It appears you don't have support to open PDFs in this web browser. To view this file, Open with your PDF reader
Abstract
Childhood bereavement is an adverse event, yet children demonstrate considerable variability in health outcomes. Bereaved children are at risk of lower lifelong educational attainment, though the contribution of neurocognitive performance is yet to be investigated. Using data from the population-based Generation R Study wherein nearly 10,000 pregnant mothers were recruited between 2002 and 2006, we estimated the association of bereavement by age 10 years with four subtests of cognitive function and academic achievement at 13 years. Bereavement by 10 years of age was experienced by n = 796 (37.3%) of youth and was associated with a 1.12-point (SD = 0.55; p-value = 0.04) lower full-scale IQ at 13 years, which was mainly driven by lower matrix reasoning scores (βadjusted = −0.27, SE = 0.11, p < 0.02). There were no differences in academic achievement or other subtests of neurocognitive function between bereaved and non-bereaved children. Secondary analyses adjusting for pre-bereavement nonverbal cognitive ability and mental health problems revealed an overall association between bereavement and subsequent full-scale IQ and matrix reasoning, but only among those youth whose caregivers reported that the loss had an emotional influence on the child. These novel findings leveraging prospective assessments in a population-based birth cohort highlight risk and resilience mechanisms warranting further research.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details


1 Erasmus Medical Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychology, Rotterdam, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.5645.2) (ISNI:0000 0004 0459 992X)
2 Harvard University, Department of Epidemiology, T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, USA (GRID:grid.38142.3c) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 754X)
3 Erasmus Medical Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychology, Rotterdam, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.5645.2) (ISNI:0000 0004 0459 992X); Erasmus University, Department of Psychology, Education and Child Studies, Rotterdam, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.6906.9) (ISNI:0000 0000 9262 1349)
4 Erasmus Medical Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychology, Rotterdam, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.5645.2) (ISNI:0000 0004 0459 992X); Harvard University, Department of Epidemiology, T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, USA (GRID:grid.38142.3c) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 754X); Harvard University, Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, USA (GRID:grid.38142.3c) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 754X)
5 Harvard University, Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, USA (GRID:grid.38142.3c) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 754X)