is also expected to improve wild animal welfare and health outcomes, especially in environments where wildlife live near or frequently interact with humans [9–11] (i.e., are particular individuals, population, or species more susceptible to human impacts due to axes of individual variation or constraints?–a point we explore below).
For wildlife living in dynamic and challenging anthropogenic environments, inter-individual variation in, for example, sociodemographic characteristics or socio-spatial behavior has been shown to influence the relative propensity for risk-taking [12, 13]. Such wildlife living in synanthropy or sympatry with humans are often identified as ‘invasive’ and have been associated with high aggression, boldness, and activity [9, 14]. For example, heightened exploratory tendencies in Barbary ground squirrels (Atlantoxerus getulus) have been posited to be advantageous during island invasions, providing benefits in predator poor areas [15, 16]. Furthermore, such suites of traits have also been posited to generally increase in urban populations [17, 18]. In a social animal, individual tendencies can alter group- or population-level composition, and thereby influence which personality types succeed in a group [19–21]. These processes are complicated, however, in that individual animals and species show variation in the likelihood of persisting amidst, and taking risks during, heightened human proximity [12–14]. Thus, understanding the underlying personality structure across- and within-species is important from an applied animal behavior perspective; fundamental research on human-wildlife interactions would be advanced through a nuanced understanding of why individual animals, populations, or species are resilient to human encroachment, while others are more vulnerable.
Addressing these gaps, we explored personality structure in situ in ten wild groups of urban or urban-rural nonhuman primates, across three different species: rhesus macaques, bonnet macaques (M. radiata), and long-tailed macaques (M. fascicularis). Relative to captive populations, wild populations are important given evidence of greater variation in reactive physiology [22] and in the phenotypic expression of personality traits [23, 24]. We focused on free-living populations of these species within their natural ranges that also overlapped with humans and anthropogenic landscapes to varying extents. The genus Macaca was well suited for our objectives, due to its historically prolonged synanthropic associations with people at human-wildlife interfaces [25–27]. Furthermore, macaques have been shown to vary in social tolerance, which has historically led to researchers grouping them into ‘grades’ of social style [28, 29], although the latter is now understood to be more of a ‘continuum’ [30, 31]. At the interspecific level, these social styles have been shown to be broadly (albeit somewhat inconsistently) influenced by phylogenetic relatedness. For instance, degrees of social tolerance among female long-tailed macaques (that form the core of macaque groups) are expected to be relatively intermediate to the degrees of tolerance present among the more socially despotic rhesus macaques and the more tolerant bonnet macaques [28, 29]. This framework is complicated, however, by a less clear effect of phylogenetic co-variation in male-male tolerance, with evidence of the opposite pattern [32]. Bonnet macaques diverged from a shared ancestor 3.5 million years ago and so are more distantly related to long-tailed and rhesus macaques that together form a monophyletic group to which bonnet macaques are basal [33]. Finally, bonnet macaques have lower instances of interactions with humans (specifically human-directed aggression) in many parts of their range, compared to rhesus and long-tailed macaques [12, 34, 35].
Although our study is exploratory in nature, it is embedded within a robust body of supporting literature. Specifically, studies with an explicit comparative personality rating design have included: Assamese macaques (M. assamensis), Barbary macaques (M. sylvanus), Japanese macaques (M. fuscata), rhesus macaques, Sulawesi black crested macaques (M. nigra), and Tonkean macaques (M. tonkeana) [36–38]. Furthermore, we acknowledge a diversity of personality assessments on other macaques species [39–48].
Findings from comparative approaches can offer critical insights into shared and divergent personality structure. For instance, Adams et al. [36] analyzed ratings data from personality assessments on six macaque species. Their questionnaire was based off the human personality Five Factor Model [49–51], which is defined by the factors of Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Openness, and Neuroticism. They found Friendliness (representing facets of Extraversion and Agreeableness) across all six species of Macaca and Openness across five species [36, 48]. They also found variation in personality structure such that species with a despotic social style clustered more closely together relative to the remaining social styles [36]. Similarly, Sussman et al. [47] reported that the despotic rhesus macaques exhibited greater scores on an aggressiveness component, and had aggressive behaviors contribute to all behaviorally coded components, relative to long-tailed macaques.
We also aimed to explore how human presence might mediate trait expression. We included this aim due to known variance in risk-taking behaviors attributed to anthropogenic effects [12–14]. While nonhuman personality ratings are posited to homogenize variation across contexts [4], there is a robust literature that suggests situations can alter the expression of personality in humans and animals [5, 52–54]. Indeed, individual differences in rhesus macaques’ Sociability interact with social housing situations to influence disease susceptibility [55, 56]. Variation of personality trait expression in response to distinct situations has been poorly described in the wild. Evidence from wild Tibetan macaques (M. thibetana), however, suggests that behaviors relevant to Insecurity, Boldness, and Sociability personality factors exhibit situational variance–contingent on the presence-absence of tourists and provisioning [42]. Similarly, comparisons between personality factor structures from two populations of Barbary macaques found evidence of an Opportunism factor in the provisioned population [40, 46]. Also in Barbary macaques, Baker et al. [37] identified a human-animal sociability factor that was associated with latency to touch a novel object. This factor was posited to be due to a reduced fear of items provisioned by human caregivers. Similarly, exposure trials to human testers found significant cross-species differences in a ‘Sociability towards Humans’ factor whereby long-tailed macaques exhibited negative scores, rhesus macaques were intermediate, and pig-tailed macaques (M. nemestrina) exhibited positive scores [47].
Due to the aforementioned evidence of such variance [37, 47], we included ratings on how individual macaques varied in response to humans. Human personality research has utilized assessments that draw upon experiences during specific situations [57, 58]. These assessments of specific situations provide a powerful tool to examine whether and which personality traits vary across situations. Thus, we sought to compare how personality factors from interactions with conspecifics, differed from personality factors in situations defined by interactions by macaques with humans.
We sought to accomplish three aims. Across three species of wild macaques living in anthropogenic urban or urban-rural environments, (1) we compared personality structure across the three species, we utilized the same rating system, facilitating quantitative and qualitative comparisons. The use of the same methodology has been emphasized as a priority in comparative personality work on wild animals [46]. (2) We compared situation-specific differences in personality structure. To this end, we developed personality ratings constrained to situations involving interactions between humans and our subjects. (3) Finally, we compared individual scores in the resulting factor models from aim 1 and aim 2. Although this comparative study was largely exploratory, we predicted that personality structure would exhibit greater structural similarity across species consistent with their relative similarities in species-typical social styles and phylogenetic relatedness. That is, long-tailed macaques would structurally fall between the more phylogenetically similar rhesus macaques and the more phylogenetically basal bonnet macaques–such intermediate placement would be attributable to their phylogenetic relatedness, but also their relative social style. Similarly, we expected that species which occupied more urbanized areas (i.e., long-tailed and rhesus macaques, relative to bonnet macaques) would exhibit personality structures more strongly defined by aggressive, bold, and active traits [9, 14].
Methods
Site and study groups
We studied adult and sub-adult monkeys from three species at three different sites: rhesus macaques at Shimla, India (31.05° N, 77.1° E), long-tailed macaques at Batu Caves and Templer Park, Malaysia (3.23° N, 101.7° E and 3.29° N, 101.6° E, respectively), and bonnet macaques near Thenmala, India (8.90° N, 77.10° E). The bonnet macaques were observed nearby to a small town (Thenmala) and had reduced access to anthropogenic food compared to the long-tailed and rhesus macaques that occupied sites near cities (Kuala Lumpur and Shimla, respectively) [59]. The parks that the long-tailed macaques occupied, despite geographic proximity, had marked differences in the number of tourist visitors per day [60]. Behavioral data collection was conducted between 2016–2018. We obtained ratings from a total of 328 wild subjects. Group and sex breakdowns are available in Table 1 with further site and subject details available elsewhere [12, 59, 61, 62].
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Survey
We used a personality survey previously published in Maninger et al. [63]. Their survey was assembled from published nonhuman personality studies available at the time [44, 50, 64, 65] in addition to item contributions from animal husbandry technicians. These surveys were originally constructed primarily via a behavioral bottom-up approach for items (i.e., derived from adjectives and definitions that describe observable behavior of the relevant species) [66]; definitions were constructed or modified with careful consideration of the study species and circumstances. The Maninger et al.’s [63] survey included 50 items. We modified the surveys as follows. First, we omitted 5 items: cooperative, flexible/not rigid, helpful, imaginative/creative, and imitative. Then, because the present study included females, we added 6 items and their associated definitions from Stevenson-Hinde et al. [44]. These items were apprehensive, effective, equable, sensitive, sociable, and strong. Finally, we added the item feisty, with a descriptor of: “having or showing lively aggressiveness; spunky.”
Sites differed in the number of observers who contributed: five observers rated rhesus macaques, four rated long-tailed macaques, and five rated the bonnet macaques. Raters were specific to each site and, within each site, not every observer rated every subject. Raters were extensively trained on the survey tool by EBM to ensure they understood the meaning of each adjective, could think of examples of behaviors related to low/average/high ratings of each adjective (without referencing specific animals familiar to observers), and understood the difference between ratings being made of the animals in general versus the animals in human context. Scenarios were presented, without referencing specific animals, to aid in defining what would constitute high and low scores. This process required back-and-forth translation between English (the language of the ratings) and the languages that were spoken by the members of the data collection teams (all raters were fluent in English). EBM engaged in an iterative process at each site to ensure everyone was trained in the same way and understood the protocols. Subjects were the same between the general and human situation ratings, though one long-tailed macaque subject was missing a rating for sociable in the human situation.
All field observers who completed the surveys were familiar with all individual animals; they collected data on activity budgets and social behavior using focal animal sampling of individual animals [67] for 18–20 months in the rhesus and long-tailed macaques and 11 months in the bonnet macaques [12, 59, 61, 62]. During these data collection periods, observers were asked to rate each subject on adjectives with associated descriptions, and to limit discussion about the rating of specific monkeys. Raters were asked to skip subjects that they were not confident that they knew. Ratings were collected after all behavioral data were collected and not at the locale at which behavioral data were collected; ratings were based on subjects in general, rather than particular instances of behavior. Eight items were excluded from the human situation ratings: jealous, manipulative, nurturant, playful, popular, protective, stingy/greedy, and warm/affectionate. These adjectives were excluded during the training process prior to scoring individual animals after the team was unable to generate specific examples of behavior relating to the adjectives. Observers completed the general and human situation ratings in parallel.
Despite the extensive training prior to ratings, at one of the sites EBM observed conversation between raters during their completion of human situation ratings (but not the general ratings). This observation suggests that ratings that sample were potentially not independent, which we inferred to be due to a greater difficulty in completing human situation ratings. Personal communications (December 8, 2023) with multiple raters from other sites suggests that this was not a uniform occurrence. Thus, we have retained these data.
Data processing and analyses
All data processing and analyses were conducted using R (v4.2.2) [68]. We assessed inter-rater reliability for each of the items using intraclass correlations via the ICC() function in the psych package [69]. As our aim was to aggregate ratings across raters, we relied on ICC(3,k) following guidelines and underlying rationale detailed elsewhere [70–74]. We also report ICC(3,1). The two estimates are mathematically similar [71, 75], though their interpretation differs [70, 75]. ICC(3,k) assesses the reliability of a composite for use with mean ratings [71, 73, 74], while ICC(3,1) is important for individual rater reliabilities [71, 73]. We dropped items that had an a priori cut-off of ICC(3,k) ≤ 0.40. A cut-off of 0.40 follows published recommendations for scales [76]. Though single items are expected to have lower reliabilities than scales [77], ICC(3,k) has higher estimates than ICC(3,1) [78] with measurement error reduced through the process of averaging ratings [78]. Generally, however, ICC cut-offs range across studies with researchers identifying liberal cut-offs of ICC > 0.00, while others are more conservative (i.e., ICC[3,k] > 0.50 or > 0.70) [e.g., 72, 77, 79, 80]. We replicated our analyses to verify that our reliability threshold or use of ICC(3,k) was not overly informing our model outcomes.
We conducted factor analyses independently for each of the three species using fa() function within the psych package [69]. The process was identical for each dataset. We chose to analyze each species separately rather than pool the datasets, primarily because we had no a priori rationale to assume that factor structure would be identical across the three species.
For each factor model, we first averaged raters’ scores across each subject, so that every monkey only had a single rating for each item. We utilized a similar model selection procedure across species to facilitate comparability. We followed recommendations for assessing the number of factors to extract and model fit [69, 81, 82]. First, we extracted Kaiser, Meyer, Olkin Measures of Sampling Adequacy (MSA) for each item and discarded items < 0.50. We then used parallel analysis, empirical BIC, and very simple structure (complexity of 1) to interrogate the probable number of factors. If there was a discrepancy between these estimates, we took the median number of factors. We removed any items whose communality–a measure of the proportion of item variance explained by the factor model–was ≤ 0.4 or ≥ 0.99. We then confirmed each item had an MSA ≥ 0.50 and that the resulting models’ square root of the sum of squared correlation residuals was < 0.10. We relied on minimum residuals solutions and used oblique (oblimin) transformations throughout. We ran this process for each of the three species’ general and human situation ratings, resulting in six factor models. We selected factor names from heavily loading items within each factor, with some consideration of whether there were comparable factors in the other species.
Comparing factor models.
Raters were asked to mean-center their ratings; thus, even without scaling, comparisons across datasets are not as straightforward as analyzing average ratings across the models. Therefore, we relied on comparisons of structural equivalence through two methods of comparison: 1) obtaining Pearson’s correlation and Tucker’s congruence coefficients from the factor loadings for items present in species’ factor models, 2) calculating fuzzy set intersections for all the items present in any of the species’ factor models. The former approach provides insight as to whether factors show quantitatively similar loadings, and is conducted pairwise between the species. The fuzzy set analysis, instead, assumes a similar factor structure across all species providing insight into the intersection of composite factor items. That is, these fuzzy set analyses are important for examining which items are likely to be representative of these factors across all three species, rather than defining factor structure within a species.
To obtain correlation and congruence coefficients, we followed recommendations [83] to performed pairwise Procrustes rotations (using the Procrustes() function [69]) between the species using their loading matrices. Loading matrices were restricted to items present in both species’ models. We then calculated congruence coefficients using the fa.congruence() function and correlation coefficients using the cor() function in the psych [69] and stats [68] R packages. We recognize standards of a consensus-derived cut-off of 0.85 for congruent versus incongruent set within a single study [83], but that 0.80 has been posited to be an agreeable cut-off across studies [84]. We are foremost focused on similarity, rather than absolute congruence–even so, we use fair 0.85–0.94 and good congruence (0.95–1.00), and a hard cut-off for congruence of 0.80. A score of 0.80 was intermediate between fair and poor [83]. For transparency, we use poor congruence, following the five point scale bins used by Lorenzo-Seva and ten Berge [83], but note that their point estimate of poor was approximated by 0.72. The use of 0.80 is not without precedent [84] in, for example, cross-cultural work [85]. This caveat is relevant here because we assessed subjects across species boundaries and raters themselves were cross-cultural.
Fuzzy set intersections [86, 87] were calculated using R code provided in Adams et al. [36]. Thus, we examined which items intersected with factors that were structurally similar across species. For example, if we found a dimension such as Extraversion across three species, we might be interested in which items showed the greatest support across the three groups for heavily contributing to Extraversion. Intersections were functionally equivalent to the minimum absolute loading for valid items in any given factor’s fuzzy set. Thus, in the prior example, if species’ loadings for the item sociable within the factor Extraversion were 0.12, 0.25, 0.76, then the fuzzy intersection for sociable would be 0.12 for Extraversion. This provides a conservative estimate of concordance for items that compose factors across multiple species. That is, fuzzy intersections represent the minimum estimate of mutually shared space. For items that are not present due to lack of reliability in one or more species, the estimate is drawn from known information (i.e., the loadings from species that had the items as reliable). Before running fuzzy set analyses, we reverse scored item loadings such that factors were directionally consistent across species.
As fuzzy intersection scores are drawn from the loadings themselves, they range from -1 to 1 per item, and can be interpreted similarly to factor loadings [36]. We followed a similar approach to that reported elsewhere to determining which items were salient for a fuzzy set [36]. We conducted 1000 permutations, where we selected a random factor from each species for each iteration. We extracted all the fuzzy item scores for each randomly extracted fuzzy set. We then took the 95th percentile of the absolute value of all permuted iterations. We conducted the same procedure within the personality factor models and the human factor models and then aggregated the fuzzy item scores. The 95th percentile for these scores was 0.43. To confirm neither the personality factor models nor the human situation models were overly influential towards this cut-off, we calculated the 95th percentile within each (0.36 and 0.49, respectively). This cut-off was not overly dissimilar from 0.40, which is frequently used as meaningful for factor loadings. Thus, we simplified our judgement for salient fuzzy set scores to a threshold of ≥|0.40|.
Finally, though the average item ratings showed high association between the two survey results, we recognize that factor analyses may result in the emergence of latent structures that are distinct between the two datasets. Thus, we compared subjects’ factors scores from the general factors to those from the human situation ratings. To compare the factor structures from the general (conspecific) personality ratings and the human situation personality ratings, we computed factor scores for each subject using ten Berge’s estimation [88]. Then, for each of the taxa, we computed Pearson’s correlation coefficients across the factor scores in the general versus human situations. As previously noted, one long-tailed macaque subject was excluded from these comparisons due to a missing rating in the human situation.
Results
General conspecific personality ratings
The following items fell below our a priori interrater reliability cut-off and were omitted: reckless, sensitive, unemotional, and vigilant for rhesus macaques; excitable, sensitive, and unpredictable for long-tailed macaques; and fearful, independent, insecure, sensitive, tolerant, and vigilant for bonnet macaques (Table 2; S1 Table in S1 File). The mean average for retained items was 0.68 ±0.12sd for ICC(3,k) and 0.34 ±0.14sd for ICC(3,1) (S2 Table in S1 File). Replicated analyses with varying ICC cut-offs verified that our reliability threshold did not overly influence our model outcomes (S1 Text; S1 Fig; S3 Table in S1 File).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Criteria of item exclusion are annotated. Factors ordered by rhesus macaque structure.
Several items were excluded due to low communality. For the rhesus macaques, we excluded eccentric and tense due to low communality. For the long-tailed macaques, we excluded eccentric, intelligent, and vigilant due to low communality. Finally, for the bonnet macaques, no additional items were excluded.
From the original 51 items, we retained 45 for five factor solutions in the long-tailed and rhesus macaque models, and 45 for a four-factor solution in the bonnet macaque model. For the factor names, we have appended a superscript for each of the relevant species (‘R’ for rhesus macaques, ‘L’ for long-tailed, and ‘B’ for bonnet). The rhesus macaque dataset supported a five-factor solution (Table 2; S5 Table in S1 File), with the factors explaining 73% of the cumulative variation in the data. These factors were named as follows, with the proportion of variation explained in parentheses: IrritableR (0.20); ConfidentR (0.20); SociableR (0.13); ActiveR (0.10); and EquableR (0.10). The long-tailed macaque dataset supported a five-factor solution (Table 2; S6 Table in S1 File), with the factors explaining 76% of the cumulative variation in the data. Factor names and the proportion of variation explained were: ConfidentL (0.26); SociableL (0.16); Irritable/EquableL (0.16); ActiveL (0.12); and PlayfulL (0.06). The bonnet macaque dataset supported a four-factor solution (Table 2; S7 Table in S1 File), with the factors explaining 75% of the cumulative variation in the data. Factor names and the proportion of variation explained were: ConfidentB (0.32); ActiveB (0.16); SociableB (0.16); and EquableB (0.11).
The models had low root mean square residuals (RMSR) with a degrees of freedom correction (0.04, 0.03, and 0.03 for the bonnet, long-tailed, and rhesus macaque models, respectively), and were all below 0.08 indicating acceptable goodness-of-fit. Alpha and total omega estimates were high across the three models (≥ 0.95). Communalities were high, with means of 0.73 ±0.11sd for rhesus, 0.76 ±0.13sd for long-tailed, and 0.75 ±0.12sd for bonnet macaques. We present interfactor correlation coefficients in the supplementary (S8 Table in S1 File). Our model structures relative to item and subject sample sizes were sufficient based on screening recommendations for communality values, item:factor ratios, and sample size [89].
The final models retained several complex items (i.e., items with moderate-to-heavy loadings across multiple factors). Construction of simple models with sequential omission of complex items were highly similar in their factor loadings and resulting structure (S2 Text; S9 Table in S1 File). Additionally, lower reliability cut-offs (i.e., ICC[3,1] ≤ 0.10 or ≤ 0.00) did not alter our model structures (S1 Text; S1 Fig; S3 Table in S1 File). Thus, we proceeded with full models containing complex items to prioritize item retention and comparability between the structures.
Species comparisons of general conspecific factor models.
To compare structures, we first merged factor loading matrices by pairs for all three of the factor models and conducted cross-correlations on items present in all models (Table 3). We observed high cross-correlations in ActiveRBL, ConfidentRBL, and SociableRBL (whereby superscripts signify the species for which each factor is present with ‘RBL’ indicating rhesus, bonnet, and long-tailed macaques) evidencing structural comparability across the taxa, with an average |r| of 0.90. We also found high comparability for two factors, Equable and Irritable, with an average |r| between factor loadings of 0.86.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
We found a general pattern of fair congruence across ActiveRBL, ConfidentRBL, and SociableRBL, with the exception of poor congruence for SociableBL (Table 3). Equable and Irritable exhibited more complex pairwise dynamics between the three species. We found good congruence between IrritableR and Irritable/EquableL, as well as fair congruence between EquableR and PlayfulL. Both IrritableR and Irritable/EquableL had fair congruence with EquableB, Both EquableR and PlayfulL were incongruent with all the bonnet macaque factors.
We also completed fuzzy set analyses on five factors that were consistent across the three species (ActiveRBL, ConfidentRBL, SociableRBL, and IrritableRL and/or EquableRBL) (Table 4 and S10 Table in S1 File). As the item composition and presence of Irritable and Equable varied across species, we treated these as distinct factors with the understanding that the bonnet macaque factor structure was more heavily represented by Equable-like items, the rhesus macaque model had two distinct factors (Irritable and Equable), and the long-tailed macaque factor was composed of items from both factors (Irritable/Equable). We found that each of the five fuzzy sets had four or more items with values ≥ |0.40| and, therefore, were likely representative of major factors across species. We appended a superscript ‘F’ to distinguish fuzzy sets from factors. Using a threshold of ≥ |0.40|: ConfidentF included apprehensive, bold, cautious, confident, direct/forceful/gets own way, effective, fearful, insecure, intelligent, nervous/anxious/not calm, strong, submissive/subordinate, and timid. SociableF included affiliative/companionable, eccentric, independent, nurturant, popular, sociable, solitary, and warm/affectionate. ActiveF included active/energetic, curious/exploratory/inquisitive, impulsive, lazy, reckless, and slow. IrritableF included bullying, excitable, feisty, irritable, and jealous. EquableF included calm/equable, equable, gentle, and tolerant. We found evidence for similar item placement in each of these five factors, though we underscore the lack of distinct factors between Irritable and Equable across all three species.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Items are represented in all species models, unless otherwise indicated with superscript of the species where items are in the model (annotated with BLR for bonnet, long-tailed, or rhesus macaques, respectively). See S10 Table in S1 File for the remaining items.
Human situation personality ratings
The following items fell below our a priori reliability cut-off and were omitted: affiliative/companionable, sensitive, submissive/subordinate, and tense for rhesus macaques; affiliative/companionable, aggressive, bold, calm/equable, eccentric, equable, excitable, feisty, gentle, impulsive, independent, insecure, intelligent, irritable, sensitive, sociable, solitary, strong, tolerant, unemotional, unpredictable, and vigilant for bonnet macaques (Table 5; S11 Table in S1 File). For long-tailed macaques, none of the remaining items were excluded based on ICC(3,k). The mean average for reliable items was 0.63 ±0.12sd for ICC(3,k) and 0.29 ±0.12sd for ICC(3,1) (S12 Table in S1 File). Replicated analyses with varying ICC cut-offs verified that our reliability threshold did not overly influence our model outcomes (S1 Text; S1 Fig; S4 Table in S1 File).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Criteria of item exclusion are annotated. Factors ordered by rhesus macaque structure.
Several items were also excluded due to low MSA or low communality. For the rhesus macaques, we excluded sociable and solitary due to low communality. For the long-tailed macaques, we excluded eccentric and sensitive, due to low communality. For the bonnet macaques, we excluded understanding and fearful for low communality.
From the original 51 items, we retained 37 in a four-factor rhesus macaque model, 41 in a four-factor long-tailed macaque model, and 19 in a three-factor bonnet macaque model. For the factor names, we appended a superscript for each of the relevant species (‘R’ for rhesus macaques, ‘L’ for long-tailed, and ‘B’ for bonnet) as well as a subscript (H for human situation) to distinguish these factors from the general factors. The rhesus macaque dataset supported a four-factor solution (Table 5; S13 Table in S1 File), with the factors explaining 72% of the variation in the data. Factor names and the proportion of variation explained were: IrritableRH, 0.25; ExploratoryRH, 0.20; ApprehensiveRH, 0.20; and LazyRH, 0.07. The long-tailed macaque dataset supported a four-factor solution (Table 5; S14 Table in S1 File), with the factors explaining 71% of the variation in the data. Factor names and the proportion of variation explained were: ApprehensiveLH, 0.25; ExploratoryLH, 0.20; IrritableLH, 0.18; and LazyLH, 0.08. The bonnet macaque dataset supported a three-factor solution (Table 5; S15 Table in S1 File), with the factors explaining 73% of the variation in the data. Factor names and the proportion of variation explained were: EffectiveBH, 0.34; Lazy/ExploratoryBH, 0.25; and ApprehensiveBH, 0.13.
The resulting models had low RMSR, with a degrees of freedom correction (0.05, 0.04, and 0.03 for the bonnet, long-tailed, and rhesus macaque models, respectively), and were below 0.08 indicating acceptable goodness-of-fit. Alpha and total omega estimates were high across the three models (≥ 0.95). Communalities were high, with means of 0.72 ±0.12sd for rhesus, 0.71 ±0.13sd for long-tailed, and 0.73 ±0.10sd for bonnet macaques. Interfactor correlation coefficients are presented in the supplementary materials (S16 Table in S1 File). Our model structures relative to item and subject sample sizes were sufficient based on screening recommendations for communality values, item:factor ratios, and sample size [89].
Following our approach with the general models, we screened the human situation factor models to determine if structure was markedly changed with the omission of complex items; simple models were highly correlated (r ≥ |0.91|) with full models containing complex items (S3 Text, S17 Table in S1 File). Additionally, distinct reliability cut-offs (i.e., ICC[3,1] ≤ 0.10 or ≤ 0.00) did not alter our model structures (S1 Text; S1 Fig; S4 Table in S1 File).
Species comparisons of human-situation ratings.
We compared the factor models between the three species (Table 6). We observed high cross-correlations in two factors that showed structural comparability across the three taxa (ApprehensiveRLBH, ExploratoryRLH, and LazyRLH with Lazy/ExploratoryB comparable to both of the latter factors) as well as one factor that showed comparability between long-tailed and rhesus macaques (IrritableRL), with an average |r| between the respective factor loadings of 0.87.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
We found good congruence for IrritableRLH, ApprehensiveRBH, and ApprehensiveLBH (Table 6). We found fair congruence for ApprehensiveRLH, ExploratoryRLH, between Lazy/ExploratoryBH and ExploratoryLH, as well as Lazy/ExploratoryBH and ExploratoryRH. We also found poor congruence between EffectiveBH and IrritableLH. EffectiveBH was incongruent with all of the rhesus macaque factors. Finally, LazyRLH was incongruent. To determine what items resulted in the greatest contribution to incongruence, we conducted a stepwise removal (S4 Text in S1 File). Congruence for LazyRLH was improved by the removal of insecure.
We completed fuzzy set analyses on three factors that were represented across all three species (ApprehensiveRLB, ExploratoryRL, LazyRL with Lazy/ExploratoryB comparable to both latter factors) and one factor that was represented across just long-tailed and rhesus macaques (IrritableRL) (Table 7; S18 Table in S1 File). We appended a superscript ‘F’ to distinguish fuzzy sets from factors. Using a threshold of ≥ |0.40|, we found that across all three species: the ExploratoryF fuzzy set included the items active/energetic, affiliative/companionable, curious/exploratory/inquisitive, eccentric, impulsive, opportunistic, sociable, and solitary; the ApprehensiveF fuzzy set included the items apprehensive, bold, fearful, insecure, nervous/anxious/not calm, strong, and tense. The IrritableF fuzzy set included the items aggressive, bullying, calm/equable, defiant, equable, excitable, feisty, gentle, irritable, reckless, tolerant, understanding, and unemotional; the LazyF fuzzy set included the items depressed, lazy, and slow. Thus, we found evidence for similar item placement in each of these four factors.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
See S18 Table in S1 File for the remaining items.
Comparing general conspecific and human situation models
After averaging across observers, the reliable item ratings were highly correlated across the general and human situation surveys (correlations were run across subjects within each item; the mean r ± sd = 0.62 ± 0.14 [rhesus macaques], 0.65 ± 0.16 [long-tailed macaques], and 0.71 ± 0.10 [bonnet macaques]). In the long-tailed macaque ratings, only affiliative/companionable and sociable had Pearson’s correlations < 0.30; similarly, in the rhesus macaque ratings, only sociable fell below the same threshold; in the bonnet macaque ratings, none of the items fell below this threshold (the opportunistic coefficient of r = 0.50 was the lowest).
To assess differences between the general factor model and the human situation model, we extracted regression factor scores from and compared subjects’ factor scores between the two factor structures (Table 8). Across the three taxa, the ActiveRLB factors showed moderate-to-high associations with ExploratoryRLH or Lazy/ExploratoryBH in human situations (r = -0.82 in bonnet, 0.69 in long-tailed, -0.43 in rhesus macaques): more Active monkeys were more Exploratory (note that these correlations are directionally consistent when considering factor item structure, though item loadings of Lazy/ExploratoryBH and ActiveR would need to be reversed for the correlation to be positive across all three species). Although, across taxa, the ActiveRLB factors were moderately correlated (r ≥0.30) with all of the human situation factors except ApprehensiveRBH in rhesus and bonnet macaques as well as IrritableLH long-tailed macaques. The ConfidentRLB factors showed moderate-to-high associations with ApprehensiveRLBH in human situations (r = -0.62 in bonnet, -0.79 in long-tailed, -0.75 in rhesus macaques). In bonnet macaques, however, ConfidentB was also highly correlated with EffectiveBH in human situations in bonnet macaques (r = 0.71). The IrritableR, EquableR, and Irritable/EquableL factors were highly associated with IrritableRLH in human situations for long-tailed (r = 0.73) and rhesus macaques (r = 0.60 and -0.54 for IrritableR and EquableR, respectively). The negative correlation for EquableR was due to opposing loadings between IrritableR and EquableR (e.g., see the item gentle’s loadings). In bonnet macaques, EquableB was moderately associated with ApprehensiveBH in human situations (r = 0.35). SociableB was moderately-to-highly associated with ApprehensiveBH (r = 0.56) and Lazy/ExploratoryBH (r = -0.46) in human situations for bonnet macaques, but for long-tailed macaques SociableL was moderately associated with IrritableLH in human situations (r = -0.31). SociableR was not associated with any of the human situation factors for rhesus macaques. In rhesus macaques, the IrritableR factor had moderate-to-high correlations with all five of the factors isolated from human situations (r = 0.38–0.60). In long-tailed macaques, PlayfulL was moderately associated with LazyLH in human situations (r = 0.47).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Significance, uncorrected for multiple comparisons, is annotated with asterisks, such that: * < 0.05; ** < 0.01; *** <0.001.
Comparisons between item compositions in our fuzzy set analyses (Tables 4 and 7) showed similarities with the results from our factor score correlations. First, ConfidentF and ApprehensiveFH both included apprehensive, insecure, fearful, and nervous/anxious/not calm as salient items. ActiveF and LazyFH included lazy and slow, but ExploratoryFH also included active/energetic, curious/exploratory/inquisitive, and impulsive. SociableF and ExploratoryFH both included affiliative/companionable, eccentric, sociable, and solitary. Finally, the IrritableFH human situation fuzzy set, present only in rhesus and long-tailed macaques, included items associated with EquableF (calm/equable, equable, gentle, and tolerant) as well as IrritableF (bullying, excitable, feisty, and irritable).
Discussion
Our study provides evidence of the generalizability of personality structure across three group-living wild animal species. When rated by teams of human observers familiar with their behavior, rhesus, long-tailed, and bonnet macaques all evidenced four factors: ConfidentRLB, SociableRLB, ActiveRLB and Irritable/Equable (which varied in its structure across species). This organization is broadly comparable to those found in rhesus and long-tailed macaques from captive or free-ranging (i.e., naturalistic laboratory) populations [38, 63, 90–93]. Three personality factors, limited to human situations, emerged across all our study: ExploratoryRLBH, ApprehensiveRLBH, and LazyRLBH, though LazyRLH was incongruent. Additionally, both rhesus and long-tailed macaques had an IrritableRLH factor.
Our methods were consistent across species, and therefore facilitated cross-species comparability in underrepresented study animals and environments [4]. Consequently, we addressed a critical gap in the literature related to understanding comparative animal personality and individual behavioral differences [23], by assessing both cross-species and cross-situational differences in behavioral components linked to personality. The presence of the same four general personality traits across species (Confident, Sociable, Active, and Irritable/Equable) indicates a core phylogenetic similarity, an important finding among wild populations that are potentially subject to adaptively responding to current conditions, and is similar to outcomes reported with distinct surveys in a comparative survey of six macaque species–which included wild, free-ranging, and captive populations [36]. In line with our expectations for an association between factor structure and social styles, rhesus macaques, the more despotic of the species, exhibited a personality structure more strongly defined by aggression: IrritableR explained the highest proportion of model variation (0.25). Long-tailed macaques had a factor with high loadings in items representing both Irritable and Equable (i.e., Irritable/EquableL), while bonnet macaques had a factor more strongly defined by EquableB items.
The human situation factor models supported our expectations both of social style and adaptive response to urbanization: long-tailed and rhesus macaques both exhibited personality structures more strongly defined by aggressive and bold traits as evident via the IrritableRLH factor. Thus, in addition to the influences of species-typical evolutionary history or social styles [28, 36], the expression of personality may also be influenced through adaptive behavioral responses to rapidly changing urban or urban-rural environments. Indeed, this is consistent with more recent perspectives of aspects of macaque social structure having been shaped by both evolutionary underpinnings and adaptations to current socio-ecological factors [30, 31]. Disentangling the relative contributions and explanatory power of these evolutionary and adaptive processes would require the consideration of additional macaque groups and/or species, and by applying formal phylogenetic methods [94].
General conspecific factor models
We found evidence for four structurally equivalent factors across the three species: ConfidentRLB, SociableRLB, ActiveRLB, and an Irritable/Equable complex. This evidence supports similarities in these trait-like structures composed of similar items among these macaque species. For rhesus macaques, our general factor structure was comparable to those reported by Capitanio and colleagues [63, 90, 91] with ConfidentR similar to Confidence (inclusive of: bold, cautious, direct, effective, fearful, submissive, timid); SociableR to Sociability (inclusive of: affiliative, warm, sociable, solitary); EquableR with Equability (inclusive of: calm, equable, understanding), and IrritableR with Irritability (inclusive of: irritable) (S19 Table in S1 File). Our factor structure differed in that aggressive was placed with IrritableR, rather than Confidence (or, here, ConfidentR); similarly, excitable was placed with IrritableR, rather than Excitable. Additionally, tense and reckless were absent in our study (due to low communality and ICC, respectively). Tense and reckless loaded on Confidence and Irritability in the Capitanio lab studies, respectively. Finally, we found divergence in ActiveR (i.e., active/energetic, curious/exploratory/inquisitive, playful, slow as they loaded on Excitable, Sociable/Confident, Sociable, and Equable/Excitable, respectively). This divergence was largely attributable to the absence of ActiveR as a personality factor in Capitanio and colleagues’ studies. We posit that one possibility for this distinction might be that our subjects were wild while those from the other studies were captive. In support of this assertion, our factors contained similar items as those in dimensions reported in free-ranging rhesus macaques on Cayo Santiago [48]. We found structural equivalence between our IrritableR factor and their Dominance dimension (inclusive of: bullying, stingy/greedy, jealous, aggressive, irritable, defiant, excitable, gentle, manipulative); ConfidentR and Confidence (inclusive of: fearful, submissive, timid, cautious, anxious); SociableR and Friendliness (inclusive of: sociable, protective, intelligent, depressed, solitary); and ActiveR with Activity (inclusive of: active, lazy, and playful). Such equivalence is indicative of consistent structure in rhesus macaques across populations, environments, and protocols.
For long-tailed macaques, our general factor structure was comparable to a model from 34 captive long-tailed macaques [93]. We found structural equivalence between our ConfidentL factor and their Emotionality (inclusive of: anxious, apprehensive, confident, fearful, insecure, popular, strong); ActiveL with Activity (inclusive of: active, curious, opportunistic); SociableL with Sociability (inclusive of: popular, protective, sociable, solitary); and, EquableL with Tolerance (inclusive of: aggressive, equable, understanding). Our factor structure differed in that effective loaded on ConfidentL, rather than Activity. Additionally, playful and strong loaded on PlayfulL, rather than Emotionality [93]. Additionally, in a combined trait adjective and behavior factor model, Uher et al. [92] reported broadly similar factors of: Playful-active-curious, Aggressive-competitive, Prosocial-gregarious, and Assertive-nonanxious. Their use of a survey that included both behavioral descriptive verb and trait adjective items limits the utility of direct comparisons; nevertheless, we found structural equivalence with our PlayfulL and ActiveL factors and their reported Playful-active-curious factor (playful, curious, active, impulsive). Such equivalence, while more limited in scope relative to rhesus macaques, is indicative of consistent structure in long-tailed macaques across populations, environments, and protocols.
To our knowledge there is no published study on bonnet macaque personality, so we are not able to establish concordance with previous research for this species. Compared to the other two species we studied, however, our bonnet macaque model lacked a fifth factor–that is, the rhesus macaque model included IrritableR while the long-tailed macaque model included PlayfulL. One explanation for this could be species-typical differences in phylogenetic distances and/or the consequential cross-species differences in social styles. Alternatively, we can examine the factor structure of the long-tailed and rhesus macaques. For instance, as supported by our simplified model that excluded complex items, the omitted PlayfulL factor in long-tailed macaques could be similar to ActiveL [92] or Emotionality [93]. Even so, our rhesus macaque model also suggested a five-factor model; thus, the discrepancy in bonnet macaque model structure can likely only be resolved through future work.
We found a progression in the factor compositions among our populations that align with the grades of social style [28, 36]. Long-tailed macaques, as a grade 2 species, had a single Irritable/EquableL factor that represented many of the items present in the rhesus macaque IrritableR factor and EquableR factors. The single bonnet macaque factor, however, was primarily composed of items associated with EquableB as would be expected for a more tolerant grade 3 species. A more expansive survey design with strong a priori expectations regarding these structural differences would likely be necessary to elucidate these species differences.
Finally, though we referred to social style ‘grades’, such variance is likely better represented as a continuum of ‘social reaction norms’ in response to the same types of conditions [31, 95]. To better test for a continuum of interspecies differences in personality traits, it would be necessary to include extremely tolerant or egalitarian species (e.g., crested or Tonkean macaques [29, 96]), or nonhuman primate genera that characterize interspecific variation in social tolerance. For example, there are many primate taxa that exhibit variation in social styles among subtaxa (e.g., leaf-eating monkeys [Colobinae], mangabeys [Lophocebus; Rungwecebus; Cercocebus], guenons [Cercopithecus], or baboons [Papio]) and would, therefore, be suitable for providing additional insight into a structural continuum for social style.
Human situation factor models
We expected that species might have distinct responses to sympatry with humans or synanthropy, and could expect particularly salient trait expression among species in dense anthropogenic environments and/or with more frequent human interactions. Human situation models supported the existence of four factors: ExploratoryRLBH, LazyRLBH, and ApprehensiveRLBH; and, IrritableRLH in long-tailed and rhesus macaques. Many items in IrritableRLH (aggressive, calm/equable, irritable, impulsive, gentle, feisty, excitable, equable, tolerant, unemotional, understanding) were omitted in bonnet macaques due to low reliability. We also note that EffectiveBH in the bonnet macaque model contained several items that loaded on ApprehensiveRLH (cautious, confident, and timid); other items, however, in ApprehensiveRLH were excluded due to low reliability or communality scores in the bonnet macaque model (e.g., bold, insecure, fearful, strong).
The reduced reliability in the human situation ratings for bonnet macaques on items that loaded on IrritableRLH could potentially be explained by differences in behavior that has species-typical or socio-ecological underpinnings. For instance, this population of bonnet macaques has much lower rates of human-directed aggression events, relative to the rhesus and long-tailed macaques [12]. These lower instances are inter-related to the less urban locale that the bonnet macaques occupied. Thus, the absence of Irritable likely reflects behavioral characteristics, unique either to this particular bonnet macaque population that experiences low levels of anthropogenic impact, or to bonnet macaques more generally [35, 97, 98]. Disentangling these differences awaits the future assessments of intraspecific cross-population differences of human situation personality ratings.
While comparable survey designs of human situation ratings are scarce, our and others’ work indicates that there are traits that are salient in human situations. For instance, we found similar factor structure to that reported by Pritchard et al. [42]. Their raters frequently observed subjects on man-made platforms with frequent human-animal interactions [99–101]. Our ApprehensiveFH fuzzy set resembled factors of Insecurity (apprehensive, fearful, insecure, submissive, tense), ExploratoryFH to Boldness (aggressive, equable, excitable, gentle, irritable), and IrritableFH to Reactivity (active, curious). The human intruder test [102] isolated Activity (which includes exploratory behaviors), Emotionality (which includes fear-associated behaviors), and Aggression as salient response factors in 5000 infant rhesus macaques [102, 103]. Finally, our ApprehensiveFH, IrritableFH, and ExploratoryFH fuzzy sets show similarities to Fearfulness, Aggressiveness, and Cautiousness components, respectively, from behavioral responses to human tests [47].
While parallels between these studies suggest similarities among traits relevant to human-interactions, these items may not be distinguishable from general ratings despite somewhat distinct factor structure. For example, Baker et al. [37] isolated a Human–Sociability factor in Barbary macaques, but this factor was composed of both human-animal items (nervous, cooperative, cautious, and social) as well as general items (allogrooming, flexible, and effective). Similarly, our ExploratoryFH fuzzy set included items associated with the SociableF fuzzy set. Namely, affiliative/companionable, sociable, and solitary. Importantly, in the human situation ratings, these items are markers of prosocial behaviors expressed to humans rather than to conspecifics. Whether the ExploratoryFH factor offers insight into how macaques respond to humans requires future research. Such personality trait associations would interact with the effects of other known socio-demographic qualities’ influences on human engagement [12, 13].
Comparisons between general and human situation models
We found similar structures in a set of general and human situation factors. For instance, ActiveRLB was associated with the ExploratoryRLBH factors and shared similar items. Dammhahn [104], conducting open field and novel object tests mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus), reported similar results whereby activity, exploration, and boldness were posited to be linked in a behavioral syndrome, i.e. behavioral traits correlated and coupled across situations [105]. Thus, if we assume a behavioral syndrome between ActiveRLB and ExploratoryRLBH, then Active-like traits could be used to anticipate how macaques would interact with humans as a management tool. This interpretation, however, is challenging to reconcile with the absence of an Active trait in personality rating studies on captive rhesus macaques that regularly interacted with humans [63]; this discrepancy might parallel the absence of a pure Active trait in our human situation results.
We posit that Apprehensive and Confident are so structurally similar that they are likely to be functionally equivalent factors across situations. The ConfidentRLB factor scores were correlated with the human situation factor scores of ApprehensiveRLBH and EffectiveBH. Additionally, ConfidentRB and ApprehensiveRLBH factor loadings and fuzzy sets shared many of the same items. This proposition is not unfounded given that the stress response system is highly conserved [106, 107], that Boldness is a broadly quantified trait across challenging contexts [1], and that Boldness is important to fitness across contexts [108].
In restricting our focus to the rhesus and long-tailed models, the Irritable/Equable complex was strongly associated with the IrritableRLH factor. These are likely to be functionally similar given that both factors shared high loadings and fuzzy set contributions from several items. This interpretation, however, does not incorporate the knowledge that the general IrritableR factor was moderately associated with all of the human situation factors in rhesus macaques. Sussman et al. [47] reported a similar occurrence, with aggressive behaviors contributing to all components resulting from cage-side behavioral coding. As previously emphasized, rhesus macaques are generally expected to be despotic [28, 29] with demonstrated covariation in their personality structure [36].
Finally, we isolated a LazyRLH factor in rhesus and long-tailed macaques, and a Lazy/ExploratoryBH factor in bonnet macaques. While LazyRLH was incongruent, three items exhibited high factor loadings in both species, and mean loading scores were similar–as evident with the Pearson’s correlation coefficients. The three items that composed LazyFH (lazy, slow, depressed) do not strictly require the subject to interact with extrinsic stimuli as opposed to some of the items contained within ExploratoryFH. Given Active was associated with ExploratoryH and LazyH, we posit that these two factors reflect differences in assessment based on an individual’s active response to extrinsic stimuli (ExploratoryH) or overall behavior (LazyH). Resolving these factors by including similar items would improve clarity, as well, as to the incongruence with LazyRLH.
Limitations and further considerations
Our study is not without limitations. First, behavioral nuances may not have been directly observable to the human raters who collected data, as raters were distinct across sites although trained extensively on the same tool by the same trainer (EBM). In other words, the infrequent and homogenous interactions that our bonnet macaques had with humans [12] could also be attributed to raters’ limited capacity to effectively obtain a strong impression of how individuals vary in their responsivity to humans. Furthermore, our item and subject sample size, relative to the number of factors, was more limited for the bonnet macaques.
Second, inter-observer cultural differences might have impacted personality ratings. For instance, observers who contributed to our project were diverse in national and cultural identity. In humans, cultural variation in stereotypes can skew raters’ expectations [109]. We do not dispute the validity of ratings and the capacity for these ratings to measure personality ratings of nonhumans, without being instantiated as anthropomorphic projections [38]. Rather, raters might inadvertently introduce cultural framings or biases–though standardized training and reliability are expected to mitigate such effects. Such care in assessment design would facilitate parsing out whether items were not reliable due to stereotypes, errors in interpretation of the traits or definitions, and/or a psychobiological lack of variation or expression of that trait in the subject.
Conclusion
Differences and similarities across the three species’ factor models parallel distinctions in social style grades. Future research would benefit from expanding the taxa studied to accommodate covariation that exists in anthropogenic presence and phylogenetic relatedness. Furthermore, executing distinct surveys that emphasize varied responses to situations in wild complex systems would be key for determining which general traits exhibit cross-situational stability versus situation-specific variance. Such approaches are important, not just in the context of comparative personality among primates, but also in understanding individual-, population-, and species-level differences in tolerance of human sympatry.
Supporting information
S1 File. Supplementary materials for personality trait structures across three species of Macaca, using survey ratings of responses to conspecifics and humans.
Contains all supplementary text, tables, and figures.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309946.s001
Acknowledgments
We appreciate the support from numerous host nation organizations for research permissions at the three sites, including: the Department of Wildlife and National Parks Peninsular Malaysia, Economic Planning Unit Malaysia, the Forestry Department of Peninsular Malaysia, Himachal Pradesh Forest Department, the Kerala Forest and Wildlife Department, and Tourism Selangor. Local collaborators made this work possible, including: Lalit Mohan, Sandeep Rattan, Santosh Thakur, Nadine Ruppert, Ahmad Ismail, Shahrul Anuar Mohd Sah, and Ullasa Kodandaramaiah. Numerous research assistants facilitated data collection and logistics necessary for the existence of this work, including: Nalina Aiempichitkijkarn, Bidisha Chakraborty, Taniya Gill, Silvia La Gala, Kawaljit Kaur, Camille Luccisano, Eduardo Saczek, Benjamin Sipes, Alvara Sobrino, Mohammed Ismail, Rajarshi Saha, and Menno van Berkel.
References
1. 1. Blaszczyk MB. Primates got personality, too: Toward an integrative primatology of consistent individual differences in behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 2020;29(2):56–67. pmid:31721372
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
2. 2. Carter AJ, Feeney WE, Marshall HH, Cowlishaw G, Heinsohn R. Animal personality: what are behavioural ecologists measuring?: What are animal personality researchers measuring. Biological Reviews. 2013 May;88(2):465–75.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
3. 3. Coleman K. Comparative Perspectives on Temperament and Personality in Human and Nonhuman Animals. In: Schmidt LA, Poole KL, editors. Adaptive Shyness: Multiple Perspectives on Behavior and Development [Internet]. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2020 [cited 2020 May 15]. p. 253–77. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-38877-5_14
4. 4. Freeman HD, Gosling S. Personality in nonhuman primates: a review and evaluation of past research. Am J Primatol. 2010 Aug 1;72(8):653–71. pmid:20568079
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
5. 5. Funder DC. Personality. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001;52:197–221. pmid:11148304
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
6. 6. Martin JS, Jaeggi AV, Koski SE. The social evolution of individual differences: Future directions for a comparative science of personality in social behavior. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2023 Jan 1;144:104980. pmid:36463970
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
7. 7. Norman M, Rowden LJ, Cowlishaw G. Potential applications of personality assessments to the management of non-human primates: a review of 10 years of study. PeerJ. 2021 Sep 7;9:e12044. pmid:34589296
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
8. 8. Robinson LM, Weiss A. Primate Personality and Welfare. In: Robinson LM, Weiss A, editors. Nonhuman Primate Welfare: From History, Science, and Ethics to Practice [Internet]. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2023 [cited 2023 Aug 17]. p. 395–411. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-82708-3_17
9. 9. Merrick MJ, Koprowski JL. Should we consider individual behavior differences in applied wildlife conservation studies? Biological Conservation. 2017;209:34–44.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
10. 10. Powell DM, Gartner MC. Applications of personality to the management and conservation of nonhuman animals. In: From genes to animal behavior [Internet]. Springer; 2011 [cited 2017 Mar 26]. p. 185–99. Available from: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-4-431-53892-9_8
11. 11. Smith BR, Blumstein DT, Carere C, Maestripieri D. Animal personalities and conservation biology. Animal personalities: behavior, physiology, and evolution. 2013;379–411.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
12. 12. Balasubramaniam KN, Marty PR, Samartino S, Sobrino A, Gill T, Ismail M, et al. Impact of individual demographic and social factors on human–wildlife interactions: a comparative study of three macaque species. Sci Rep. 2020 Dec 15;10(1):21991. pmid:33319843
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
13. 13. Balasubramaniam KN, Kaburu SSK, Marty PR, Beisner BA, Bliss‐Moreau E, Arlet ME, et al. Implementing social network analysis to understand the socioecology of wildlife co‐occurrence and joint interactions with humans in anthropogenic environments. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2021 Dec;90(12):2819–33. pmid:34453852
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
14. 14. Chapple DG, Simmonds SM, Wong BBM. Can behavioral and personality traits influence the success of unintentional species introductions? Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2012 Jan;27(1):57–64. pmid:22001529
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
15. 15. Piquet JC, López-Darias M, van der Marel A, Nogales M, Waterman J. Unraveling behavioral and pace-of-life syndromes in a reduced parasite and predation pressure context: personality and survival of the Barbary ground squirrel. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2018 Aug 18;72(9):147.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
16. 16. van der Marel A, Waterman JM, López-Darias M. Exploring the role of life history traits and introduction effort in understanding invasion success in mammals: a case study of Barbary ground squirrels. Oecologia. 2021 Feb 1;195(2):327–39. pmid:33481090
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
17. 17. Parker TS, Nilon CH. Gray squirrel density, habitat suitability, and behavior in urban parks. Urban Ecosyst. 2008 Sep 1;11(3):243–55.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
18. 18. Warren P, Tripler C, Bolger D, Faeth S, Huntly N, Lepczyk C, et al. Urban Food Webs: Predators, Prey, and the People Who Feed Them. Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America. 2006;87(4):387–93.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
19. 19. Caspi T, Johnson JR, Lambert MR, Schell CJ, Sih A. Behavioral plasticity can facilitate evolution in urban environments. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2022 Dec 1;37(12):1092–103. pmid:36058767
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
20. 20. Cote J, Fogarty S, Brodin T, Weinersmith K, Sih A. Personality-dependent dispersal in the invasive mosquitofish: group composition matters. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2011 Jun 7;278(1712):1670–8. pmid:21068033
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
21. 21. Sapolsky RM, Share LJ. A pacific culture among wild baboons: its emergence and transmission. PLoS biology. 2004;2(4):e106. pmid:15094808
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
22. 22. Künzl C, Sachser N. The Behavioral Endocrinology of Domestication: A Comparison between the Domestic Guinea Pig (Cavia apereaf.porcellus) and Its Wild Ancestor, the Cavy (Cavia aperea). Hormones and Behavior. 1999 Feb 1;35(1):28–37. pmid:10049600
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
23. 23. de Boer SF, Buwalda B, Koolhaas JM. Untangling the neurobiology of coping styles in rodents: towards neural mechanisms underlying individual differences in disease susceptibility. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;74(Pt B):401–22. pmid:27402554
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
24. 24. Forss SIF, Schuppli C, Haiden D, Zweifel N, van Schaik CP. Contrasting responses to novelty by wild and captive orangutans. American Journal of Primatology. 2015;77(10):1109–21. pmid:26119509
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
25. 25. Balasubramaniam KN, Aiempichitkijkarn N, Kaburu SSK, Marty PR, Beisner BA, Bliss-Moreau E, et al. Impact of joint interactions with humans and social interactions with conspecifics on the risk of zooanthroponotic outbreaks among wildlife populations. Sci Rep. 2022 Jul 8;12(1):11600. pmid:35804182
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
26. 26. Balasubramaniam KN, Sueur C, Huffman MA, MacIntosh AJ. Primate infectious disease ecology: insights and future directions at the human-macaque interface. The behavioral ecology of the Tibetan macaque. 2020;249–84.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
27. 27. Riley EP. The human–macaque interface: conservation implications of current and future overlap and conflict in Lore Lindu National Park, Sulawesi, Indonesia. American Anthropologist. 2007;109(3):473–84.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
28. 28. Thierry B. Unity in diversity: Lessons from macaque societies. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 2007;16(6):224–38.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
29. 29. Thierry B, Iwaniuk AN, Pellis SM. The influence of phylogeny on the social behaviour of macaques (Primates: Cercopithecidae, genus Macaca. Ethology. 2000;106(8):713–28.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
30. 30. Balasubramaniam KN, Beisner BA, Berman CM, De Marco A, Duboscq J, Koirala S, et al. The influence of phylogeny, social style, and sociodemographic factors on macaque social network structure. American Journal of Primatology. 2018;80(1):e22727. pmid:29140552
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
31. 31. Balasubramaniam KN, Li J, Ogawa H, Berman CM. Tibetan macaque social style: covariant and quasi-independent evolution. Li J, Sun L, Kappeler PM, editors. The Behavioral Ecology of the Tibetan Macaque. 2020;141–69.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
32. 32. Schülke O, Ostner J. Ecological and social influences on sociality. In: Mitani JC, Call J, Kappeler PM, Palombit RA, Silk JB, editors. The Evolution of Primate Societies. Chicago, Il: University of Chicago Press; 2012. p. 195–219.
33. 33. Roos C, Zinner D. Chapter 1—Diversity and Evolutionary History of Macaques with Special Focus on Macaca mulatta and Macaca fascicularis. In: Bluemel J, Korte S, Schenck E, Weinbauer GF, editors. The Nonhuman Primate in Nonclinical Drug Development and Safety Assessment [Internet]. San Diego: Academic Press; 2015 [cited 2023 May 11]. p. 3–16. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780124171442000019
34. 34. Radhakrishna S, Huffman MA, Sinha A, editors. The Macaque Connection [Internet]. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2013 [cited 2015 Sep 6]. Available from: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-1-4614-3967-7
35. 35. Radhakrishna S, Sinha A. Less than wild? Commensal primates and wildlife conservation. Journal of Biosciences. 2011;36:749–53. pmid:22116271
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
36. 36. Adams MJ, Majolo B, Ostner J, Schülke O, De Marco A, Ethoikos F, et al. Personality structure and social style in macaques. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2015 Aug;109(2):338–53. pmid:26030054
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
37. 37. Baker KR, Lea SEG, Melfi VA. Comparative Personality Assessment of Three Captive Primate Species: Macaca nigra, Macaca sylvanus, and Saimiri sciureus. Int J Primatol. 2015 Jun 1;36(3):625–46.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
38. 38. Weiss A, Inoue-Murayama M, King JE, Adams MJ, Matsuzawa T. All too human? Chimpanzee and orang-utan personalities are not anthropomorphic projections. Animal Behaviour. 2012 Jun 1;83(6):1355–65.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
39. 39. Bolig R, Price CS, O’Neill PL, Suomi SJ. Subjective assessment of reactivity level and personality traits of rhesus monkeys. International Journal of Primatology. 1992 Jun;13(3):287–306.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
40. 40. Konečná M, Weiss A, Lhota S, Wallner B. Personality in Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus): Temporal stability and social rank. Journal of Research in Personality. 2012;46(5):581–90.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
41. 41. Mondragon-Ceballos R, Santillán-Doherty AM. The relationship between personality and age, sex and rank in captive stumptail macaques. Current primatology. 1994;2:241–50.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
42. 42. Pritchard AJ, Sheeran LK, Gabriel KI, Li JH, Wagner RS. Behaviors that predict personality components in adult free-ranging Tibetan macaques Macaca thibetana. Current Zoology. 2014 Jun 1;60(3):362–72.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
43. 43. Rouff JH, Sussman RW, Strube MJ. Personality traits in captive lion-tailed macaques (Macaca silenus). American Journal of Primatology. 2005;67(2):177–98. pmid:16229004
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
44. 44. Stevenson-Hinde J, Zunz M. Subjective assessment of individual rhesus monkeys. Primates. 1978;19(3):473–82.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
45. 45. Sussman AF, Mates EA, Ha JC, Bentson KL, Crockett CM. Tenure in current captive setting and age predict personality changes in adult pigtailed macaques. Animal behaviour. 2014;89:23–30. pmid:24567658
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
46. 46. Tkaczynski PJ, Ross C, MacLarnon A, Mouna M, Majolo B, Lehmann J. Measuring personality in the field: An in situ comparison of personality quantification methods in wild Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2019 Aug;133(3):313–25.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
47. 47. Sussman AF, Ha JC, Bentson KL, Crockett CM. Temperament in Rhesus, Long-Tailed, and Pigtailed Macaques Varies by Species and Sex. American Journal of Primatology. 2013;75(4):303–13. pmid:23225368
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
48. 48. Weiss A, Adams MJ, Widdig A, Gerald MS. Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) as living fossils of hominoid personality and subjective well-being. Journal of comparative psychology. 2011;125(1):72.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
49. 49. Goldberg LR. An alternative “description of personality”: the big-five factor structure. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1990;59(6):1216–29. pmid:2283588
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
50. 50. King JE, Figueredo AJ. The five-factor model plus dominance in chimpanzee personality. Journal of research in personality. 1997;31(2):257–71.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
51. 51. McCrae RR, John OP. An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of personality. 1992;60(2):175–215. pmid:1635039
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
52. 52. Funder DC. Towards a resolution of the personality triad: Persons, situations, and behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality. 2006 Feb;40(1):21–34.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
53. 53. Haage M, Bergvall UA, Maran T, Kiik K, Angerbjörn A. Situation and context impacts the expression of personality: The influence of breeding season and test context. Behavioural Processes. 2013 Nov 1;100:103–9. pmid:23988476
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
54. 54. Uher J, Asendorpf JB, Call J. Personality in the behaviour of great apes: temporal stability, cross-situational consistency and coherence in response. Animal Behaviour. 2008;75(1):99–112.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
55. 55. Capitanio JP. Nonhuman Primate Personality and Immunity: Mechanisms of Health and Disease. In: Weiss A, King JE, Murray L, editors. Personality and Temperament in Nonhuman Primates [Internet]. New York, NY: Springer; 2011 [cited 2017 Oct 21]. p. 233–55. (Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects). Available from: https://link-springer-com.ezproxy.cul.columbia.edu/chapter/10.1007/978-1-4614-0176-6_11
56. 56. Capitanio JP, Abel K, Mendoza SP, Blozis SA, McChesney MB, Cole SW, et al. Personality and serotonin transporter genotype interact with social context to affect immunity and viral set-point in simian immunodeficiency virus disease. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2008 Jul;22(5):676–89. pmid:17719201
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
57. 57. Bedford-Petersen C, Saucier G. Identifying contrasting themes that orchestrate personality expression across situations. Personality and Individual Differences. 2021 Mar 1;171:110495.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
58. 58. Conley MN, Saucier G. An initial broad-level mapping of personality-situation contingencies in self-report data. Personality and Individual Differences. 2019 Jan 1;136:166–72.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
59. 59. Marty PR, Balasubramaniam KN, Kaburu SSK, Hubbard J, Beisner B, Bliss-Moreau E, et al. Individuals in urban dwelling primate species face unequal benefits associated with living in an anthropogenic environment. Primates. 2020 Mar 1;61(2):249–55. pmid:31773350
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
60. 60. Marty PR, Beisner B, Kaburu SS, Balasubramaniam K, Bliss-Moreau E, Ruppert N, et al. Time constraints imposed by anthropogenic environments alter social behaviour in longtailed macaques. Animal Behaviour. 2019;150:157–65.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
61. 61. Balasubramaniam KN, Marty PR, Arlet ME, Beisner BA, Kaburu SSK, Bliss-Moreau E, et al. Impact of anthropogenic factors on affiliative behaviors among bonnet macaques. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2020;171(4):704–17. pmid:32064585
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
62. 62. Kaburu SS, Beisner B, Balasubramaniam KN, Marty PR, Bliss-Moreau E, Mohan L, et al. Interactions with humans impose time constraints on urban-dwelling rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behaviour. 2019;156(12):1255–82.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
63. 63. Maninger N, Capitanio JP, Mendoza SP, Mason WA. Personality influences tetanus-specific antibody response in adult male rhesus macaques after removal from natal group and housing relocation. American Journal of Primatology. 2003;61(2):73–83. pmid:14582129
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
64. 64. Gosling SD. Personality dimensions in spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1998;112(2):107. pmid:9642781
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
65. 65. Stevenson-Hinde J, Stillwell-Barnes R, Zunz M. Subjective assessment of rhesus monkeys over four successive years. Primates. 1980;21(1):66–82.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
66. 66. Uher J. Comparative personality research: methodological approaches. Eur J Pers. 2008 Aug 1;22(5):427–55.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
67. 67. Altmann J. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour. 1974;49(3/4):227–66. pmid:4597405
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
68. 68. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: (Version 4.2.2) [Computer software] R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2021. Available from: https://www.R-project.org/
69. 69. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. [Internet]. Evanston, Illinois, USA: Northwestern University; 2023. Available from: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych
70. 70. Revelle W, Condon DM. Reliability from α to ω: A tutorial. Psychological assessment. 2019;31(12):1395–411.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
71. 71. LeBreton JM, Senter JL. Answers to 20 Questions About Interrater Reliability and Interrater Agreement. Organizational Research Methods. 2008;11(4):815–52.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
72. 72. Manson JH, Perry S. Personality structure, sex differences, and temporal change and stability in wild white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2013;127(3):299–311.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
73. 73. Tinsley HE, Weiss DJ. Interrater reliability and agreement. In: Handbook of applied multivariate statistics and mathematical modeling [Internet]. Elsevier; 2000 [cited 2024 May 9]. p. 95–124. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780126913606500057
74. 74. Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological bulletin. 1979;86(2):420. pmid:18839484
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
75. 75. McGraw KO, Wong SP. Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological methods. 1996;1(1):30–46.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
76. 76. Cicchetti DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological assessment. 1994;6(4):284.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
77. 77. Eckardt W, Steklis HD, Steklis NG, Fletcher AW, Stoinski TS, Weiss A. Personality dimensions and their behavioral correlates in wild Virunga mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2015;129(1):26–41.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
78. 78. McCrae RR, Weiss A. Observer Ratings of Personality. In: Robins RW, Farley RC, Krueger RF, editors. Handbook of research methods in personality psychology. New York, NY, USA: The Guilford Press; 2007. p. 259–72.
79. 79. Rawlings B, Flynn E, Freeman H, Reamer L, Schapiro SJ, Lambeth S, et al. Sex differences in longitudinal personality stability in chimpanzees. Evolutionary Human Sciences. 2020 Jan;2:e46. pmid:37588391
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
80. 80. Weiss A, Wilson ML, Collins DA, Mjungu D, Kamenya S, Foerster S, et al. Personality in the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park. Sci Data. 2017 Oct 24;4(1):170146. pmid:29064463
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
81. 81. Costello AB, Osborne J. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Practical assessment, research, and evaluation. 2005;10(7):1–10.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
82. 82. Morton FB, Altschul D. Data reduction analyses of animal behaviour: avoiding Kaiser’s criterion and adopting more robust automated methods. Animal Behaviour. 2019;149:89–95.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
83. 83. Lorenzo-Seva U, Ten Berge JMF. Tucker’s Congruence Coefficient as a Meaningful Index of Factor Similarity. Methodology. 2006 Jan;2(2):57–64.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
84. 84. Lovik A, Nassiri V, Verbeke G, Molenberghs G. A Modified Tucker’s Congruence Coefficient for Factor Matching. Methodology. 2020 Apr 6;16(1):59–74.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
85. 85. De Raad B, Morales-Vives F, Barelds DPH, Van Oudenhoven JP, Renner W, Timmerman ME. Values in a Cross-Cultural Triangle: A Comparison of Value Taxonomies in the Netherlands, Austria, and Spain. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2016 Sep 1;47(8):1053–75.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
86. 86. Smithson M. Fuzzy Set Analysis for Behavioral and Social Sciences. Springer Science & Business Media; 2012. 332 p.
87. 87. Smithson M, Verkuilen J. Fuzzy Set Theory: Applications in the Social Sciences. Thousand Oaks, California, USA: SAGE Publications, Inc; 2006. 112 p. (Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences; vol. 147).
88. 88. ten Berge JMF, Krijnen WP, Wansbeek T, Shapiro A. Some new results on correlation-preserving factor scores prediction methods. Linear Algebra and its Applications. 1999 Mar 1;289(1):311–8.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
89. 89. MacCallum RC, Widaman KF, Zhang S, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological methods. 1999;4(1):84.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
90. 90. Capitanio JP. Personality dimensions in adult male rhesus macaques: prediction of behaviors across time and situation. American Journal of Primatology. 1999;47(4):299–320. pmid:10206208
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
91. 91. Capitanio JP, Widaman KF. Confirmatory factor analysis of personality structure in adult male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of Primatology. 2005;65(3):289–94. pmid:15772988
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
92. 92. Uher J, Werner CS, Gosselt K. From observations of individual behaviour to social representations of personality: Developmental pathways, attribution biases, and limitations of questionnaire methods. Journal of Research in Personality. 2013 Oct 1;47(5):647–67.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
93. 93. Wergård EM, Westlund K, Spångberg M, Fredlund H, Forkman B. Training success in group-housed long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) is better explained by personality than by social rank. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2016 Apr 1;177:52–8.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
94. 94. Nunn CL. The comparative approach in evolutionary anthropology and biology [Internet]. University of Chicago Press; 2011 [cited 2024 Mar 4]. Available from: https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=qj4cSzJGQJAC&oi=fnd&pg=PR5&dq=The+comparative+approach+in+evolutionary+anthropology+and+biology.+&ots=nA74rrkAEn&sig=ovrN6C5AqR6fJQP5mCI-J8Ds_Vc
95. 95. Berman CM, Thierry B. Variation in kin bias: species differences and time constraints in macaques. Behaviour. 2010;1863–87.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
96. 96. Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP. The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1997 Nov 1;41(5):291–309.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
97. 97. Ram S, Venkatachalam S, Sinha A. Changing social strategies of wild female bonnet macaques during natural foraging and on provisioning. Current Science. 2003;780–90.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
98. 98. Sinha A, Mukhopadhyay K, Datta-Roy A, Ram S. Ecology proposes, behaviour disposes: Ecological variability in social organization and male behavioural strategies among wild bonnet macaques. Current Science. 2005;1166–79.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
99. 99. McCarthy MS, Matheson MD, Lester JD, Sheeran LK, Li JH, Wagner RS. Sequences of Tibetan Macaque (Macaca thibetana) and Tourist Behaviors at Mt. Huangshan, China. prco. 2009 Mar;24(1):145–51.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
100. 100. Ruesto LA, Sheeran LK, Matheson MD, Li JH, Wagner RS. Tourist Behavior and Decibel Levels Correlate with Threat Frequency in Tibetan Macaques (Macaca thibetana) at Mt. Huangshan, China. prco. 2010 Dec;2010(25):99–104.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
101. 101. Usui R, Sheeran L, Li J hua, Sun L, Wang X, Pritchard A, et al. Park Rangers’ Behaviors and Their Effects on Tourists and Tibetan Macaques (Macaca thibetana) at Mt. Huangshan, China. Animals. 2014 Sep 15;4(3):546–61.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
102. 102. Capitanio JP. Knowledge of Biobehavioral Organization Can Facilitate Better Science: A Review of the BioBehavioral Assessment Program at the California National Primate Research Center. Animals. 2021 Aug;11(8):2445. pmid:34438902
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
103. 103. Gottlieb DH, Capitanio JP. Latent Variables Affecting Behavioral Response to the Human Intruder Test in Infant Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta): Latent Variables in Rhesus Intruder Test. Am J Primatol. 2013 Apr;75(4):314–23.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
104. 104. Dammhahn M. Are personality differences in a small iteroparous mammal maintained by a life-history trade-off? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012 Mar 7;279(1738):2645–51.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
105. 105. Sih A, Bell A, Johnson JC. Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in ecology & evolution. 2004;19(7):372–8. pmid:16701288
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
106. 106. McEwen BS, Wingfield JC. The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Horm Behav. 2003 Jan;43(1):2–15. pmid:12614627
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
107. 107. Øverli Ø, Sørensen C, Pulman KG, Pottinger TG, Korzan W, Summers CH, et al. Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles: relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2007;31(3):396–412. pmid:17182101
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
108. 108. Smith BR, Blumstein DT. Fitness consequences of personality: a meta-analysis. Behavioral Ecology. 2008;19(2):448–55.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
109. 109. Chan W, McCrae RR, De Fruyt F, Jussim L, Löckenhoff CE, De Bolle M, et al. Stereotypes of age differences in personality traits: Universal and accurate? Journal of personality and social psychology. 2012;103(6):1050. pmid:23088227
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
Citation: Pritchard AJ, Bliss-Moreau E, Balasubramaniam KN, Capitanio JP, Marty PR, Kaburu SSK, et al. (2024) Personality trait structures across three species of Macaca, using survey ratings of responses to conspecifics and humans. PLoS ONE 19(9): e0309946. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0309946
About the Authors:
Alexander J. Pritchard
Roles: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft
E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliations: California National Primate Research Center, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America, Department of Population Health & Reproduction, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4922-3347
Eliza Bliss-Moreau
Roles: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing
Affiliations: California National Primate Research Center, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America, Department of Psychology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0740-5612
Krishna N. Balasubramaniam
Roles: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliations: Department of Population Health & Reproduction, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America, School of Life Sciences, Faculty of Science & Engineering, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, United Kingdom
John P. Capitanio
Roles: Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: California National Primate Research Center, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America
Pascal R. Marty
Roles: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision
Affiliations: Department of Population Health & Reproduction, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America, Nature Reserve and Wildlife Park Goldau, Goldau, Switzerland
Stefano S. K. Kaburu
Roles: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliations: Department of Population Health & Reproduction, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America, School of Animal, Rural and Environmental Sciences, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, United Kingdom
Małgorzata E. Arlet
Roles: Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Institute of Human Biology and Evolution, Faculty of Biology, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań, Poland
Brianne A. Beisner
Roles: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition
Affiliation: California National Primate Research Center, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9440-4521
Brenda McCowan
Roles: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliations: California National Primate Research Center, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America, Department of Population Health & Reproduction, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States of America
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
1. Blaszczyk MB. Primates got personality, too: Toward an integrative primatology of consistent individual differences in behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 2020;29(2):56–67. pmid:31721372
2. Carter AJ, Feeney WE, Marshall HH, Cowlishaw G, Heinsohn R. Animal personality: what are behavioural ecologists measuring?: What are animal personality researchers measuring. Biological Reviews. 2013 May;88(2):465–75.
3. Coleman K. Comparative Perspectives on Temperament and Personality in Human and Nonhuman Animals. In: Schmidt LA, Poole KL, editors. Adaptive Shyness: Multiple Perspectives on Behavior and Development [Internet]. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2020 [cited 2020 May 15]. p. 253–77. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-38877-5_14
4. Freeman HD, Gosling S. Personality in nonhuman primates: a review and evaluation of past research. Am J Primatol. 2010 Aug 1;72(8):653–71. pmid:20568079
5. Funder DC. Personality. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001;52:197–221. pmid:11148304
6. Martin JS, Jaeggi AV, Koski SE. The social evolution of individual differences: Future directions for a comparative science of personality in social behavior. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2023 Jan 1;144:104980. pmid:36463970
7. Norman M, Rowden LJ, Cowlishaw G. Potential applications of personality assessments to the management of non-human primates: a review of 10 years of study. PeerJ. 2021 Sep 7;9:e12044. pmid:34589296
8. Robinson LM, Weiss A. Primate Personality and Welfare. In: Robinson LM, Weiss A, editors. Nonhuman Primate Welfare: From History, Science, and Ethics to Practice [Internet]. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2023 [cited 2023 Aug 17]. p. 395–411. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-82708-3_17
9. Merrick MJ, Koprowski JL. Should we consider individual behavior differences in applied wildlife conservation studies? Biological Conservation. 2017;209:34–44.
10. Powell DM, Gartner MC. Applications of personality to the management and conservation of nonhuman animals. In: From genes to animal behavior [Internet]. Springer; 2011 [cited 2017 Mar 26]. p. 185–99. Available from: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-4-431-53892-9_8
11. Smith BR, Blumstein DT, Carere C, Maestripieri D. Animal personalities and conservation biology. Animal personalities: behavior, physiology, and evolution. 2013;379–411.
12. Balasubramaniam KN, Marty PR, Samartino S, Sobrino A, Gill T, Ismail M, et al. Impact of individual demographic and social factors on human–wildlife interactions: a comparative study of three macaque species. Sci Rep. 2020 Dec 15;10(1):21991. pmid:33319843
13. Balasubramaniam KN, Kaburu SSK, Marty PR, Beisner BA, Bliss‐Moreau E, Arlet ME, et al. Implementing social network analysis to understand the socioecology of wildlife co‐occurrence and joint interactions with humans in anthropogenic environments. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2021 Dec;90(12):2819–33. pmid:34453852
14. Chapple DG, Simmonds SM, Wong BBM. Can behavioral and personality traits influence the success of unintentional species introductions? Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2012 Jan;27(1):57–64. pmid:22001529
15. Piquet JC, López-Darias M, van der Marel A, Nogales M, Waterman J. Unraveling behavioral and pace-of-life syndromes in a reduced parasite and predation pressure context: personality and survival of the Barbary ground squirrel. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2018 Aug 18;72(9):147.
16. van der Marel A, Waterman JM, López-Darias M. Exploring the role of life history traits and introduction effort in understanding invasion success in mammals: a case study of Barbary ground squirrels. Oecologia. 2021 Feb 1;195(2):327–39. pmid:33481090
17. Parker TS, Nilon CH. Gray squirrel density, habitat suitability, and behavior in urban parks. Urban Ecosyst. 2008 Sep 1;11(3):243–55.
18. Warren P, Tripler C, Bolger D, Faeth S, Huntly N, Lepczyk C, et al. Urban Food Webs: Predators, Prey, and the People Who Feed Them. Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America. 2006;87(4):387–93.
19. Caspi T, Johnson JR, Lambert MR, Schell CJ, Sih A. Behavioral plasticity can facilitate evolution in urban environments. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2022 Dec 1;37(12):1092–103. pmid:36058767
20. Cote J, Fogarty S, Brodin T, Weinersmith K, Sih A. Personality-dependent dispersal in the invasive mosquitofish: group composition matters. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2011 Jun 7;278(1712):1670–8. pmid:21068033
21. Sapolsky RM, Share LJ. A pacific culture among wild baboons: its emergence and transmission. PLoS biology. 2004;2(4):e106. pmid:15094808
22. Künzl C, Sachser N. The Behavioral Endocrinology of Domestication: A Comparison between the Domestic Guinea Pig (Cavia apereaf.porcellus) and Its Wild Ancestor, the Cavy (Cavia aperea). Hormones and Behavior. 1999 Feb 1;35(1):28–37. pmid:10049600
23. de Boer SF, Buwalda B, Koolhaas JM. Untangling the neurobiology of coping styles in rodents: towards neural mechanisms underlying individual differences in disease susceptibility. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;74(Pt B):401–22. pmid:27402554
24. Forss SIF, Schuppli C, Haiden D, Zweifel N, van Schaik CP. Contrasting responses to novelty by wild and captive orangutans. American Journal of Primatology. 2015;77(10):1109–21. pmid:26119509
25. Balasubramaniam KN, Aiempichitkijkarn N, Kaburu SSK, Marty PR, Beisner BA, Bliss-Moreau E, et al. Impact of joint interactions with humans and social interactions with conspecifics on the risk of zooanthroponotic outbreaks among wildlife populations. Sci Rep. 2022 Jul 8;12(1):11600. pmid:35804182
26. Balasubramaniam KN, Sueur C, Huffman MA, MacIntosh AJ. Primate infectious disease ecology: insights and future directions at the human-macaque interface. The behavioral ecology of the Tibetan macaque. 2020;249–84.
27. Riley EP. The human–macaque interface: conservation implications of current and future overlap and conflict in Lore Lindu National Park, Sulawesi, Indonesia. American Anthropologist. 2007;109(3):473–84.
28. Thierry B. Unity in diversity: Lessons from macaque societies. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 2007;16(6):224–38.
29. Thierry B, Iwaniuk AN, Pellis SM. The influence of phylogeny on the social behaviour of macaques (Primates: Cercopithecidae, genus Macaca. Ethology. 2000;106(8):713–28.
30. Balasubramaniam KN, Beisner BA, Berman CM, De Marco A, Duboscq J, Koirala S, et al. The influence of phylogeny, social style, and sociodemographic factors on macaque social network structure. American Journal of Primatology. 2018;80(1):e22727. pmid:29140552
31. Balasubramaniam KN, Li J, Ogawa H, Berman CM. Tibetan macaque social style: covariant and quasi-independent evolution. Li J, Sun L, Kappeler PM, editors. The Behavioral Ecology of the Tibetan Macaque. 2020;141–69.
32. Schülke O, Ostner J. Ecological and social influences on sociality. In: Mitani JC, Call J, Kappeler PM, Palombit RA, Silk JB, editors. The Evolution of Primate Societies. Chicago, Il: University of Chicago Press; 2012. p. 195–219.
33. Roos C, Zinner D. Chapter 1—Diversity and Evolutionary History of Macaques with Special Focus on Macaca mulatta and Macaca fascicularis. In: Bluemel J, Korte S, Schenck E, Weinbauer GF, editors. The Nonhuman Primate in Nonclinical Drug Development and Safety Assessment [Internet]. San Diego: Academic Press; 2015 [cited 2023 May 11]. p. 3–16. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780124171442000019
34. Radhakrishna S, Huffman MA, Sinha A, editors. The Macaque Connection [Internet]. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2013 [cited 2015 Sep 6]. Available from: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-1-4614-3967-7
35. Radhakrishna S, Sinha A. Less than wild? Commensal primates and wildlife conservation. Journal of Biosciences. 2011;36:749–53. pmid:22116271
36. Adams MJ, Majolo B, Ostner J, Schülke O, De Marco A, Ethoikos F, et al. Personality structure and social style in macaques. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2015 Aug;109(2):338–53. pmid:26030054
37. Baker KR, Lea SEG, Melfi VA. Comparative Personality Assessment of Three Captive Primate Species: Macaca nigra, Macaca sylvanus, and Saimiri sciureus. Int J Primatol. 2015 Jun 1;36(3):625–46.
38. Weiss A, Inoue-Murayama M, King JE, Adams MJ, Matsuzawa T. All too human? Chimpanzee and orang-utan personalities are not anthropomorphic projections. Animal Behaviour. 2012 Jun 1;83(6):1355–65.
39. Bolig R, Price CS, O’Neill PL, Suomi SJ. Subjective assessment of reactivity level and personality traits of rhesus monkeys. International Journal of Primatology. 1992 Jun;13(3):287–306.
40. Konečná M, Weiss A, Lhota S, Wallner B. Personality in Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus): Temporal stability and social rank. Journal of Research in Personality. 2012;46(5):581–90.
41. Mondragon-Ceballos R, Santillán-Doherty AM. The relationship between personality and age, sex and rank in captive stumptail macaques. Current primatology. 1994;2:241–50.
42. Pritchard AJ, Sheeran LK, Gabriel KI, Li JH, Wagner RS. Behaviors that predict personality components in adult free-ranging Tibetan macaques Macaca thibetana. Current Zoology. 2014 Jun 1;60(3):362–72.
43. Rouff JH, Sussman RW, Strube MJ. Personality traits in captive lion-tailed macaques (Macaca silenus). American Journal of Primatology. 2005;67(2):177–98. pmid:16229004
44. Stevenson-Hinde J, Zunz M. Subjective assessment of individual rhesus monkeys. Primates. 1978;19(3):473–82.
45. Sussman AF, Mates EA, Ha JC, Bentson KL, Crockett CM. Tenure in current captive setting and age predict personality changes in adult pigtailed macaques. Animal behaviour. 2014;89:23–30. pmid:24567658
46. Tkaczynski PJ, Ross C, MacLarnon A, Mouna M, Majolo B, Lehmann J. Measuring personality in the field: An in situ comparison of personality quantification methods in wild Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2019 Aug;133(3):313–25.
47. Sussman AF, Ha JC, Bentson KL, Crockett CM. Temperament in Rhesus, Long-Tailed, and Pigtailed Macaques Varies by Species and Sex. American Journal of Primatology. 2013;75(4):303–13. pmid:23225368
48. Weiss A, Adams MJ, Widdig A, Gerald MS. Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) as living fossils of hominoid personality and subjective well-being. Journal of comparative psychology. 2011;125(1):72.
49. Goldberg LR. An alternative “description of personality”: the big-five factor structure. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1990;59(6):1216–29. pmid:2283588
50. King JE, Figueredo AJ. The five-factor model plus dominance in chimpanzee personality. Journal of research in personality. 1997;31(2):257–71.
51. McCrae RR, John OP. An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of personality. 1992;60(2):175–215. pmid:1635039
52. Funder DC. Towards a resolution of the personality triad: Persons, situations, and behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality. 2006 Feb;40(1):21–34.
53. Haage M, Bergvall UA, Maran T, Kiik K, Angerbjörn A. Situation and context impacts the expression of personality: The influence of breeding season and test context. Behavioural Processes. 2013 Nov 1;100:103–9. pmid:23988476
54. Uher J, Asendorpf JB, Call J. Personality in the behaviour of great apes: temporal stability, cross-situational consistency and coherence in response. Animal Behaviour. 2008;75(1):99–112.
55. Capitanio JP. Nonhuman Primate Personality and Immunity: Mechanisms of Health and Disease. In: Weiss A, King JE, Murray L, editors. Personality and Temperament in Nonhuman Primates [Internet]. New York, NY: Springer; 2011 [cited 2017 Oct 21]. p. 233–55. (Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects). Available from: https://link-springer-com.ezproxy.cul.columbia.edu/chapter/10.1007/978-1-4614-0176-6_11
56. Capitanio JP, Abel K, Mendoza SP, Blozis SA, McChesney MB, Cole SW, et al. Personality and serotonin transporter genotype interact with social context to affect immunity and viral set-point in simian immunodeficiency virus disease. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2008 Jul;22(5):676–89. pmid:17719201
57. Bedford-Petersen C, Saucier G. Identifying contrasting themes that orchestrate personality expression across situations. Personality and Individual Differences. 2021 Mar 1;171:110495.
58. Conley MN, Saucier G. An initial broad-level mapping of personality-situation contingencies in self-report data. Personality and Individual Differences. 2019 Jan 1;136:166–72.
59. Marty PR, Balasubramaniam KN, Kaburu SSK, Hubbard J, Beisner B, Bliss-Moreau E, et al. Individuals in urban dwelling primate species face unequal benefits associated with living in an anthropogenic environment. Primates. 2020 Mar 1;61(2):249–55. pmid:31773350
60. Marty PR, Beisner B, Kaburu SS, Balasubramaniam K, Bliss-Moreau E, Ruppert N, et al. Time constraints imposed by anthropogenic environments alter social behaviour in longtailed macaques. Animal Behaviour. 2019;150:157–65.
61. Balasubramaniam KN, Marty PR, Arlet ME, Beisner BA, Kaburu SSK, Bliss-Moreau E, et al. Impact of anthropogenic factors on affiliative behaviors among bonnet macaques. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2020;171(4):704–17. pmid:32064585
62. Kaburu SS, Beisner B, Balasubramaniam KN, Marty PR, Bliss-Moreau E, Mohan L, et al. Interactions with humans impose time constraints on urban-dwelling rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behaviour. 2019;156(12):1255–82.
63. Maninger N, Capitanio JP, Mendoza SP, Mason WA. Personality influences tetanus-specific antibody response in adult male rhesus macaques after removal from natal group and housing relocation. American Journal of Primatology. 2003;61(2):73–83. pmid:14582129
64. Gosling SD. Personality dimensions in spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1998;112(2):107. pmid:9642781
65. Stevenson-Hinde J, Stillwell-Barnes R, Zunz M. Subjective assessment of rhesus monkeys over four successive years. Primates. 1980;21(1):66–82.
66. Uher J. Comparative personality research: methodological approaches. Eur J Pers. 2008 Aug 1;22(5):427–55.
67. Altmann J. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour. 1974;49(3/4):227–66. pmid:4597405
68. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: (Version 4.2.2) [Computer software] R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2021. Available from: https://www.R-project.org/
69. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. [Internet]. Evanston, Illinois, USA: Northwestern University; 2023. Available from: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych
70. Revelle W, Condon DM. Reliability from α to ω: A tutorial. Psychological assessment. 2019;31(12):1395–411.
71. LeBreton JM, Senter JL. Answers to 20 Questions About Interrater Reliability and Interrater Agreement. Organizational Research Methods. 2008;11(4):815–52.
72. Manson JH, Perry S. Personality structure, sex differences, and temporal change and stability in wild white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2013;127(3):299–311.
73. Tinsley HE, Weiss DJ. Interrater reliability and agreement. In: Handbook of applied multivariate statistics and mathematical modeling [Internet]. Elsevier; 2000 [cited 2024 May 9]. p. 95–124. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780126913606500057
74. Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological bulletin. 1979;86(2):420. pmid:18839484
75. McGraw KO, Wong SP. Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological methods. 1996;1(1):30–46.
76. Cicchetti DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological assessment. 1994;6(4):284.
77. Eckardt W, Steklis HD, Steklis NG, Fletcher AW, Stoinski TS, Weiss A. Personality dimensions and their behavioral correlates in wild Virunga mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2015;129(1):26–41.
78. McCrae RR, Weiss A. Observer Ratings of Personality. In: Robins RW, Farley RC, Krueger RF, editors. Handbook of research methods in personality psychology. New York, NY, USA: The Guilford Press; 2007. p. 259–72.
79. Rawlings B, Flynn E, Freeman H, Reamer L, Schapiro SJ, Lambeth S, et al. Sex differences in longitudinal personality stability in chimpanzees. Evolutionary Human Sciences. 2020 Jan;2:e46. pmid:37588391
80. Weiss A, Wilson ML, Collins DA, Mjungu D, Kamenya S, Foerster S, et al. Personality in the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park. Sci Data. 2017 Oct 24;4(1):170146. pmid:29064463
81. Costello AB, Osborne J. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Practical assessment, research, and evaluation. 2005;10(7):1–10.
82. Morton FB, Altschul D. Data reduction analyses of animal behaviour: avoiding Kaiser’s criterion and adopting more robust automated methods. Animal Behaviour. 2019;149:89–95.
83. Lorenzo-Seva U, Ten Berge JMF. Tucker’s Congruence Coefficient as a Meaningful Index of Factor Similarity. Methodology. 2006 Jan;2(2):57–64.
84. Lovik A, Nassiri V, Verbeke G, Molenberghs G. A Modified Tucker’s Congruence Coefficient for Factor Matching. Methodology. 2020 Apr 6;16(1):59–74.
85. De Raad B, Morales-Vives F, Barelds DPH, Van Oudenhoven JP, Renner W, Timmerman ME. Values in a Cross-Cultural Triangle: A Comparison of Value Taxonomies in the Netherlands, Austria, and Spain. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2016 Sep 1;47(8):1053–75.
86. Smithson M. Fuzzy Set Analysis for Behavioral and Social Sciences. Springer Science & Business Media; 2012. 332 p.
87. Smithson M, Verkuilen J. Fuzzy Set Theory: Applications in the Social Sciences. Thousand Oaks, California, USA: SAGE Publications, Inc; 2006. 112 p. (Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences; vol. 147).
88. ten Berge JMF, Krijnen WP, Wansbeek T, Shapiro A. Some new results on correlation-preserving factor scores prediction methods. Linear Algebra and its Applications. 1999 Mar 1;289(1):311–8.
89. MacCallum RC, Widaman KF, Zhang S, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological methods. 1999;4(1):84.
90. Capitanio JP. Personality dimensions in adult male rhesus macaques: prediction of behaviors across time and situation. American Journal of Primatology. 1999;47(4):299–320. pmid:10206208
91. Capitanio JP, Widaman KF. Confirmatory factor analysis of personality structure in adult male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of Primatology. 2005;65(3):289–94. pmid:15772988
92. Uher J, Werner CS, Gosselt K. From observations of individual behaviour to social representations of personality: Developmental pathways, attribution biases, and limitations of questionnaire methods. Journal of Research in Personality. 2013 Oct 1;47(5):647–67.
93. Wergård EM, Westlund K, Spångberg M, Fredlund H, Forkman B. Training success in group-housed long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) is better explained by personality than by social rank. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2016 Apr 1;177:52–8.
94. Nunn CL. The comparative approach in evolutionary anthropology and biology [Internet]. University of Chicago Press; 2011 [cited 2024 Mar 4]. Available from: https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=qj4cSzJGQJAC&oi=fnd&pg=PR5&dq=The+comparative+approach+in+evolutionary+anthropology+and+biology.+&ots=nA74rrkAEn&sig=ovrN6C5AqR6fJQP5mCI-J8Ds_Vc
95. Berman CM, Thierry B. Variation in kin bias: species differences and time constraints in macaques. Behaviour. 2010;1863–87.
96. Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP. The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1997 Nov 1;41(5):291–309.
97. Ram S, Venkatachalam S, Sinha A. Changing social strategies of wild female bonnet macaques during natural foraging and on provisioning. Current Science. 2003;780–90.
98. Sinha A, Mukhopadhyay K, Datta-Roy A, Ram S. Ecology proposes, behaviour disposes: Ecological variability in social organization and male behavioural strategies among wild bonnet macaques. Current Science. 2005;1166–79.
99. McCarthy MS, Matheson MD, Lester JD, Sheeran LK, Li JH, Wagner RS. Sequences of Tibetan Macaque (Macaca thibetana) and Tourist Behaviors at Mt. Huangshan, China. prco. 2009 Mar;24(1):145–51.
100. Ruesto LA, Sheeran LK, Matheson MD, Li JH, Wagner RS. Tourist Behavior and Decibel Levels Correlate with Threat Frequency in Tibetan Macaques (Macaca thibetana) at Mt. Huangshan, China. prco. 2010 Dec;2010(25):99–104.
101. Usui R, Sheeran L, Li J hua, Sun L, Wang X, Pritchard A, et al. Park Rangers’ Behaviors and Their Effects on Tourists and Tibetan Macaques (Macaca thibetana) at Mt. Huangshan, China. Animals. 2014 Sep 15;4(3):546–61.
102. Capitanio JP. Knowledge of Biobehavioral Organization Can Facilitate Better Science: A Review of the BioBehavioral Assessment Program at the California National Primate Research Center. Animals. 2021 Aug;11(8):2445. pmid:34438902
103. Gottlieb DH, Capitanio JP. Latent Variables Affecting Behavioral Response to the Human Intruder Test in Infant Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta): Latent Variables in Rhesus Intruder Test. Am J Primatol. 2013 Apr;75(4):314–23.
104. Dammhahn M. Are personality differences in a small iteroparous mammal maintained by a life-history trade-off? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012 Mar 7;279(1738):2645–51.
105. Sih A, Bell A, Johnson JC. Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in ecology & evolution. 2004;19(7):372–8. pmid:16701288
106. McEwen BS, Wingfield JC. The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Horm Behav. 2003 Jan;43(1):2–15. pmid:12614627
107. Øverli Ø, Sørensen C, Pulman KG, Pottinger TG, Korzan W, Summers CH, et al. Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles: relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2007;31(3):396–412. pmid:17182101
108. Smith BR, Blumstein DT. Fitness consequences of personality: a meta-analysis. Behavioral Ecology. 2008;19(2):448–55.
109. Chan W, McCrae RR, De Fruyt F, Jussim L, Löckenhoff CE, De Bolle M, et al. Stereotypes of age differences in personality traits: Universal and accurate? Journal of personality and social psychology. 2012;103(6):1050. pmid:23088227
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2024 Pritchard et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Comparative studies reliant on single personality surveys to rate wild primates are scarce yet remain critical for developing a holistic comparative understanding of personality. Differences in survey design, item exclusion, and factor selection impede cross-study comparisons. To address these challenges, we used consistently collected data to assess personality trait structures in wild rhesus (Macaca mulatta), bonnet (M. radiata), and long-tailed (M. fascicularis) macaques that varied in their degree of phylogenetic closeness, species-typical social styles, and anthropogenic exposure in urban or urban-rural environments. We administered 51-item personality surveys to familiar raters, and, after reliability and structure screenings, isolated 4–5 factor solutions among the species. Four consistent factors emerged: Confident, Sociable, Active, and Irritable/Equable. This latter factor had differential expression across species. Item composition of the Irritable/Equable factor was consistent with their anticipated differences in social styles, but confounded by cross-site anthropogenic variation. We also administered a 43-item survey confined to human-primate situations which paralleled our findings of social style variation, while also exhibiting variation that aligned with population differences in human density. Our findings indicate that macaque personality trait structures may be emergent outcomes of evolutionary and/or socioecological processes, but further research is needed to parse these processes’ relative contributions.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer