Introduction
In many socially monogamous bird species with biparental care, occasional social polygyny has been detected (Cockburn 2006; Lack 1968). Polygyny can occur opportunistically, for example if a pair is joined by a female that recently lost its mate (take-over polygyny). Alternatively, social polygyny can be a strategy when males invest in attracting additional mates, for example by defending another territory (Slagsvold et al. 1988). In case of facultative-polygynous species with biparental care, a conflict of interest may exist between the sexes because males may increase their fitness by attracting additional females (Arnqvist and Rowe 2005) while polygyny can be costly to females if male assistance is reduced (Lubjuhn et al. 2000; Moreno et al. 2002). However, due to paternity loss, socially polygynous males do not always have higher reproductive success (Schlicht and Kempenaers 2021).
Prospecting females may choose a partner based on several factors, including variation in male or territory quality, prior experience with the male (Kempenaers 1994), the availability of nest sites, or the local availability of unpaired males (i.e., the local sex ratio) (Canal et al. 2021). In migratory species, late-arriving females may have less time to assess local male quality or to find the few remaining unpaired males (Slagsvold et al. 1988; Sandell 1998). Females may also be deceived by males if they are unaware that the courting male is already mated (deception hypothesis; Alatalo et al. 1981). Alternatively, females may deliberately select an already mated male when its territory or individual quality compensates for the expected costs of the reduction in male parental care (polygyny threshold model; Orians 1969) or the future attractiveness of their young exceeds the costs in terms of other fitness components (sexy son hypothesis; Weatherhead and Robertson 1979).
The occurrence of facultative social polygyny in otherwise socially monogamous species is likely explained by case-specific circumstances and decisions made by the involved individuals (Walker and Marzluff 2017). Here, we describe a case of facultative polygyny in the European Nightjar Caprimulgus europaeus (hereafter nightjar) that was discovered based on GPS-data and accelerometer data. Nightjars belong to the familiy Caprimulgidae, comprising approximately 97 species whose breeding ecology is generally poorly understood. Although for many species the available information is limited, most species are presumably socially monogamous with biparental care. Only three species exhibit extreme plumage dimorphism (i.e., male ornaments, such as elongated feathers that are not present in females) and male emancipation from parental care, suggesting a socially polygynous mating system (Standard-winged Nightjar Caprimulgus longipennis, Pennant-winged Nightjar Caprimulgus vexillarius and White-winged Nightjar Eleothreptus candicans) (Pople 2014; Holyoak 2001).
The nightjar is a long-distance migratory species and is considered socially monogamous. As has been reported in the related Red-necked Nightjar Caprimulgus ruficollis (Sàez and Camacho 2016), males perform mate guarding in the days and nights preceding egg laying, that is, they spend much of their time close to their female partner (R. Evens and M. Lathouwers, personal observations), presumably to reduce the risk of paternity loss. Most males participate in incubation, brooding and feeding offspring and they may have two clutches per season. The male then often cares alone for the chicks from the first brood after they are 10 days old, while the female initiates the second clutch (Holyoak 2001). In such presumed socially monogamous pairs with two broods, additional ‘extra-pair’ males have been observed that assist the female in raising the second brood (Cresswell and Alexander 1990; Padget et al. 2019). It is unknown whether these ‘extra-pair’ males also sired offspring in that brood, or whether they formed a social bond with the female. Mate switching may also occur both between broods within a single season and between seasons (Cresswell and Alexander 1990). A single case of polyterritorial polygyny has previously been described (Odder Jensen 2013). This study reported on a radio-tagged male nightjar that defended two territories approximately 5 km apart, but did not determine the location of the nests. During the breeding season, the male visited each of his territories with a 1-day interval. In July, the male remained in his presumed primary territory for 3 weeks after which he was observed flying with a female and two recently fledged juveniles. In his presumed secondary territory, he was observed flying and communicating with a female only on a few occasions.
Our aim here is twofold. First, we provide information about a case of facultative polygyny in a single male territory to identify factors that may contribute to individual variation in mating decisions in the nightjar. Second, we describe how the polygynous male allocated parental care to the two broods.
Methods and Results
Field Observations
On 29 June 2022, we searched for nightjar nests in a ~5 ha heathland in Grenspark Kalmthout (51.38° N, 4.42° E; Belgium) that consists of typical breeding habitat comprising open heathland with sparse ±3 m high trees (Evens et al. 2017). The site held two or three territories in previous years. We discovered two nests that were 150 m apart (Figure 1), a typical distance between neighbouring territories (R. Evens and M. Lathouwers, personal observations). We therefore assumed that the two nests were from two pairs occupying neighbouring territories, also because each nest was close to a nesting location from previous years. The eastern and western nest both contained two eggs that hatched on 6 and 11 July, respectively.
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
On the same day, starting 1 h before dusk, we attempted to capture the presumed nightjar pair at the western nest (Figure 1) by placing two ultra-fine mist nets (Ecotone, 15 × 3 m) parallel to each other left and right of the nest. This set-up avoids disturbing the incubating female and enables to capture both the female when she leaves the nest at dusk, and the territorial male when he arrives to relieve the female from the nest. We failed to capture the female and we did not detect a male that attended this nest.
On 4 July 2022, we attempted to capture the presumed pairs at both nests, using the same method. Around dusk, we captured the pair of the eastern nest: the adult (> 2CY) male I96 and the adult female I95, who already bred as a pair in the same territory since 2018 (both individuals were captured on a nest in the same territory in 2018, 2019 and 2022). Around the same time, we captured the yearling (2CY) female M042 at the western nest. Again, we did not detect a male at the western nest. Similarly, during a third capture attempt at that nest on 8 July 2022, we did not observe a male at the nest.
On 20 July 2022, we visited both nests and found them predated. At the eastern nest, we recovered the GPS logger of female I95. At the western nest, we recovered the GPS logger of female M042 and the remains of male I96, including its logger (Figure 2). We also recovered two body feathers of an Eagle Owl (Bubo bubo), the presumed predator.
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
Information From Data Loggers
All three individuals had been fitted with a custom-designed combination of an activity logger (1.4 g Technosmart Axy5), a radio tag (0.4 g; Biotrack Ltd.) and a GPS logger (1.8 g; Pathtrack Ltd.), attached to the base of the tail with a simple ‘drop-off’ mechanism (Evens et al. 2018). The combined tag weighed approximately 5% of the body mass of the tagged birds (male I96: 69 g, female I95: 74 g, female M042: 83 g). We programmed the GPS-loggers to fix positions at 3-min intervals from before sunset (9 PM, UTC + 2) until after sunrise (6 AM, UTC + 2). To obtain synchronous activity data, we programmed activity loggers to measure acceleration continuously at 25 Hz (g, in the X-, Y- and Z-axis) within the same timeframe. This allowed the logging of the individuals' activity for approximately 100 h. For further analysis, we calculated dynamic body acceleration by smoothing the activity data using a running mean for 2-s intervals and subtracting the smoothed data from the unsmoothed data to remove the static acceleration (i.e., acceleration resulting from the body angle with respect to the earth's gravity). The loggers of female I95 and M042 contained three nights of relevant GPS data, but due to a software malfunctioning we could not use the accelerometer data of these two loggers. The logger of male I96 contained 11 nights of GPS data and 10 days of accelerometer data.
We estimated the investment of the male in parental care at the two nests as follows. First, we used the GPS-data to define periods when male I96 was near (between 0 and 25 m, taking possible GPS-error into account; Evens et al. 2018) each of the nests (Figure 1). Second, we used the accelerometer data to determine periods of inactivity (i.e., not flying or singing), assuming that the male was either incubating the eggs or brooding the chicks (Figure 3). Note that with this procedure we might have somewhat underestimated the true presence at the nests, because nest visits shorter than the 3-min GPS sampling interval were not included. Third, we used the accelerometer data to quantify the song activity of the male (Eisenring et al. 2022) to determine where the male sang with respect to the location of the two nest sites (Figure 4A).
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
The GPS and accelerometer data showed that male I96 only sang in proximity of the eastern nest, reflecting the presumed song posts from the previous year. However, the male visited the nests of both females during the 10-day tracking period, although the frequency and duration differed between nests (Figures 1 and 3; Table 1). The number of visits to the eastern nest (female I95; n = 34) was higher than the number of visits to the western nest (female M042; n = 19). Moreover, male visits to the eastern nest lasted longer (paired t-test: t = 2.73, p = 0.009; I95: 30 min ± 1 min (mean ± SE); M042: 19 min ± 24 s). During the first six tracking nights (4–9 July; Figure 3) the accelerometer data suggest that the male made short visits at dusk and dawn to the chicks of I95, and brooded the chicks for longer periods at night. During the same period, the male only visited the nest of M042 once per night (n = 3 nights) or not at all (n = 3 nights). The acceleration data suggest that the male incubated the eggs at the nest of M042 twice during this period (on 5 and 6 July 2022; Figure 3). On 11 July 2022, the number of short dusk visits to M042 increased (Figure 3), and on 12 and 13 July 2022, the male also showed prolonged nocturnal visits at the western nest, suggesting that he started feeding and brooding the recently hatched chicks at this nest too. The time spent at the eastern nest, where offspring were approximately 6 days old, decreased during this period.
TABLE 1 Overview of data on male I96's nest attendance at the nest of primary female I95 and at the nest of secondary female M042.
Nest I95 | Nest M042 | ||||||||
Day | Visit | Start | Stop | Duration | Day | Visit | Start | Stop | Duration |
05-07-22 | 1 | 2:19 | 2:26 | 0:03 | 05-07-22 | 1 | 2:28 | 2:44 | 0:16 |
05-07-22 | 1 | 20:52 | 21:13 | 0:21 | 05-07-22 | 1 | 20:40 | 20:46 | 0:06 |
05-07-22 | 2 | 21:25 | 21:49 | 0:24 | |||||
05-07-22 | 3 | 22:43 | 23:22 | 0:39 | |||||
06-07-22 | 4 | 2:16 | 2:49 | 0:33 | |||||
06-07-22 | 1 | 20:32 | 20:35 | 0:03 | 07-07-22 | 1 | 1:22 | 1:43 | 0:21 |
06-07-22 | 2 | 20:40 | 20:41 | 0:01 | |||||
06-07-22 | 3 | 21:18 | 21:29 | 0:11 | |||||
06-07-22 | 4 | 22:05 | 23:21 | 1:16 | |||||
07-07-22 | 5 | 2:03 | 2:25 | 0:22 | |||||
07-07-22 | 6 | 2:58 | 3:08 | 0:10 | |||||
07-07-22 | 1 | 20:40 | 20:41 | 0:01 | |||||
07-07-22 | 2 | 20:49 | 20:50 | 0:01 | |||||
07-07-22 | 3 | 20:58 | 21:22 | 0:24 | |||||
08-07-22 | 4 | 22:01 | 22:10 | 0:09 | |||||
08-07-22 | 5 | 0:40 | 1:52 | 1:12 | |||||
08-07-22 | 6 | 2:37 | 2:40 | 0:03 | |||||
08-07-22 | 1 | 20:46 | 20:56 | 0:10 | |||||
08-07-22 | 2 | 21:13 | 21:16 | 0:03 | |||||
08-07-22 | 3 | 21:31 | 22:10 | 0:39 | |||||
08-07-22 | 4 | 22:50 | 23:37 | 0:47 | |||||
09-07-22 | 5 | 1:14 | 2:12 | 0:58 | |||||
09-07-22 | 1 | 21:25 | 22:22 | 0:57 | |||||
09-07-22 | 2 | 23:34 | 1:40 | 2:06 | |||||
09-07-22 | 3 | 2:43 | 2:44 | 0:01 | |||||
10-07-22 | 1 | 21:19 | 21:20 | 0:01 | 10-07-22 | 1 | 20:46 | 20:58 | 0:12 |
11-07-22 | 2 | 0:01 | 0:40 | 0:39 | 10-07-22 | 2 | 22:04 | 22:46 | 0:42 |
11-07-22 | 1 | 22:10 | 23:20 | 1:10 | 11-07-22 | 1 | 20:28 | 20:29 | 0:01 |
12-07-22 | 2 | 0:07 | 1:40 | 1:33 | 11-07-22 | 2 | 20:37 | 20:38 | 0:01 |
12-07-22 | 3 | 2:37 | 2:43 | 0:06 | |||||
12-07-22 | 1 | 20:37 | 20:38 | 0:01 | 12-07-22 | 1 | 20:46 | 20:52 | 0:06 |
12-07-22 | 2 | 22:55 | 23:07 | 0:12 | 12-07-22 | 2 | 21:16 | 21:58 | 0:42 |
13-07-22 | 3 | 2:10 | 2:25 | 0:15 | 12-07-22 | 3 | 23:43 | 0:46 | 1:03 |
13-07-22 | 1 | 22:22 | 23:40 | 1:18 | 13-07-22 | 1 | 20:25 | 20:26 | 0:01 |
13-07-22 | 2 | 21:01 | 21:04 | 0:03 | |||||
14-07-22 | 3 | 0:43 | 1:39 | 0:56 | |||||
14-07-22 | 4 | 2:10 | 2:25 | 0:15 | |||||
15-07-22 | 1 | 0:13 | 0:49 | 0:36 | 14-07-22 | 1 | 20:46 | 20:55 | 0:09 |
14-07-22 | 2 | 21:34 | 22:22 | 0:48 | |||||
15-07-22 | 1 | 19:58 | 20:07 | 0:09 | |||||
15-07-22 | 2 | 20:22 | Pred |
During the few days, we obtained synchronous GPS-data of the male and the two females, we observed that male I96 attended both nests when the respective female departed on a foraging flight. For example, on 4 July 2022 (partial data due to deployment of the logger), male I96 incubated the eggs of the eastern nest (female I95) until she returned from foraging. Afterwards, he immediately flew towards the western nest (female M042), where he relieved the female from incubating. He did not wait for M042 to return from foraging, but left the eggs 10 min before her return. During the second night (5 July; Figure 3), the male first visited the western nest, but M042 had already left the nest 10 min earlier. The male incubated for only 10 min and it took another 45 min before M042 returned. After his departure at the nest of M042, the male immediately flew towards the eastern nest where he relieved female I95. In the next hours, the male and his primary female relieved each other three times from brooding freshly hatched chicks, such that they were never left unattended. On the third night (6 July) male I96 relieved female M042 from incubating once and did not leave the eggs unattended (no data from female I95 were available for that night).
Discussion
Using GPS-data and accelerometer data, we provide first insights into the division of parental care activities of a male nightjar (I96) that formed a pair with two females whose nests were approximately 150 m apart. The two clutches were started approximately 1 week apart. Compared to a previous report on social polygyny, the two broods of this polygynous male were further apart in distance (±40 m) (Cleere and Nurney 1998), but closer in time (±14 days) (Cleere and Nurney 1998). Based on the difference in laying date, the difference in effort invested by the male in both nests, the fact that female I95 already bred with the male since 2018 and that he only sang in proximity of the nest of I95, we conclude that I95 was the primary female, and that the inexperienced, yearling female M042 was the secondary female of this polygynous trio.
Our data suggest that the male assisted the females during incubation, as well as during the nestling period (presumably by feeding and brooding the chicks). Typically, when the male came to the nest, the primary and/or secondary female went to the foraging areas. Our data further show that the secondary female received less assistance during incubation than the primary female and that her eggs were often left unattended. Such reduced assistance during incubation has also been observed in other socially polygynous species with biparental care (e.g., Lundberg and Alatalo 1992; Pinxten and Eens 1994). Thus, the eggs of the unattended nest need to be rewarmed more frequently, presumably increasing the metabolic costs to the secondary female (Biebach 1979). Once the chicks of the secondary female hatched, male assistance suddenly increased, presumably at a cost to the primary female. Our data only provide information on presence at the nest during part of the nestling period, so we cannot assess whether the male reduced the feeding rate at the primary brood in favour of the secondary brood (Lifjeld and Slagsvold 1991), although this is to be expected (Pinxten and Eens 1994).
In most facultative polygynous species with male parental care, the male predominantly or exclusively assists the primary female (Huk and Winkel 2006). Previous studies on several polygynous species showed that the division of male care depends on the difference in the timing of the two nests, with an increase in male care at the secondary nest when the hatching interval between the primary and secondary nests becomes smaller (e.g., Kempenaers, Verheyen, and Dhondt 1995). Here, the hatching interval was approximately 7 days and the male contributed to both nests, which fits with observations on other species.
This is only the second report on social polygyny in the European Nightjar. Because of the low detectability of nests and the general absence of population-wide paternity studies, the frequency of facultative social polygyny might be underestimated in this species. Although we do not have direct observational data that may shed light on why this polygynous event occurred, we briefly discuss several non-mutually exclusive explanations.
First, the area where both nests were found usually contains two or three territories of socially monogamous pairs. Yet, male density at the study site was lower than that observed in previous seasons. In 2022, the one male that was present might have had a larger territory at his disposal (Walseng et al. 2022). Alternatively, the male might have become polygynous after taking over a female that was initially paired to a neighbouring territory holder that then died (e.g., Schlicht and Kempenaers 2021). This hypothesis is supported by the absence of any song activity of the male near the secondary nest. Depending on when the take-over occurred (if it did) relative to the female fertile period, the initial male might have sired at least one or all offspring in the brood. Unfortunately, the nest was predated before we could obtain DNA samples from the nestlings to investigate paternity.
Second, this case of facultative social polygyny may have arisen because an inexperienced female mated with an already paired male, either because she was initially not aware of the mating status of the male (e.g., if he displayed away from his primary female) or because the female could not find an unpaired male or because mating with this paired male was a better option than mating with another unpaired male (compensation for reduced care via high male quality or high territory quality; she shared the same foraging site with the male, Figure 4B). The secondary female was a second-calendar year bird, and young birds usually arrive later at the breeding sites than older, more experienced adults (R. Evens and M. Lathouwers, personal observation). Late-arriving inexperienced females are under time constraints to mate and may thus more easily be deceived by a male courting away from the primary female, or may have difficulties finding an unpaired male.
In summary, we described a case of facultative polygyny and quantified the time allocation (as a proxy for parental investment) by a polygynous male nightjar. The breeding ecology and mating behaviour of this crepuscular bird species remains little understood. Future studies, using a variety of methods including parentage analysis, will help shed light on the prevalence of social polygyny in nightjars, which is essential to make accurate population estimates, and on the causes and consequences of this mating system.
Author Contributions
Ruben Evens: conceptualization (lead), data curation (equal), formal analysis (equal), investigation (equal), methodology (lead), supervision (equal), writing – original draft (lead), writing – review and editing (equal). Michiel Lathouwers: conceptualization (equal), data curation (equal), methodology (equal), validation (equal), writing – review and editing (equal). Jitse Creemers: conceptualization (equal), methodology (equal), visualization (equal), writing – review and editing (equal). Eddy Ulenaers: conceptualization (equal), resources (equal), writing – review and editing (equal). Marcel Eens: conceptualization (equal), funding acquisition (equal), supervision (equal), writing – review and editing (equal). Bart Kempenaers: conceptualization (equal), methodology (equal), resources (equal), validation (equal), writing – review and editing (lead).
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Marc Evens, Annie Loenders, Käthe Robert and Gunter Flipkens for help during fieldwork, and Koen Thijs, Jef De Winter, Lucas Bergmans, Joris Elst, Annie Loenders, Karen Vanmarcke and Fien and Fleur Evens for support. Permissions for the study were granted by the Belgian military, the Agency for Nature and Forest and the Royal Belgian Institute for Natural Sciences. In loving memory of R. Pinxten. Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Data Availability Statement
All data and code are available from the OSF Repository at: .
Alatalo, R. V., A. Carlson, A. Lundberg, and S. Ulfstrand. 1981. “The Conflict Between Male Polygamy and Female Monogamy: The Case of the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca.” American Naturalist 117: 738–753.
Arnqvist, G., and L. Rowe. 2005. Sexual Conflict. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Biebach, H. 1979. “Energetik des Brütens Beim Star (Sturnus vulgaris).” Journal für Ornithologie 120: 121–138.
Canal, D., L. Schlicht, S. Santoro, C. Camacho, J. Martínez‐Padilla, and J. Potti. 2021. “Phenology‐Mediated Effects of Phenotype on the Probability of Social Polygyny and Its Fitness Consequences in a Migratory Passerine.” BMC Ecology and Evolution 21: 55.
Cleere, N., and D. Nurney. 1998. Nightjars. A Guide to Nightjars and Related Nightbirds. Robertsbridge, UK: Pica.
Cockburn, A. 2006. “Prevalence of Different Modes of Parental Care in Birds.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273: 1375–1383.
Cresswell, B., and I. Alexander. 1990. “A Case of Mate‐Switching Between Broods in the Nightjar.” Ringing & Migration 11: 73–75.
Eisenring, E., M. Eens, J. N. Pradervand, et al. 2022. “Quantifying Song Behavior in a Free‐Living, Light‐Weight, Mobile Bird Using Accelerometers.” Ecology and Evolution 12: [eLocator: e8446].
Evens, R., N. Beenaerts, E. Ulenaers, N. Witters, and T. Artois. 2018. “An Effective, Low‐Tech Drop‐Off Solution to Facilitate the Retrieval of Data Loggers in Animal‐Tracking Studies.” Ringing & Migration 33: 10–18.
Evens, R., N. Beenaerts, N. Witters, and T. Artois. 2017. “Study on the Foraging Behaviour of the European Nightjar Caprimulgus europaeus Reveals the Need for a Change in Conservation Strategy in Belgium.” Journal of Avian Biology 48: 1238–1245.
Holyoak, D. T. 2001. Nightjars and Their Allies: The Caprimulgiformes. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Huk, T., and W. Winkel. 2006. “Polygyny and Its Fitness Consequences for Primary and Secondary Female Pied Flycatchers.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273: 1681–1688.
Kempenaers, B. 1994. “Polygyny in the Blue Tit: Unbalanced Sex Ratio and Female Aggression Restrict Mate Choice.” Animal Behaviour 47: 943–957.
Kempenaers, B., G. R. Verheyen, and A. A. Dhondt. 1995. “Mate Guarding and Copulation Behaviour in Monogamous and Polygynous Blue Tits: Do Males Follow a Best‐Of‐a‐Bad‐Job Strategy?” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 36: 33–42.
Lack, D. 1968. Ecological Adaptations for Breeding in Birds. London, UK: Methuen.
Lifjeld, J. T., and T. Slagsvold. 1991. “Sexual Conflict Among Polygynous Pied Flycatchers Feeding Young.” Behavioral Ecology 2: 106–115.
Lubjuhn, T., W. Winkel, J. T. Epplen, and J. Brün. 2000. “Reproductive Success of Monogamous and Polygynous Pied Flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca).” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 48: 12–17.
Lundberg, A., and R. V. Alatalo. 1992. “The Pied Flycatcher.” 267.
Moreno, J., J. P. Veiga, M. Romasanta, and S. Sánchez. 2002. “Effects of Maternal Quality and Mating Status on Female Reproductive Success in the Polygynous Spotless Starling.” Animal Behaviour 64: 197–206.
Odder Jensen, N. 2013. “A Case of Polyterritorial Polygyny in European Nightjar Caprimulgus europaeus.” Dansk Ornitologisk Forenings Tidsskrift 107: 239–244.
Orians, G. H. 1969. “On the Evolution of Mating Systems in Birds and Mammals.” [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1086/282628103589-603].
Padget, O., J. Morford, A. Ward, et al. 2019. “Observation of Nest Visitation by Multiple Males in the European Nightjar Caprimulgus europeaus.” Bird Study 66: 423–424.
Pinxten, R., and M. Eens. 1994. “Male Feeding of Nestlings in the Facultatively Polygynous European Starling: Allocation Patterns and Effect on Female Reproductive Success.” Behaviour 129: 113–140.
Pople, R. G. 2014. “Breeding Biology of the White‐Winged Nightjar (Eleothreptus candicans) in Eastern Paraguay.” Revista Brasileira de Ornitologia 22: 219–233.
Sàez, P., and C. Camacho. 2016. “Chotacabras cuellirrojo – Caprimulgus ruficollis.” In Enciclopedia Virtual de los Vertebrados Españoles, edited by A. Salvador and M. B. Morales. Madrid, Spain: Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales.
Sandell, M. I. 1998. “Female Aggression and the Maintenance of Monogamy: Female Behaviour Predicts Male Mating Status in European Starlings.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 265: 1307–1311.
Schlicht, E., and B. Kempenaers. 2021. “Origin and Outcome of Social Polygyny in the Blue Tit.” Ardea 109: 91–118.
Slagsvold, T., J. T. Lifjeld, G. Stenmark, and T. Breiehagen. 1988. “On the Cost of Searching for a Mate in Female Pied Flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca.” Animal Behaviour 36: 433–442.
Walker, L. E., and J. M. Marzluff. 2017. “Simultaneous Polygyny by a Male Black‐Capped Vireo (Vireo atricapilla) in Central Texas.” Wilson Journal Of Ornithology 129: 212–215.
Walseng, B., J. M. Durant, D. O. Hessen, et al. 2022. “Long‐Term Annual and Spatial Variation of Polygyny in the White‐Throated Dipper (Cinclus cinclus).” Ecology and Evolution 12: [eLocator: e9416].
Weatherhead, P. J., and R. J. Robertson. 1979. “Offspring Quality and the Polygyny Threshold: “The Sexy Son Hypothesis”.” American Naturalist 113: 201–208.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2024. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the "License"). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
ABSTRACT
In many socially monogamous bird species with biparental care, occasional social polygyny has been detected. We provide information about a case of facultative polygyny in the European Nightjar (
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details

1 Department of Biology, Behavioural Ecology and Ecophysiology Group, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium, Department of Ornithology, Max Planck Institute for Biological Intelligence, Eberhard‐Gwinner‐Straße, Germany, Earth and Life Institute, Terrestrial Ecology and Biodiversity Conservation Group, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain‐la‐Neuve, Belgium
2 Centre for Environmental Sciences, Research Group Zoology, Biodiversity and Toxicology, Hasselt University, Diepenbeek, Belgium, Department of Geography, University of Namur, Namur, Belgium
3 Department of Biology, Behavioural Ecology and Ecophysiology Group, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
4 Agentschap Natuur en Bos, Regio Noord‐Limburg, Brussels, Belgium
5 Department of Ornithology, Max Planck Institute for Biological Intelligence, Eberhard‐Gwinner‐Straße, Germany