Introduction
Parental reflective functioning and its relevance
Parental Reflective Functioning (PRF) relates to the parents’ capacity to see their child as an intentional agent motivated by mental states, as well as the capacity to recognize how their own mental states affect their child [1]. As such, PRF was described as an application of mentalizing, namely the capacity to consider both others’ (other-mentalizing) and one’s own intentional mental states (self-mentalizing) [2]. PRF, as measured by the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ) [3], is comprised of three dimensions. First, the Certainty about Mental States (CMS) captures the parents’ awareness that mentalizing is inherently inferential and comes with some uncertainty. Second, the Interest and Curiosity about Mental States (IC) represents an attitude associated with more tolerance towards infants’ distress [4,5] and better regulation of the parents´ own mental states [3]. Third, Pre-mentalizing (PM) aims to identify non-mentalizing modes, which would inhibit parents from entering the subjective world of their children [4] and can lead to misattributions of their children’s actions [6].
PRF has been implicated in other relevant constructs such as children’s development of Theory of Mind [7], epistemic trust [8], and attachment security [for an overview see 9]. For instance, Meins et al. [10] found PRF to predict child attachment in the Strange Situation Test. Overall, high PRF seems to increase the likelihood of secure attachment in children [11] and can act as a protective factor for development in children with early adversities [12]. On the mother’s side, low PRF increases occurrences of maladapted maternal behavior [13]. This connection between PRF and attachment has been dubbed "loose coupling", describing the idea that secure attachment and emotional availability are not always, but often [14], associated with high PRF [15]. In practice, indicators of functional PRF like IC and CMS are less often related to secure attachment than indicators of dysfunctional PRF, such as PM [3], which represents one of the main reasons to include the PM dimension in the PRFQ. One explanation for this well-documented connection between PRF and attachment security might be how PRF changes the way parents interact with their children. PRF leads to more parental sensitivity towards the child and also towards distress displayed by the child [16,17]. This, in turn, is an important predictor of the emergence of secure attachment [18].
The measurement of PRF
Historically, the majority of available measures for PRF and Mentalizing were interviews [19], such as a specialized version of the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) or the Parent Development Interview (PDI). While these measures allow for very detailed information and data analysis, their time-intensive nature makes them unsuitable for large-scale assessments [3]. As such, the PRFQ is one of the first economic self-report scales of PRF [3]. The PRFQ, as developed and validated by Luyten et al. [3], is an 18-item self-report scale. It consists of the three subscales CMS, IC, and PM each containing six items measured on a 7-point Likert scale.
While there has been some uncertainty regarding the factor structure of the PRFQ, Luyten et al. [3] proposed that their 18-item questionnaire comprises three factors, of which only CMS and IC are correlated. In their second study [3], this correlation was no longer significant. The model also contains several unspecified error covariances. To date, the PRFQ has been validated in various countries, including Portugal, Canada, South Korea, China, Italy, Hungary, Finland, and Iran [20–28]. Many of these studies found deviating factor structures, with factor counts ranging from two to five, and varying factor correlations, and as many as six items being removed. A more detailed discussion of these differences can be found in the online supplementary material.
The current study
The present study has two goals: First, we aim to replicate the factor model of the original PRFQ. Second, we aim to validate the German version of the PRFQ. This German translation has been used first by Krink et al. [17]. After translating the PRFQ into German backtranslation was conducted by an English native speaker to ensure face validity. However, Krink et al. [17] only assessed the reliability as measured by Cronbach’s α and, until today, this German adaptation has not been validated further [29]. While some publications have used the validation of the original PRFQ to demonstrate sufficient construct validity for the German translation [e.g., 30], this is inadvisable since it needs to be assured that a translation is not only verbally correct but also transmits the intended meaning, tone, and cultural differences [31]. To close this research gap, we aim to provide a full validation of the German translation of the PRFQ.
Hypotheses
The following hypotheses have been formulated based on previous research by Campbell et al. [32] and Luyten et al. [3].
Factor structure
1. H1: We assume a replication of the model of Luyten et al. [3]–Study 1. The original model provides a good model fit (H1.1) and has the same significant factors and loadings as Luyten et al. [3] (H1.2).
Reliability
1. H2: All scales and subscales prove a reliable measure as indicated by both a Cronbach’s α and a McDonald’s ω value of > .70.
Concurrent validity
To test the concurrent validity, a total of 30 individual hypotheses were preregistered, which can be found in the online supplement (for an overview see Supplementary Table 1 in S1 Table).
1. H3: We expect CMS, IC, and PM to be significantly correlated to relationship satisfaction.
We expect CMS to be correlated with the Epistemic Trust Mistrust and Credulity Questionnaire (ETMCQ). We expect PM to be significantly correlated to the ETMCQ, perceived stress, postpartum depressiveness, education, and changes in the working environment due to COVID-19.
Method
Ethics statement
The study had been approved by the independent ethics committee of the medical faculty, Ruprecht-Karls-Universität, Heidelberg, in agreement with the ethics committee of the Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich (vote: S-446/2017) and the declaration of Helsinki 2013, seventh revision.
Participants & procedure
The sample for this study is part of a longitudinal project called CoviFam, as part of a larger longitudinal, observational project called COMPARE [33]. In the CoviFam project, we investigate the well-being in a community sample of young families in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. The initial recruiting for the CoviFam project took place from 06/05/2020 to 20/11/2020.
All data used in this study [34] stems from the third measuring time point, since previous timepoints did not contain the PRFQ. The used data was collected from the 23/02/2022 to the 06/05/2022. Participants were informed about the content of the study as well as their right to withdraw at any time. Afterwards, they gave their written, informed consent prior to completing an online questionnaire at a time and place of their choice.
The target population were mothers of children aged 0 to 3 years at the beginning of the longitudinal study, which were recruited for the study by various means such as social media channels (i.e., X/Twitter, Facebook and Instagram posts), medical institutions and professionals, and professional support organizations. We estimate that 53% of all participants have been made aware of the study via social media and the remaining 47% by word of mouth or flyers at health care facilities. Recruitment took place in Germany, Austria, and Switzerland. Next to country of residence and children’s age the only other inclusion criterion was sufficient German language skills. The final size of this community sample was N = 378, after 11 cases had been removed due to missing data on the “Partnerschaftsfragebogen kurz” (Couples questionnaire short) or IDs entered erroneously by the mothers, following the specification of listwise deletion in our pre-registration. Another 40 participants were removed for indicating a gender other than female, to stick with the parameters used by Luyten et al. [3]. Thus, our sample missingness rate was 11 / (378 + 11 + 40) = 2.6%.
The data was collected before this validation was conceived, therefore followed no relevant stopping rules. However, we ensured that the sample size was sufficient by adhering to the convention of 10 participants per item [35] for both the training and validation dataset (10 x 18 x 2 = 360 < 378). The mothers’ mean age was M = 35.79 (SD = 4.37, range = 19–50 years). The children’s mean age was M = 43.44 months (SD = 13.32, range = 10.2–78.6). We included indicators of child wellbeing in the form of problems with crying, sleeping, and eating alongside the other sociodemographics in Table 1.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Materials
The study consisted of an extensive collection of online-based self-report questionnaires. Means and standard deviations for all measures can be found in Supplementary Table 8 in S3 Table. Additionally, the following additional demographic variables were obtained: Children’s and parents’ age, the level of secondary education achieved, and if or if not the parents’ working environment had changed due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Most of our data stems from items measured on Likert scales, which are technically ordinally scaled. However, these ordinal scales may still be used for parametric tests [36] with little difference to nonparametric testing [37–40]. As such, we treated all Likert-scale questionnaires as interval scaled.
Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire.
Parental Reflective Functioning was measured via the PRFQ by Luyten et al. [3]. The original PRFQ is an 18-item 3-factor self-report questionnaire using a 7-point Likert scale. The items of each subscale are averaged, yielding three mean values for each participant. The items used in the validation by Luyten et al. [3] as well as their translations for the present study can be found in Supplementary Tables 2–4 in S2 Table. In the original version, the reliability of PM was α = .70, for CMS α = .82, and for IC α = .75.
Epistemic trust, mistrust, and credulity questionnaire.
The Epistemic Trust, Mistrust, and Credulity Questionnaire (ETMCQ) is a 18 item, three factor self-report questionnaire measuring epistemic trust on a 7-point Likert scale on three separate subscales for each participant [32]. As of yet, no validation of the German translation has been published. Reliability of the English version was α = .76–.81 for Trust, α = .65–.72 for Mistrust, and 75–.81 for Credulity. All items can be found in the Supplementary Table 5 in S2 Table.
Perceived stress scale.
The German version of the Perceived Stress Scale [41] was used to measure stress. It is a 10 item, one-factor self-report questionnaire used on a 5-point Likert scale. After reverse coding, all items are averaged. Reliability has been reported at ω = .89. All items can be found in Supplementary Table 6 in S2 Table.
Edinburgh postnatal depression scale.
The German version [42] of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) [43] was employed to measure depressiveness. While different factor structures have been used in the past, we opted for the 10-item, one-factor solution, measured on a 4-point Likert Scale. Values were simply reverse coded where necessary and then averaged. Reliability is α = .87. All items can be found in Supplementary Table 7 in S2 Table.
Partnerschaftsfragebogen Kurz.
Lastly, the Partnerschaftsfragebogen Kurz (PFB-K) [44] was used to measure couple relationship satisfaction. The PFB-K consists of 10 Items, 9 of which measure on a 4-point Likert scale and the 10th item measuring on a 6-point Likert scale. All 9 items are averaged to a single value of overall satisfaction with the relationship. Reliability has been calculated to be between α = .85 and .91 [45]. The items cannot be shared due to copyright.
Analysis
Our approach for data analysis was to establish a factor structure via confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), and assess reliability, measurement fit, and concurrent validity. The standard p < .05 criterion was used to determine if factor loadings and correlations were significant. No further corrections were applied as all analyses contain different combinations of variables and a robust estimator was used. No transformations or item parceling was applied.
All data analyses were carried out in R Studio (version 4.3.3) [46], utilizing the following packages: lavaan (version 0.6–16) [47], tidyverse (version 2.0.0) [48], eeptools (version 1.2.5) [49], lubridate (version 1.9.2) [50], psych (version 2.3.6) [51], naniar (version 1.0.0) [52], coefficientalpha (version 0.7.2) [53], Hmisc (version 5.1–0) [54], moments (version 0.14.1) [55], and e1071 (version 1.7–13) [56]. All packages were updated on the 20/08/2024. The study followed the recommendations of Jackson et al. [57] for the application of CFA. Little’s MCAR test was used to inspect whether data was missing completely at random (MCAR). In our pre-registration, we opted against imputation and for listwise deletion in the case of values MCAR, since we wanted to stick with genuine measurements.
A CFA was run with the original factor model of the PRFQ (Study 1) [3]. To assess model fit, the recommendations of Hu and Bentler [58] and Jackson et al. [57] were followed. We reported the χ2 value despite its tendency to be overly sensitive to sample size [59] but relied on the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) to determine model fit. The cut-off values for good model fit were CFI > .95, TLI > .95, SRMR < .08, and RMSEA < .06 [58]. A covariance matrix was chosen as input matrix and a robust maximum likelihood (MLR) estimator was used.
For the resulting model, reliability was assessed using McDonald‘s Omega (ω) [60] and Cronbach’s Alpha (α) [61]. Values of > .70 were seen as indicative of a reliable scale [62].
The skewness, kurtosis and distribution were assessed. A parallel analysis using a 95th percentile criterion was run to inspect the suggested number of factors [63,64]. An exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with a maximum likelihood estimator, promax rotation, and the number of factors as suggested by parallel analysis was used to inspect item loadings. The subscale assignment for each item remained as in the original version, since we wanted to stick to the theoretically determined subscales.
Next, the sample was randomly split into two equal groups for training (n = 189) and testing (n = 189) a new model via CFA. Using the training data set, a total of 8 models were tested using lavaan’s cfa() function [46]. The models incorporated varying factor correlations, residual item correlations, and deleted items (see Supplementary Table 9 in S4 Table for all tested models). We aimed to identify a model with at least an acceptable fit and having the least number of modifications. After having discovered a working model, it was cross-validated using the testing data set. Finally, the reliability was once again calculated using Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s ω.
The concurrent validity was analyzed via Pearson correlations between the PRFQ, the ETMCQ, PSS, EPDS, age of child and parents, and relationship satisfaction, via a Spearman correlation between the PRFQ and education, and lastly via a point biserial correlation between the PRFQ and changes in the working environment. Effect sizes were presumed as small for r > .10, medium for r > .30, and r > .50 [65]. An overview of the expected correlations can be seen in the online supplements. Additionally, we explored a bifactorial model. We employed a Schmid-Leiman transformation and an oblimin factor rotation, using the approach outlined in [66]. This model added a general factor to the original model. To assess model fit, the Root Mean Squared Residuals (RMSR) was employed, with the same cut-off as the standardized RMSR, < .09 for excellent model fit [58].
Results
Analysis of missing data, skewness, and kurtosis
The only missing data were the answers of 11 participants in the PFB-K, measuring relationship satisfaction. Little’s MCAR test was non-significant (χ2 = 66.0, df = 62, p = .340), indicating that missing data was completely at random and list-wise deletion was applied. For the PM subscale, skewness was calculated at 2.14 and kurtosis at 3.86. For the CMS subscale, skewness was -0.20 and kurtosis -0.87. Lastly, for the IC subscale, skewness was -1.02 and kurtosis was 0.77.
Confirmatory factor analysis of the original PRFQ model
Using the original model for a CFA, all items loaded significantly onto their respective factors, and the IC and CMS subscale correlated significantly (r = .48, p < .001). However, the model showed poor fit [61]: CFI = .80, TLI = .78, SRMR = .09, RMSEA = .07 (CI [.06, .08]), χ2 = 374.62 (p < .001). As such, the original model could not be replicated. The reliability assessment with Cronbach’s α showed (1) α = .80 for CMS, (2) α = .66 for IC, and (3) α = .54 for PM. The reliability as analyzed by McDonald’s ω returned (1) ω = .88 for CMS, (2) ω = .73 for IC, and (3) ω = .61 for PM. None of these values could be significantly improved by removing items. Further information on skew and distribution is available in the online supplement (for graphical representations see Supplementary Fig. 1–3 in S1 Fig). Given that the original model did not hold, all results beyond this point are directly based on the new model we propose for the German PRFQ.
Model adaptation for the German translation
First, a parallel analysis was run to check the number of suggested factors. Both the parallel analysis and the scree plot suggested three factors, with Eigenvalues of 3.3 for factor 1, 1.3 for factor 2, and 0.8 for factor 3. Next, an EFA with the suggested three factors was used to inspect factor loadings of individual items (see Supplementary Table 10 in S5 Table). The CMS factor explained 16% of variance with loadings ranging from .43 to .74, the IC factor 8% with loadings between .13 and .71, and the PM factor 9% with loadings ranging from .19 to .65.
The sample was randomly split in two groups. The first group was used to find a new model by testing a total of 8 CFA models (see the online supplement for an overview of the models). Ultimately, a model that provided good fit was found (see Fig 1). After removing the second item of the IC subscale, the fifth item of the CMS subscale, adding an error covariance between the second and fourth item of the PM scale, and allowing the factors to freely correlate, all fit indices indicated an acceptable to good model fit: CFI = .94, TLI = .93, SRMR = .07, RMSEA = .04 (CI [.01, .06]). The χ2 value was 127.65 (p = .032). The IC factor correlated significantly with the CMS factor (r = .49, p < .001), as well as with the PM factor (r = -.59, p = .001). The correlation between CMS and PM was non-significant (r = -.23, p = .094). To cross validate the new 16-item model, the second half of the sample was then used to run another CFA. The fit indices showed acceptable to good model fit again: CFI = .92, TLI = .90, SRMR = .07, RMSEA = .05 (CI [.03, .06]). The χ2 value was 145.05 (p = .002). The reliability values of the subscales, as measured by Cronbach’s α, pertaining to the new model, are as follows: 1. CMS: α = .79, 2. IC: α = .64, and 3. PM: α = .54. These values indicate a reliable CMS subscale. The reliability of the IC and PM subscale did not meet our reliability cutoff of α = .70. The reliability values of the subscales, as measured by McDonald’s ω, pertaining to the new model, are as follows: (1) CMS: ω = .84, (2) IC: ω = .69, and (3) PM: ω = .61. These values indicate a reliable CMS subscale. The reliability of both IC and PM fell short of our cut-off value of ω = .70.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
All items were retained regardless of factor loadings, given that we wanted to retain as much of the original content validity and theory base as possible. Only two items were removed to achieve model fit. Significant estimates are marked with*. Estimates in front of parathesis are unstandardized, estimates inside of parenthesis are standardized. Standard errors are in front of parentheses, error variances inside of parentheses. Unstandardized and standardized estimates for covariances were identical. Figure available at https://osf.io/j69wx/, under a CC-BY 4.0 license.
Alternative models
The bifactorial model using the same 16-item architecture was run with the addition of a general factor. The model yielded an RMSR of .25, indicating a misspecified model. Hence, this approach was not further pursued. Similarly, both two and four factor models yielded insufficient fit (CFI = .806, TLI = .774, SRMR = .085, RMSEA = .075 (CI [.060, .089] and CFI = .898, TLI = .879, SRMR = .069, RMSEA = .054 (CI [.039, .068]), respectively).
Concurrent validity
Summarizing the assessment of the concurrent validity, 18 out of 30 preregistered correlations were correctly anticipated. All incorrectly anticipated correlations were in the small effect range (r = -.12 to .15). Only the correlation between CMS and perceived stress stood out, with an effect size of small to medium (r = -.23), indicating that individuals reporting higher levels of stress tended to struggle more with PRF. All correlations can be seen in Table 2.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Discussion
The overall goals of the present study were (1.) to replicate the original PRFQ factor structure or (2.) to propose a new model for the German PRFQ via exploratory means, as well as (3.) to assess the reliability of the German PRFQ and (4.) to analyze its concurrent validity.
Interpretation and comparisons of the results
Model replication and alternative proposition.
The original factor model of Luyten et al. [3] could not be replicated and the CFI, TLI, and SRMR indicated that the model was misspecified [58]. Notably, given that Luyten et al. [3] had to add a number of not specified error covariances to achieve model fit, we were not able to test the exact same model. The remaining discussion will only address our final model for the German PRFQ.
Alternative proposition of a new model.
Regarding a new model for the German PRFQ, parallel analyses suggested a three-factor model. A subsequent EFA with three factors was run, following the recommendations by Hair et al. [67] and considering factor loadings of .30 to be significant. For CMS, all items loaded significantly onto the subscale, along with item 5 cross-loading onto the PM subscale. For IC, items 1–5 loaded onto IC, item 4 additionally cross-loaded onto PM and item 6 did not significantly load onto any of the subscales. A number of other translations also had issues with factor loadings with item 6 [20,27,28]. Lastly, for PM, all items but number 3 loaded on the subscale. The EFA showed all item loadings as intended, with cross-loadings being rare and if present only low in effect size. It was therefore concluded that the EFA did not warrant modifications to the factor structure.
The new model demonstrated good fit as judged using RMSEA and SRMR and acceptable fit using TLI and CFI (see 1). This model contained the same three factors as the original model but allowed free correlation. The second item of the IC scale ("I wonder a lot about what my child is thinking and feeling.") and the fifth item of the CMS scale ("I always know why I do what I do to my child.") were removed. Lastly, we had to add one error covariance between the second and fourth PM items. Ideally, the latter measure would have been avoided, but since the items came from the same subscale and correlated, this was deemed acceptable. The χ2 value was significant, indicating misspecification. However, given this metric’s tendencies to dismiss adequate models in larger sample sizes [59], coupled with the results from the other four fit indices, we assumed our model to show good to excellent fit. In our cross-validation, RMSEA and SRMR again indicated good fit and TLI and CFI indicated acceptable fit, which suggests no overfitting. This cross-validation together with our conservative approach with cut-off values further strengthens our final model and supports the original factor model [3].
Interestingly, the Finnish validation [23] also excluded item 2 from the IC subscale. A possible explanation might be its double-barreled wording ("I wonder a lot about what my child is thinking and feeling."), asking about both the cognitive component (thinking) and the affective component (feeling). Additionally, the German translation "Ich frage mich häufig…" might come across as more serious and obsessive as compared to the original English "I wonder a lot…". The fifth CMS item is the only item concerned with the mental state of the parent, not the child ("I always know why I do what I do to my child."). While theoretically still part of PRF, it is a clear difference from the other items. The Certainty About Mental States Questionnaire (CAMSQ), a self-report measure designed to only assess CMS, also differentiates between self- and other-certainty, and achieves good reliability [68]. The CAMSQ therefore gives empirical evidence that the two abilities of mentalizing about oneself and others are related, but not the same, and subsequently why the removal of this item improves fit. However, while the CAMSQ [68] only captures the dimension of certainty about mental states, which is also captured by the CMS subscale in the PRFQ, the PRFQ provides the possibility to capture two additional dimensions of PRF. Another issue could be the wording of the item "I always know why I do what I do to my child." in the German translation ("Ich weiß immer genau, warum ich das mit meinem Kind tue, was ich tue"), which changes the item from "i always know why…" to "i always know exactly why…". Thus, we advise that researchers should reassess this item after having removed the "genau" (= exactly) and then rerun the model to check if this change makes the item viable again. Looking back to the EFA results, this item was also the only one cross-loading in that particular subscale.
While the prospect of having a bifactorial model would have opened some exciting avenues for future research, the RMSR value that we obtained from our analysis could not support this modeling approach.
Reliability analysis.
Judging by Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s ω, the CMS subscale showed good reliability, while the reliabilities of the IC and PM subscale needs further assessment and improvement. These findings are still mostly on par with other validations of the PRFQ. While PM only barely made the cut-off of .70 in the original validation (α = .70), it was below the cut-off in the Italian, Korean, and Finnish validations [21,23,24], with the Italian version also struggling with the reliability of IC. In its first application [26], the 18-item version of the German PRFQ struggled with reliability of the IC (α = .47) and PM (α = .52) subscale. On the other hand, the Portuguese [22] and Canadian validations [20] showed reliable measures of all three subscales.
Considering these results as well as those of prior studies, the internal consistencies of the IC and PM subscales appear to be an issue in most languages [69]. A possible explanation for this might be found in the complexity of the construct, reflected in its measurement using complex coding of interviews and multi-factored self-report questionnaires. PRF and Mentalizing require both affective and cognitive processes [70], meaning that parts of these particular mental abilities rely on unconscious processes [16] which might not be easily captured by self-report, especially PM. Another consideration is social desirability, particularly with some very strongly worded PM items. This might partly explain the extreme skew of the PM subscale. However, this subscale has in part been developed for clinical risk samples. Given that we investigated a general, non-clinical sample, low average scores may be expected. Overall, the German PRFQ still shows acceptable performance in terms of reliability.
Concurrent Validity Analysis. The majority of our predictions about concurrent validity were in line with our results (18 out of 30 hypotheses tested). The negative correlation between CMS and perceived stress stood out. However, both the Canadian validation [20] and Luyten et al. [3] also showed that individuals reporting higher levels of stress also scored lower on PRF, however their effects remained insignificant. One possible reason for this might be the increase in stress during the COVID-19 pandemic, since high stress might impact mentalizing capacity [71,72]. Perceived stress increased by more than a standard deviation in our sample [73] compared to German mothers pre-pandemic (M = 22.29, SD = 3.43 vs. M = 13.07, SD = 6.08) [74]. Therefore, contextual effects on PRF cannot be ruled out. Another source for the deviation could be that, following the theory behind the CMS scale, it should be mapped non-linearly, since both exceedingly high and low values should be considered unfavorable (hyper- and hypomentalizing respectively). This mismatch between theory and practice might also cause artifacts in our correlation, though it would not explain why this has not been an issue for other validation studies of the PRFQ. Finally, taking all analyses into consideration, it can be assumed that the German PRFQ demonstrated adequate concurrent validity.
Limitations
The main limitations of the present study stem from its sample, mainly its high SES background. The majority of parents were in the two highest income brackets and also had the highest achievable school degree. For further validation, it would be desirable to also include lower SES backgrounds. Additionally, the data we used did not contain any information on ethnic identity. Second, the sample was highly motivated, since this was the third time they participated in the longitudinal study, meaning some self-selection has to be assumed. As a third limitation, the reliability of the presented questionnaire still poses an issue and requires further clarification.
Recommendations for further research
To increase reproducibility [75], we encourage the use of open science methods. The majority of other validations provided no preregistration or open data, which made replication significantly harder. By providing these additional materials, subsequent studies have a stronger foundation to build upon.
Another issue is the scaling of the CMS subscale. The IC and PM subscales are mostly modeled in a linear fashion. However, as already discussed, CMS might follow an inverted U-shape function because of hyper- and hypomentalizing being situated on the opposing ends. A first piece of evidence for this approach might be found in the fundamental difference in distribution between the subscales. In our study, the IC subscale skews towards high values and the PM subscale towards low values. But for CMS, the majority of parents indicated middle values. This suggests that there might be a fundamental difference in how the scaling of the three subscales works. As such, a more appropriate way for future research to analyze CMS might be an unfolding model.
Conclusion
We propose a 3-factor, 16-item model for the German adaptation of the PRFQ. We managed to show acceptable to good fit, concurrent validity, and good reliability for the CMS subscale, while the reliability of the IC and PM subscale needs further assessment and improvement. The 16-item German PRFQ has been proven a valid and promising self-report measure for future studies into attachment and psychopathology. Given the complex nature of mentalizing and PRF, we encourage further validation studies of our German version of the PRFQ by other independent research groups.
Supporting information
S1 File. Factor structure differences between PRFQ translations.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314074.s001
(DOCX)
S1 Fig. Histograms of the German PRFQ Subscales.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314074.s002
(DOCX)
S1 Table. Predicted correlations and hypotheses for concurrent validity.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314074.s003
(DOCX)
S2 Table. Item translations of all used questionnaires.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314074.s004
(DOCX)
S3 Table. Sample means and standard deviations for all questionnaires.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314074.s005
(DOCX)
S4 Table. Overview of all tested models and their respective fit indices.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314074.s006
(DOCX)
S5 Table. Factor loadings as obtained by the EFA.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314074.s007
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all volunteers who participated in the online survey and all colleagues who contributed to this study and article.
References
1. 1. Luyten P, Nijssens L, Fonagy P, Mayes LC. Parental Reflective Functioning: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. The Psychoanalytical Study of the Child 2017; 70(a):174–99.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
2. 2. Fonagy P, Gergely G, Jurist E, Target M. Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. New York: Other Press; 2002.
3. 3. Luyten P, Mayes LC, Nijssens L, Fonagy P. The parental reflective functioning questionnaire: Development and preliminary validation. PLoS ONE 2017; 12(5). pmid:28472162
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
4. 4. Rutherford HJ, Booth CR, Luyten P, Bridgett DJ, Mayes LC. Investigating the association between parental reflective functioning and distress tolerance in motherhood. Infant Behavior and Development 2015; 40:54–63. pmid:26025253
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
5. 5. Rutherford HJ, Goldberg B, Luyten P, Bridgett DJ, Mayes L.C. Parental reflective functioning is associated with tolerance of infant distress but not general distress: evidence for a specific relationship using a simulated baby paradigm. Infant Behavior and Development 2013; 36(4):635–41. pmid:23906942
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
6. 6. Burkhart ML, Borelli JL, Rasmussen HF, Brody R, Sbarra DA. Parental mentalizing as an indirect link between attachment anxiety and parenting satisfaction. Journal of Family Psychology 2017; 31(2):203–13. pmid:28054798
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
7. 7. Nijssens L, Luyten P, Malcorps S, Vliegen N, Mayes L. Parental reflective functioning and theory of mind acquisition: A developmental perspective. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 2021; 39(2):584–602. pmid:34056741
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
8. 8. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Allison E. Epistemic petrification and the restoration of epistemic trust: a new conceptualization of borderline personality disorder and its psychosocial treatment. Journal of Personality Disorders 2015; 29(5):575–609. pmid:26393477
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
9. 9. Fonagy P, Allison E. The role of mentalizing and epistemic trust in the therapeutic relationship. Psychotherapy 2014; 51(3):372–80. pmid:24773092
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
10. 10. Meins E, Fernyhough C, Fradley E, Tuckey M. Rethinking maternal sensitivity: Mothers’ comments on infants’ mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 2001; 42(5):637–48. pmid:11464968
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
11. 11. Slade A, Grienenberger J, Bernbach E, Levy D, Locker A. Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: A preliminary study. Attachment & Human Development 2005; 7(3):283–98. pmid:16210240
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
12. 12. Malcorps S, Vliegen N, Fonagy P, Luyten P. A four-year multi-wave prospective study on the role of parental reflective functioning and parenting stress in the development of socio-emotional problems in internationally adopted children. Development and Psychopathology 2024; 36(1):266–79. pmid:36382423
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
13. 13. Kristen K, Arietta S, Grienenberger JF. Maternal reflective functioning, mother–infant affective communication, and infant attachment: Exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behavior in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attachment & Human Development 2005; 7(3):299–311. pmid:16210241
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
14. 14. Salo SJ, Pajulo M, Vinzce L, Raittila S, Sourander J, Kalland M. Parent Relationship Satisfaction and Reflective Functioning as Predictors of Emotional Availability and Infant Behavior. Journal of Child and Family Studies 2021; 30:1214–28.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
15. 15. Sharp C, Fonagy P. The parent’s capacity to treat the child as a psychological agent: constructs, measures and implications for developmental psychopathology. Social Development 2008; 17(3):737–54.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
16. 16. Duschinsky R, Foster S. Mentalizing and epistemic trust: The work of Peter Fonagy and colleagues at the Anna Freud Centre. New York: Oxford University Press; 2021.
17. 17. Krink S, Muehlhan C, Luyten P, Romer G, Ramsauer B. Parental reflective functioning affects sensitivity to distress in mothers with postpartum depression. Journal of Child and Family Studies 2018; 27(5):1671–81.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
18. 18. Ainsworth MDS. Maternal sensitivity scales. Power 1969; 6:1379–88.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
19. 19. Fonagy P, Target M, Steele H, Steele M. Reflective-functioning manual version 5.0 for application to adult attachment interviews. London: University College London; 1998.
20. 20. Roo M de, Wong G, Rempel GR, Fraser SN. Advancing Optimal Development in Children: Examining the Construct Validity of a Parent Reflective Functioning Questionnaire. JMIR Pediatrics and Parenting 2019; 2(1). pmid:31518301
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
21. 21. Lee Y, Meins E, Larkin F. Translation and preliminary validation of a Korean version of the parental reflective functioning questionnaire. Infant mental health journal 2021; 42:47–59. pmid:32744756
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
22. 22. Moreira H, Fonseca A. Measuring Parental Reflective Functioning: Further Validation of the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire in Portuguese Mothers of Infants and Young Children. Child Psychiatry & Human Development 2022:1–13. pmid:35064394
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
23. 23. Pajulo M, Tolvanen M, Pyykkönen N, Karlsson L, Mayes L, Karlsson H. Exploring parental mentalization in postnatal phase with a self-report questionnaire (PRFQ): Factor structure, gender differences and association with sociodemographic factors. The Finn Brain Birth Cohort Study. Psychiatry Research 2018; 262:431–9. pmid:28939390
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
24. 24. Pazzagli C, Delvecchio E, Raspa V, Mazzeschi C, Luyten P. The Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire in Mothers and Fathers of School-Aged Children. Journal of Child and Family Studies 2017; 27:80–90.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
25. 25. Szabó B, Miklósi M, Boda M, Futó J. The adaptation of the parental reflective functioning questionnaire adolescent version to the Hungarian language and presentation of its psychometric characteristics. European Psychiatry 2022; 65(S1):207–8.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
26. 26. Ye P, Ju J, Zheng K, Dang J, Bian Y. Psychometric Evaluation of the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire in Chinese Parents. Frontiers in Psychology 2022; 13. pmid:35153949
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
27. 27. Szabó B, Futó J, Luyten P, Boda M, Miklósi M. The psychometric properties of the Hungarian Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire. European journal of developmental psychology 2024; 21(1):106–23.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
28. 28. Bahramkhani M, Mohammad Hoseini S, Sarichloo ME. Factor Structure of Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire for Mothers Working at Qazvin University of Medical Sciences. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci 2024; 18(1).
* View Article
* Google Scholar
29. 29. Georg AK, Schröder-Pfeifer P, Cierpka M, Taubner S. Maternal parenting stress in the face of early regulatory disorders in infancy: a machine learning approach to identify what matters most. Frontiers in psychiatry 2021; 12. pmid:34408674
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
30. 30. Mattheß J, Eckert M, Richter K, Koch G, Reinhold T, Vienhues P et al. Efficacy of Parent-Infant-Psychotherapy with mothers with postpartum mental disorder: study protocol of the randomized controlled trial as part of the SKKIPPI project. Trials 2020; 21(1):1–12. pmid:32503611
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
31. 31. Griffee DT. Questionnaire Translation and Questionnaire Validation: Are They the Same? In: St. Louis, MO: Annual Meeting of the American Association of Applied Linguistics; 2001.
32. 32. Campbell C, Tanzer M, Saunders R, Booker T, Allison E, Li E et al. Development and validation of a self-report measure of epistemic trust. PLoS ONE 2021; 16(4). pmid:33861805
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
33. 33. Zietlow A-L, Woll CFJ, Nonnenmacher N, Müller M, Labonte V, Ditzen B et al. Study protocol of the COMPARE-Interaction study: the impact of maternal comorbid depression and anxiety disorders in the peripartum period on child development. BMJ Open 2022; 12(1):e050437. pmid:35058257
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
34. 34. Wildner AS, Küçükakyüz SM, Woll CFJ. PRFQ (GER) Validation; 2023. URL: https://osf.io/j69wx/.
35. 35. Everitt BS. Multivariate analysis: The need for data, and other problems. The Britisch Journal of Psychiatry 1975; 126(3):237–240. pmid:1125504
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
36. 36. Harpe SE. How to analyze Likert and other rating scale data. Currents in Pharmacy Teaching and Learning 2015; 7(6):836–50.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
37. 37. Baggaley AR, Hull AL. The effect of nonlinear transformations on a Likert scale. Eval Health Prof 1983; 6(4):483–91. pmid:10264243
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
38. 38. Barendse MT, Oort FJ, Timmerman ME. Using Exploratory Factor Analysis to Determine the Dimensionality of Discrete Responses. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 2015; 22(1):87–101.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
39. 39. DiStefano C. The Impact of Categorization With Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 2002; 9(3):327–46.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
40. 40. Jorgensen TD, Johnson AR. How to Derive Expected Values of Structural Equation Model Parameters when Treating Discrete Data as Continuous. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 2022; 29(4):639–50.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
41. 41. Reis D, Lehr D, Heber E, Ebert DD. The German Version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10): Evaluation of Dimensionality, Validity, and Measurement Invariance With Exploratory and Confirmatory Bifactor Modeling. Assessment 2019; 26(7):1246–59. pmid:28627220
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
42. 42. Bergant A, Nguyen T, Heim K, Ulmer H, Dapunt O. Deutschsprachige Fassung und Validierung der »Edinburgh postnatal depression scale«. DMW-Deutsche Medizinische Wochenschrift 1998; 123(3):35–40. pmid:9472218
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
43. 43. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science 1987; 150:782–6. pmid:3651732
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
44. 44. Haldweg K. Fragebogen zur Partnerschaftsdiagnostik (FPD). Handanweisung. Hogrefe Verlag für Psychologie; 1996.
45. 45. Meuwly N, Schoebi D, Bierhoff HW. TBSTK Rezension: Fragebogen zur Partnerschaftsdiagnostik (FPD; 2., neu nominierte und erweiterte Auflage). Psychologische Rundschau 2018; 69(4):375–93.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
46. 46. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria; 2021. URL: http://www.R-project.org/.
47. 47. Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. Journal of Statistical Software 2012; 48(2):1–36.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
48. 48. Wickham H, Averick M, Bryan J, Chang W, McGowan LD, François R et al. Welcome to the tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software 2019; 4(43):1686.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
49. 49. Becker JP, Knowles JE. Convenience Functions for Education Data; 2020. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/eeptools/eeptools.pdf.
50. 50. Grolemund G, Wickham H. Dates and Times Made Easy with lubridate. Journal of Statistical Software 2011; 40(3):1–25.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
51. 51. Revelle R. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. Evanston, Illinois; 2022. URL: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.
52. 52. Tierney N, Cook D, McBain M, Fay C, O´Hara-Wild M, Hester J et al. Data Structures, Summaries, and Visualisations for Missing Data; 2021. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/naniar/naniar.pdf.
53. 53. Zhang Z, Yuan KH. Robust Coefficient Alpha and Omega with Missing and Non-Normal Data; 2020. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/coefficientalpha/coefficientalpha.pdf.
54. 54. Harrell FE. Harrell Miscellaneous; 2022. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/Hmisc/Hmisc.pdf.
55. 55. Komsta L. Moments, Cumulants, Skewness, Kurtosis and Related Tests; 2022. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/moments/moments.pdf.
56. 56. Meyer D, Dimitriadou E, Hornik K, Weingessel A, Leisch F, Chang CC et al. Misc Functions of the Department of Statistics, Probability Theory Group (Formerly: E1071), TU Wien; 2022. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/e1071/e1071.pdf.
57. 57. Jackson DL, Gillaspy JA, Purc-Stephenson R. Reporting practices in confirmatory factor analysis: An overview and some recommendations. Psychological Methods 2009; 14(1):6–23. pmid:19271845
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
58. 58. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 1999; 6(1):1–55.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
59. 59. Kyriazos TA. Applied Psychometrics: Sample Size and Sample Power Considerations in Factor Analysis (EFA, CFA) and SEM in General. Psychology 2018; 9:2207–30.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
60. 60. McDonald RP. Test Theory: A Unified Treatment. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1999.
61. 61. Cronbach LJ. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 1951; 16(3):297–334.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
62. 62. Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistics notes: Cronbach’s alpha. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.) 1997; 314(7080):572. pmid:9055718
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
63. 63. Hoyle RH, editor. Handbook of Structural Equation Modeling. New York: Guilford Press; 2014.
64. 64. Zwick WR, Velicer WF. Factors influencing five rules for determining the number of components to retain. Psychological Bulletin 1986; 99:432–42. pmid:26810950
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
65. 65. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychological Bulletin 1992; 112(1):155–9. pmid:19565683
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
66. 66. Waller NG. Direct Schmid-Leiman Transformations and Rank-Deficient Loadings Matrices. Psychometrika 2018; 83(4):858–70. pmid:29204802
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
67. 67. Hair JF, Tatham RL, Anderson RE, Black W. Multivariate data analysis. 5th ed. London: Prentice-Hall; 1998.
68. 68. Müller S, Wendt LP, Zimmermann J. Development and Validation of the Certainty About Mental States Questionnaire (CAMSQ): A Self-Report Measure of Mentalizing Oneself and Others. Assessment 2023; 30(3):651–74. pmid:34905983
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
69. 69. Carlone C, Milan S, Decoste C, Borelli JL, McMahon TJ, Suchman NE. Self-report measure of parental reflective functioning: A study of reliability and validity across three samples of varying clinical risk. Infant mental health journal 2023; 00:1–15. pmid:36857469
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
70. 70. Slade A. Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attachment & Human Development 2005; 7(3):269–81. pmid:16210239
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
71. 71. Bo S, Sharp C, Fonagy P, Kongerslev M. Hypermentalizing, attachment, and epistemic trust in adolescent BPD: Clinical illustrations. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 2017; 8(2):172. pmid:26691672
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
72. 72. Schulreich S, Tusche A, Kanske P, Schwabe L. Altruism under Stress: Cortisol Negatively Predicts Charitable Giving and Neural Value Representations Depending on Mentalizing Capacity. Journal of Neuroscience 2022; 42(16):3445–60. pmid:35288436
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
73. 73. Woll CFJ. Mother-infant relationship, dyadic interaction, and child development: the role of maternal mental health. Dissertation, LMU München: Fakultät für Psychologie und Pädagogik; 2023.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
74. 74. Klein EM, Brähler E, Dreier M, Reinecke L, Müller KW, Schmutzer G et al. The German version of the Perceived Stress Scale–psychometric characteristics in a representative German community sample. BMC Psychiatry 2016; 16. pmid:27216151
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
75. 75. Maxwell SE, Michael YL, George SH. Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? American Psychologist 2015; 70(6):487–98. pmid:26348332
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
Citation: Wildner AS, Küçükakyüz SM, Marx AKG, Nolte T, Reck C, Fonagy P, et al. (2024) Parental considerations about their childs’ mental health: Validating the German adaptation of the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire. PLoS ONE 19(12): e0314074. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0314074
About the Authors:
Andreas S. Wildner
Roles: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft
E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Clinical Psychology of Childhood and Adolescence & Counseling Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0009-0006-8252-3367
Su Mevsim Küçükakyüz
Roles: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft
Affiliation: Clinical Psychology of Childhood and Adolescence & Counseling Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany
Anton K. G. Marx
Roles: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Clinical Psychology of Childhood and Adolescence & Counseling Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9611-9759
Tobias Nolte
Roles: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, Clinical, Education & Health Psychology, Psychoanalysis Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom
Corinna Reck
Roles: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Clinical Psychology of Childhood and Adolescence & Counseling Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany
Peter Fonagy
Roles: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, Clinical, Education & Health Psychology, Psychoanalysis Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom
Patrick Luyten
Roles: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, Clinical, Education & Health Psychology, Psychoanalysis Unit, University College London, London, United Kingdom
Alexandra von Tettenborn
Roles: Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Clinical Psychology of Childhood and Adolescence & Counseling Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany
Mitho Müller
Roles: Methodology, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Clinical Psychology of Childhood and Adolescence & Counseling Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8431-540X
Anna-Lena Zietlow
Roles: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Institute of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Clinical Child and Adolescence Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Christian F. J. Woll-Weber
Roles: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft
Affiliations: Clinical Psychology of Childhood and Adolescence & Counseling Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany, Clinical Child and Adolescence Psychology and Psychotherapy, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0352-4796
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
1. Luyten P, Nijssens L, Fonagy P, Mayes LC. Parental Reflective Functioning: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. The Psychoanalytical Study of the Child 2017; 70(a):174–99.
2. Fonagy P, Gergely G, Jurist E, Target M. Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. New York: Other Press; 2002.
3. Luyten P, Mayes LC, Nijssens L, Fonagy P. The parental reflective functioning questionnaire: Development and preliminary validation. PLoS ONE 2017; 12(5). pmid:28472162
4. Rutherford HJ, Booth CR, Luyten P, Bridgett DJ, Mayes LC. Investigating the association between parental reflective functioning and distress tolerance in motherhood. Infant Behavior and Development 2015; 40:54–63. pmid:26025253
5. Rutherford HJ, Goldberg B, Luyten P, Bridgett DJ, Mayes L.C. Parental reflective functioning is associated with tolerance of infant distress but not general distress: evidence for a specific relationship using a simulated baby paradigm. Infant Behavior and Development 2013; 36(4):635–41. pmid:23906942
6. Burkhart ML, Borelli JL, Rasmussen HF, Brody R, Sbarra DA. Parental mentalizing as an indirect link between attachment anxiety and parenting satisfaction. Journal of Family Psychology 2017; 31(2):203–13. pmid:28054798
7. Nijssens L, Luyten P, Malcorps S, Vliegen N, Mayes L. Parental reflective functioning and theory of mind acquisition: A developmental perspective. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 2021; 39(2):584–602. pmid:34056741
8. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Allison E. Epistemic petrification and the restoration of epistemic trust: a new conceptualization of borderline personality disorder and its psychosocial treatment. Journal of Personality Disorders 2015; 29(5):575–609. pmid:26393477
9. Fonagy P, Allison E. The role of mentalizing and epistemic trust in the therapeutic relationship. Psychotherapy 2014; 51(3):372–80. pmid:24773092
10. Meins E, Fernyhough C, Fradley E, Tuckey M. Rethinking maternal sensitivity: Mothers’ comments on infants’ mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 2001; 42(5):637–48. pmid:11464968
11. Slade A, Grienenberger J, Bernbach E, Levy D, Locker A. Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: A preliminary study. Attachment & Human Development 2005; 7(3):283–98. pmid:16210240
12. Malcorps S, Vliegen N, Fonagy P, Luyten P. A four-year multi-wave prospective study on the role of parental reflective functioning and parenting stress in the development of socio-emotional problems in internationally adopted children. Development and Psychopathology 2024; 36(1):266–79. pmid:36382423
13. Kristen K, Arietta S, Grienenberger JF. Maternal reflective functioning, mother–infant affective communication, and infant attachment: Exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behavior in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attachment & Human Development 2005; 7(3):299–311. pmid:16210241
14. Salo SJ, Pajulo M, Vinzce L, Raittila S, Sourander J, Kalland M. Parent Relationship Satisfaction and Reflective Functioning as Predictors of Emotional Availability and Infant Behavior. Journal of Child and Family Studies 2021; 30:1214–28.
15. Sharp C, Fonagy P. The parent’s capacity to treat the child as a psychological agent: constructs, measures and implications for developmental psychopathology. Social Development 2008; 17(3):737–54.
16. Duschinsky R, Foster S. Mentalizing and epistemic trust: The work of Peter Fonagy and colleagues at the Anna Freud Centre. New York: Oxford University Press; 2021.
17. Krink S, Muehlhan C, Luyten P, Romer G, Ramsauer B. Parental reflective functioning affects sensitivity to distress in mothers with postpartum depression. Journal of Child and Family Studies 2018; 27(5):1671–81.
18. Ainsworth MDS. Maternal sensitivity scales. Power 1969; 6:1379–88.
19. Fonagy P, Target M, Steele H, Steele M. Reflective-functioning manual version 5.0 for application to adult attachment interviews. London: University College London; 1998.
20. Roo M de, Wong G, Rempel GR, Fraser SN. Advancing Optimal Development in Children: Examining the Construct Validity of a Parent Reflective Functioning Questionnaire. JMIR Pediatrics and Parenting 2019; 2(1). pmid:31518301
21. Lee Y, Meins E, Larkin F. Translation and preliminary validation of a Korean version of the parental reflective functioning questionnaire. Infant mental health journal 2021; 42:47–59. pmid:32744756
22. Moreira H, Fonseca A. Measuring Parental Reflective Functioning: Further Validation of the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire in Portuguese Mothers of Infants and Young Children. Child Psychiatry & Human Development 2022:1–13. pmid:35064394
23. Pajulo M, Tolvanen M, Pyykkönen N, Karlsson L, Mayes L, Karlsson H. Exploring parental mentalization in postnatal phase with a self-report questionnaire (PRFQ): Factor structure, gender differences and association with sociodemographic factors. The Finn Brain Birth Cohort Study. Psychiatry Research 2018; 262:431–9. pmid:28939390
24. Pazzagli C, Delvecchio E, Raspa V, Mazzeschi C, Luyten P. The Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire in Mothers and Fathers of School-Aged Children. Journal of Child and Family Studies 2017; 27:80–90.
25. Szabó B, Miklósi M, Boda M, Futó J. The adaptation of the parental reflective functioning questionnaire adolescent version to the Hungarian language and presentation of its psychometric characteristics. European Psychiatry 2022; 65(S1):207–8.
26. Ye P, Ju J, Zheng K, Dang J, Bian Y. Psychometric Evaluation of the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire in Chinese Parents. Frontiers in Psychology 2022; 13. pmid:35153949
27. Szabó B, Futó J, Luyten P, Boda M, Miklósi M. The psychometric properties of the Hungarian Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire. European journal of developmental psychology 2024; 21(1):106–23.
28. Bahramkhani M, Mohammad Hoseini S, Sarichloo ME. Factor Structure of Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire for Mothers Working at Qazvin University of Medical Sciences. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci 2024; 18(1).
29. Georg AK, Schröder-Pfeifer P, Cierpka M, Taubner S. Maternal parenting stress in the face of early regulatory disorders in infancy: a machine learning approach to identify what matters most. Frontiers in psychiatry 2021; 12. pmid:34408674
30. Mattheß J, Eckert M, Richter K, Koch G, Reinhold T, Vienhues P et al. Efficacy of Parent-Infant-Psychotherapy with mothers with postpartum mental disorder: study protocol of the randomized controlled trial as part of the SKKIPPI project. Trials 2020; 21(1):1–12. pmid:32503611
31. Griffee DT. Questionnaire Translation and Questionnaire Validation: Are They the Same? In: St. Louis, MO: Annual Meeting of the American Association of Applied Linguistics; 2001.
32. Campbell C, Tanzer M, Saunders R, Booker T, Allison E, Li E et al. Development and validation of a self-report measure of epistemic trust. PLoS ONE 2021; 16(4). pmid:33861805
33. Zietlow A-L, Woll CFJ, Nonnenmacher N, Müller M, Labonte V, Ditzen B et al. Study protocol of the COMPARE-Interaction study: the impact of maternal comorbid depression and anxiety disorders in the peripartum period on child development. BMJ Open 2022; 12(1):e050437. pmid:35058257
34. Wildner AS, Küçükakyüz SM, Woll CFJ. PRFQ (GER) Validation; 2023. URL: https://osf.io/j69wx/.
35. Everitt BS. Multivariate analysis: The need for data, and other problems. The Britisch Journal of Psychiatry 1975; 126(3):237–240. pmid:1125504
36. Harpe SE. How to analyze Likert and other rating scale data. Currents in Pharmacy Teaching and Learning 2015; 7(6):836–50.
37. Baggaley AR, Hull AL. The effect of nonlinear transformations on a Likert scale. Eval Health Prof 1983; 6(4):483–91. pmid:10264243
38. Barendse MT, Oort FJ, Timmerman ME. Using Exploratory Factor Analysis to Determine the Dimensionality of Discrete Responses. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 2015; 22(1):87–101.
39. DiStefano C. The Impact of Categorization With Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 2002; 9(3):327–46.
40. Jorgensen TD, Johnson AR. How to Derive Expected Values of Structural Equation Model Parameters when Treating Discrete Data as Continuous. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 2022; 29(4):639–50.
41. Reis D, Lehr D, Heber E, Ebert DD. The German Version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10): Evaluation of Dimensionality, Validity, and Measurement Invariance With Exploratory and Confirmatory Bifactor Modeling. Assessment 2019; 26(7):1246–59. pmid:28627220
42. Bergant A, Nguyen T, Heim K, Ulmer H, Dapunt O. Deutschsprachige Fassung und Validierung der »Edinburgh postnatal depression scale«. DMW-Deutsche Medizinische Wochenschrift 1998; 123(3):35–40. pmid:9472218
43. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science 1987; 150:782–6. pmid:3651732
44. Haldweg K. Fragebogen zur Partnerschaftsdiagnostik (FPD). Handanweisung. Hogrefe Verlag für Psychologie; 1996.
45. Meuwly N, Schoebi D, Bierhoff HW. TBSTK Rezension: Fragebogen zur Partnerschaftsdiagnostik (FPD; 2., neu nominierte und erweiterte Auflage). Psychologische Rundschau 2018; 69(4):375–93.
46. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria; 2021. URL: http://www.R-project.org/.
47. Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. Journal of Statistical Software 2012; 48(2):1–36.
48. Wickham H, Averick M, Bryan J, Chang W, McGowan LD, François R et al. Welcome to the tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software 2019; 4(43):1686.
49. Becker JP, Knowles JE. Convenience Functions for Education Data; 2020. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/eeptools/eeptools.pdf.
50. Grolemund G, Wickham H. Dates and Times Made Easy with lubridate. Journal of Statistical Software 2011; 40(3):1–25.
51. Revelle R. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. Evanston, Illinois; 2022. URL: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.
52. Tierney N, Cook D, McBain M, Fay C, O´Hara-Wild M, Hester J et al. Data Structures, Summaries, and Visualisations for Missing Data; 2021. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/naniar/naniar.pdf.
53. Zhang Z, Yuan KH. Robust Coefficient Alpha and Omega with Missing and Non-Normal Data; 2020. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/coefficientalpha/coefficientalpha.pdf.
54. Harrell FE. Harrell Miscellaneous; 2022. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/Hmisc/Hmisc.pdf.
55. Komsta L. Moments, Cumulants, Skewness, Kurtosis and Related Tests; 2022. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/moments/moments.pdf.
56. Meyer D, Dimitriadou E, Hornik K, Weingessel A, Leisch F, Chang CC et al. Misc Functions of the Department of Statistics, Probability Theory Group (Formerly: E1071), TU Wien; 2022. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/e1071/e1071.pdf.
57. Jackson DL, Gillaspy JA, Purc-Stephenson R. Reporting practices in confirmatory factor analysis: An overview and some recommendations. Psychological Methods 2009; 14(1):6–23. pmid:19271845
58. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 1999; 6(1):1–55.
59. Kyriazos TA. Applied Psychometrics: Sample Size and Sample Power Considerations in Factor Analysis (EFA, CFA) and SEM in General. Psychology 2018; 9:2207–30.
60. McDonald RP. Test Theory: A Unified Treatment. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1999.
61. Cronbach LJ. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 1951; 16(3):297–334.
62. Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistics notes: Cronbach’s alpha. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.) 1997; 314(7080):572. pmid:9055718
63. Hoyle RH, editor. Handbook of Structural Equation Modeling. New York: Guilford Press; 2014.
64. Zwick WR, Velicer WF. Factors influencing five rules for determining the number of components to retain. Psychological Bulletin 1986; 99:432–42. pmid:26810950
65. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychological Bulletin 1992; 112(1):155–9. pmid:19565683
66. Waller NG. Direct Schmid-Leiman Transformations and Rank-Deficient Loadings Matrices. Psychometrika 2018; 83(4):858–70. pmid:29204802
67. Hair JF, Tatham RL, Anderson RE, Black W. Multivariate data analysis. 5th ed. London: Prentice-Hall; 1998.
68. Müller S, Wendt LP, Zimmermann J. Development and Validation of the Certainty About Mental States Questionnaire (CAMSQ): A Self-Report Measure of Mentalizing Oneself and Others. Assessment 2023; 30(3):651–74. pmid:34905983
69. Carlone C, Milan S, Decoste C, Borelli JL, McMahon TJ, Suchman NE. Self-report measure of parental reflective functioning: A study of reliability and validity across three samples of varying clinical risk. Infant mental health journal 2023; 00:1–15. pmid:36857469
70. Slade A. Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attachment & Human Development 2005; 7(3):269–81. pmid:16210239
71. Bo S, Sharp C, Fonagy P, Kongerslev M. Hypermentalizing, attachment, and epistemic trust in adolescent BPD: Clinical illustrations. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 2017; 8(2):172. pmid:26691672
72. Schulreich S, Tusche A, Kanske P, Schwabe L. Altruism under Stress: Cortisol Negatively Predicts Charitable Giving and Neural Value Representations Depending on Mentalizing Capacity. Journal of Neuroscience 2022; 42(16):3445–60. pmid:35288436
73. Woll CFJ. Mother-infant relationship, dyadic interaction, and child development: the role of maternal mental health. Dissertation, LMU München: Fakultät für Psychologie und Pädagogik; 2023.
74. Klein EM, Brähler E, Dreier M, Reinecke L, Müller KW, Schmutzer G et al. The German version of the Perceived Stress Scale–psychometric characteristics in a representative German community sample. BMC Psychiatry 2016; 16. pmid:27216151
75. Maxwell SE, Michael YL, George SH. Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? American Psychologist 2015; 70(6):487–98. pmid:26348332
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2024 Wildner et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Introduction
Parental Reflective Functioning describes the parents’ ability to view their child as motivated by mental states. The Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ) represents an 18-item and three-factor self-report measure. Our goal was to conduct the first German validation study.
Method
In a community sample of 378 mothers of children aged 10.2–78.6 months, we used Confirmatory Factor Analysis with a cross-validation approach to assess model fit. Reliability was measured using Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s ω. Concurrent validity was assessed using correlations with relevant constructs.
Results
The three-factor structure of the original validation could be confirmed. The German model only needed minor modifications: two items had to be removed, and one error covariance was added. The resulting 16-item questionnaire with the three subscales “Pre-mentalizing”, “Interest and Curiosity about Mental States”, and “Certainty about Mental States” was successfully cross-validated (CFI = .94, TLI = .93, SRMR = .07, RMSEA = .04 (CI [.01, .06])). These factors were related in theoretically expected ways to parental attachment dimensions, emotional availability, parenting stress, and infant attachment status.
Conclusion
While reliability could still be improved, the German 16-item version of the PRFQ represents a valid measure of parental reflective functioning.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer