Introduction
Sexuality is an important area of development in youth. As a part of normal sexual development sexual experiences become more common and intimate (1) as dating progresses from contacts with a romantic flavour in mixed gender group socializing to mutually satisfactory dyadic romantic relationships (2). This accumulation of sexual experiences is considered a part of the formation of sexual identity. Among the vast majority of youth sexual encounters happen within romantic relationships, while casual sexual contacts outside of a steady relationship are not common (3).
Sexual orientation indicates the gender of one’s erotic interest. The term sexual minority refers to individuals who are sexually interested in their own or in the opposite and their own gender, individuals whose partners are the same or the same and opposite gender or individuals who identify as lesbian, gay or bisexual (LGB) (4).
Gender identity refers to how individuals experience their gender (5). It describes who and what individuals feel they are. In the vast majority of people sex chromosomes, primary and secondary gender characteristics and gender identity are uniform (cisgender identity). However, the perceived gender may differ from the biological sex. The term transgender is used as an umbrella term, subsuming a myriad of gender identities differing in one form or other from biological sex (5). Gender dysphoria is a state in which a person feels constantly and strongly that they are of other than their biological sex, their biological sex gives them a persistent feeling of malaise and they want to rid themselves of primary and secondary gender characteristics (6). Identifying as something other than one’s biological sex does not automatically cause gender dysphoria.
Sexual experiences, sexual maturation, sexual orientation and gender identity are dimensions of their own, but they relate to each other in many ways.
Many different psychological and social factors can be presumed to slow the sexual development of trans youth (transgender identifying adolescents).
During puberty, increased hormone secretion triggers a series of physical changes, especially in the primary and secondary sex characteristics. Among trans youth and those who suffer from gender dysphoria these changes can lead to mental discomfort and concern over body image (7, 8), such as aversion towards genitals (9), and this may delay their willingness to engage in romantic and erotic encounters. On the other hand, sexual orientation and gender identity minorities may have fewer chances to find suitable partners than their heterosexual and cisgender peers simply due to the nature of belonging to a minority (10). These factors may be detrimental to sexual self-confidence and could inhibit the accumulation of romantic and erotic experiences. Additionally, gender dysphoric and transgender identifying adolescents often have concurrent mental disorders (11-13). Mental disorders in turn may result in lowered self-esteem and social isolation, that may further delay romantic and erotic experiences. On the other hand, mental disorders during adolescence are also related to early and risk-taking sexual behaviour (14-16). This may arise from inability to protect oneself, or from seeking comfort from sexually intimate encounters. Therefore, transgender identifying adolescents could, due to excessive mental disorders and developmental challenges, be especially prone to earlier and riskier sexual behaviour than is appropriate for the developmental phase.
Research on the sexuality of trans youth is scarce and the results are mixed as studies have found trans youth have similar, less or more sexual experiences than their cisgender peers. For example, a United States study on unselected population found that LGBTQ youth (lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer or questioning) had more experiences of dating and romantic relationships than their mainstream peers (10). However, this study focused on LGBTQ adolescents rather than specifically on transgender adolescents. Contrary to this, studies in the Netherlands and in Finland found that adolescents referred to evaluation due to gender dysphoric characteristics had fewer sexual experiences than their peers in the general population (17, 18) whereas a Canadian study based on activist material found no difference in the numbers of sexual experiences (19).
Not only the number but also the nature of sexual experiences of trans youth may differ from that in the general population. For example, a study found that 50% of the trans youth in their sample engaged in sex with a partner without involving their own genitals (20). This could indicate that trans youth may feel discomfort related to their genitals. On the other hand, the trans youth still engaged in sex with a partner, even if genitals were not involved, indicating sexual activity is not totally ceased even if one might struggle with questions related to transgender identity and one’s body. This could indicate that transgender identity need not disturb reaching adolescent developmental milestones.
Trans youth appear to have more negative sexual experiences such as sexual harassment and dating violence than their mainstream peers (21-23). This has been explained by heterosexism, a cultural tendency to display aggression towards gender and sexual minorities, which serves to maintain traditional male and female roles and the superiority of heterosexuality over other sexualities (24-26). On the other hand, sexual and gender minority youth may also perpetrate sexual harassment more often than their peers in the general population (21). The research so far has not focused on the perpetration of sexual harassment specifically among transgender youth and has proposed no explanation for this phenomenon.
Young transgender identifying individuals, especially late adolescent/young adult trans women (male-to-female) engage in sex work in disproportionally large numbers (27, 28). The research suggests this is related to multiple factors, such as drug use and lack of perceived social support (29) which, in turn, are related to the marginalization and rejection often experienced by transgender identifying individuals (30). This may suggest that transgender youth are particularly vulnerable to subjection to interactions resulting in sexual risk taking such as commercialized sex, and at increased risk for negative sexual experiences such as sexual coercion and violence related to sexual encounters.
To summarize, the empirical research on transgender identity and sexual development in adolescence is scarce. Currently available research on the topic may further fail to distinguish between the sexual development of gender and sexual minority youth, may be based on selected samples, or focus solely on clinically referred subjects. Research has suggested a proneness to both delayed and early advancing and risk-taking sexual behaviours among trans youth. Thus, in light of the currently available research it is hard to understand the sexuality of trans youth in the community, and how this compares to that of same aged general population. As gender identity and sexual development are closely related and also major aspects of adolescent development, we feel further studies on the subject are warranted. The aim of this study is to explore the sexual experiences of trans identifying youth regardless of their sexual orientation. In more detail we ask
1)Do normative sexual experiences differ between transgender and cisgender youth?
2)Do transgender and cisgender youth differ regarding negative sexual experiences (dating violence and sexual harassment)?
Sexual development is an important and normative part of adolescence. By researching the sexual experiences of trans identifying adolescents in the population and comparing these to their cisgender peers, we can shed light on the connection between trans identification and sexual development in adolescence. Possible differences in negative sexual experiences between transgender and cisgender youth may have implications for understanding identity development in adolescence but may also reveal discriminating cultural phenomena that warrant attention.
Methods
Population
The Adolescent Mental Health Cohort and Replication (AMHCR) study is a mental health survey among pupils in their ninth year of comprehensive school. The person-identifiable survey was conducted in the academic years 2002-03, 2012-13 and 2018-19 in the city of Tampere, Finland. The present study is based on the cross-sectional survey of 2018-19. Of the secondary schools (grades 7-9) run by the city of Tampere, 17 agreed to participate, while two schools could not participate due to logistic reasons. The target group of the AMHCR were the 9th graders in the participating schools. Parents were informed in advance about the forthcoming study by a message distributed through the digital application used in Finnish schools for communication between school and family. The adolescents responded to the online survey after being informed in writing and orally about the nature of the study and voluntariness of participation. They logged in to the survey using personal codes during a school lesson supervised by a teacher, who provided information on the study but did not interfere with responding. After reading the written information the adolescents were asked to indicate their consent online. The study was duly approved by the ethics committee of Tampere University Hospital and given appropriate administrative permission by the appropriate authorities of the City of Tampere. In total 1,425 adolescents logged in to the survey. Of these, 39 (2.7%) declined to respond, leaving 1,386 participants, of whom 676 (47.4%) reported that their sex (as indicated in identity documents) was female and 710 (49.8%) male. The mean (SD) age of the participants was 15.59 (0.41) years, of whom 82.4% were living with both parents. Of these 5.5% reported that their father and 4.4% that their mother had only basic education, while 24.3% reported that at least one of their parents had been unemployed or laid off during the past 12 months.
Sex and gender identity
At the beginning of the survey, the respondents reported their sex as indicated in their identity documents, with response alternatives “boy” and “girl”. It was explicitly mentioned that this question referred to sex as indicated in official identity documents. According to reported sex, the respondents are referred to here as boys and girls, or males and females.
Later, in the section of the survey addressing health, respondents were asked about their perceived gender as follows: “Do you perceive yourself to be…”, with response options “a boy/a girl/both/none/my perception varies” (31). According to sex and perceived gender, the respondents were categorized to one of three gender identities: cisgender identity (indicated male sex and perceives himself as a boy, or female sex and perceives herself as a girl), opposite sex identification (male sex, perceived to be a girl; or female sex, perceived to be a boy), and other/non-binary gender identity (independent of sex: perceived to be both a boy and a girl, perceived to be neither a boy nor a girl, variable). Of the respondents, 96.9% (n = 1,329) reported cisgender identity, 0.2% (n = 3) opposite sex identification, and 2.9% (n = 40) other/non-binary gender identity. In the analyses, cisgender and transgender (= opposite sex identification or other/non-binary gender identity, n = 43) were compared.
Normative sexual experiences
Experiences of steady relationships were in this study elicited by asking “Are you in a steady relationship?” Response alternatives were “yes/not now but I have been earlier/I have not been in a steady relationship”. In the analyses, going steady was dichotomized to ever vs. never. The progression of consensual sexual experiences from lighter (e.g. holding hands) to more intimate was measured by asking if the respondent had experienced (1) kissing on the mouth (yes/no), (2) light petting (fondling on top of clothes, yes/no), (3) heavy petting (fondling under clothes or naked, yes/no) and (4) sexual intercourse (yes/no) (32). All these experiences are normative, i.e. most of the adolescents gain these experiences and they are largely considered a part of ordinary development (2).
Sexual harassment
Experiences of sexual harassment were studied here as dependent variables. The adolescents were asked if they had ever experienced any of the following: (1) Disturbing sexual propositions or harassment by telephone or through the Internet; (2) Sexually insulting name-calling such as poof or whore; (3) Being touched in intimate body parts against one’s will; (4) Being pressured or coerced into sex; (5) Being offered money, goods or drugs/alcohol in payment for sex. The response alternatives to all five questions were yes/no. For the purposes of the present study, we classified the items according to Fitzgerald et al. (33) as gender harassment (sexual name-calling), unwelcome sexual attention (disturbing propositions and/or harassment or unwelcome touching) and sexual coercion (pressured/coerced into sex and/or being offered money for sex).
Dating violence
Both subjection to and perpetration of dating violence were elicited. The respondents were asked “Have you ever been subjected to violent behaviour (such as hitting, punching, hair-pulling or similar) by a date or steady partner?” and “Have you ever acted violently (for example by hitting, punching, hair-pulling or similar) towards a date or a steady partner?”, both with response alternatives “yes” and “no”.
Confounding
Confounding factors controlled for were age, sex, honesty of responding and depression.
Age and sex
Age was calculated from date of responding and date of birth and was used as a continuous variable in the analyses. Confounding by age needs to be controlled for because during adolescence, even small age differences may have an impact on all aspects of development (34, 35). Similarly, confounding by sex is important because boys and girls develop at different rates during adolescence (36) and because subjection to sexual harassment and aggressive behaviours are unevenly distributed across sex (37, 38).
Honesty of responding
It has been demonstrated that some adolescents deliberately mispresent themselves in survey studies, exaggerating their belonging to minorities as well as their problem behaviours, symptoms and psychosocial problems (39-41). Consequently, the proportion of those belonging to minorities (such as disabled adolescents, immigrants, sexual minorities) appears implausibly high and associations between minority status and psychosocial problems are overestimated. In relation to gender identity, such overestimation may risk a perception in society that gender variant youth are victims rather than active subjects participating in building the contemporary adolescent community. A sincerity screening question (such as: “Have you responded honestly in this survey?”) has been suggested as an appropriate method for controlling for such bias (40, 41). Gender identity is currently extensively portrayed in the media (42) and is thus likely to be a topic which tempts adolescents to give facetious responses. In the present study, a sincerity screening question was presented at the end of the survey as follows: “Have you responded in this survey as honestly as possible?” with response alternatives “yes” and “no”. In the analyses, the sincerity question was used to categorize study participants into those who answered yes (87.7%), those who answered no (2.8%) and those who skipped the sincerity question (9.5%).
Depression
A Finnish modification of the short form of the Beck Depression Inventory (R-BDI) (43) was used to measure depression. R-BDI is a Finnish modification of the 13-item Beck Depression Inventory (44), in which options indicating positive mood have been added to each item. The questionnaire has been shown to possess good reliability in measuring depression in adolescent populations (45). All items are coded 0-3, thus the sum score of the scale ranges from 0 to 39. Depression was used as a continuous measure.
Statistical analyses
Distributions of the outcome variables were calculated among the whole sample and by sex. Sexual experiences, experiences of subjection to sexual harassment and experiences of dating violence as a victim or a perpetrator were first compared between cisgender and transgender adolescents using cross-tabulations with chi-square statistics/Fisher’s exact test where appropriate. Next, multivariate associations were studied using logistic regression. The sexual experiences, sexual harassment and dating violence variables were entered each in turn as the dependent variable. Gender identity was entered as the independent variable, age (continuous) and sex were controlled for. Next, the sincerity screening variable was added into the analyses, and finally depression (continuous). Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) are given. To avoid bias due to multiple testing, the cut-point for statistical significance was set at p < 0.01.
Results
Normative sexual experiences
Of all the participants, 80.3% reported having had a crush or been in love and 49.8% reported having been in a steady relationship. Types of sexual experiences grew scarcer according to increasing intimacy, with 51.7% reporting experiences of kissing, 37.7% reporting light petting, 27.0% reporting heavy petting and 18.6% reporting intercourse. Experience of either heavy petting or intercourse was reported by 28.5%.
Boys reported more commonly than girls all the normative sexual experiences elicited except having had a crush on or having been in love with somebody (Table 1).
TABLE 1.
Sexual experiences, subjection to sexual harassment and dating violence experiences (% (n/N)) among Finnish pupils in their ninth year at school and comparison between girls and boys
Girls | Boys | p girls vs. boys | |
---|---|---|---|
Has had a crush or been in love | 79.9 (518/648) | 80.6 (541/671) | 0.75 |
Ever in a steady relationship | 42.0 (272/648) | 57.5 (383/666) | < 0.001 |
Kissing | 48.0 (307/639) | 55.2 (365/661) | 0.006 |
Light petting | 33.5 (211(629) | 41.7 (272/652) | 0.002 |
Heavy petting | 24.2 (152/627) | 29.6 (190/641) | 0.02 |
Intercourse | 15.7 (100/637) | 21.5 (140/651) | 0.005 |
Heavy petting or intercourse | 25.4 (158/621) | 31.5 (199/632) | 0.01 |
Gender harassment | 25.5 (164/644) | 21.1 (140/663) | 0.04 |
Unwelcome attention | 25.0 (160/641) | 6.5 (43/664) | < 0.001 |
Sexual coercion | 8.0 (51/637) | 2.6 (17/657) | < 0.001 |
Subjection to dating violence | 2.9 (18/618) | 4.8 (30/628) | 0.06 |
Perpetration of dating violence | 1.8 (11/614) | 3.8 (24/624) | 0.02 |
Experience of having a crush/being in love was borderline less common among transgender than cisgender adolescents (67.5% vs. 80.9%, p = 0.03), and experiences of steady relationships borderline more common among transgender identifying youth (66.7% vs. 49.5%, p = 0.03), but otherwise the proportions of none of the normative sexual experiences differed between cisgender and transgender adolescents. We found no discernible difference in reported normative sexual experiences in multivariate comparisons between cisgender and transgender adolescents after controlling for age and sex and further for honesty of responding and depression (Table 2).
TABLE 2.
Odds Ratios (95% confidence intervals) for dating and sexual experiences, sexual harassment and dating violence among transgender identifying 15-year-old adolescents as compared to their cisgender peers in Finland
Model 1. Controlled for sex and age | Model 2. Controlled for sex, age and reported honesty of responding | Model 3. Controlled for sex, age, honesty and depressive symptoms | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
cisgender | transgender | p | cisgender | transgender | p | cisgender | transgender | p | |
Love and romance | |||||||||
Has had a crush or been in love | ref | 0.5 (0.3-1.0) | 0.04 | ref | 0.5 (0.3-1.0) | 0.05 | ref | 0.5 (0.2-0.9) | 0.03 |
Ever in a steady relationship | ref | 2.3 (1.1-4.6) | 0.02 | ref | 2.2 (1.1-4.6) | 0.03 | ref | 1.6 (0.8-3.4) | 0.20 |
Sexual experiences | |||||||||
Kissing | ref | 1.4 (0.7-2.7) | 0.31 | ref | 1.3 (0.7-2.5) | 0.41 | ref | 1.0 (0.5-2.0) | 0.93 |
Light petting | ref | 1.1 (0.6-2.1) | 0.85 | ref | 1.1 (0.5-2.0) | 0.88 | ref | 0.8 (0.4-1.6) | 0.54 |
Heavy petting | ref | 1.3 (0.7-2.7) | 0.43 | ref | 1.3 (0.7-2.7) | 0.43 | ref | 1.0 (0.5-2.19 | 0.98 |
Intercourse | ref | 1.2 (0.6-2.8) | 0.59 | ref | 1.3 (0.6-2.8) | 0.57 | ref | 0.9 (0.4-2.1) | 0.83 |
Heavy petting or intercourse | ref | 1.5 (0.7-2.9) | 0.29 | ref | 1.4 (0.7-2.9) | 0.31 | ref | 1.1(0.6-2.3) | 0.73 |
Subjection to sexual harassment | |||||||||
Gender harassment | ref | 1.1 (0.5-2.3) | 0.79 | ref | 1.0 (0.5-2.2) | 0.94 | ref | 0.7 80.3-1.5) | 0.32 |
Unwelcome sexual attention | ref | 1.3 (0.6-2.9) | 0.56 | ref | 1.3 (0.6-3.1) | 0.48 | ref | 0.9 (04-2.2) | 0.87 |
Sexual coercion | ref | 3.2 (1.3-8.1) | 0.01 | ref | 3.2 (1.3-8.1) | 0.01 | ref | 2.3 (0.9-6.0) | 0.09 |
Dating violence | |||||||||
Subjection to dating violence | ref | 3.1 (1.0-9.0) | 0.04 | ref | 2.6 (0.9-8.0) | 0.09 | ref | 1.4 (0.4-4.7) | 0.56 |
Perpetration of dating violence | ref | 4.4 (1.5-13.3) | 0.008 | ref | 4.3 (1.4-13.6) | 0.01 | ref | 2.2 (0.6-7.6) | 0.21 |
Subjection to sexual harassment and experiences of dating violence
Of all participants, experiences of gender harassment were reported by 23.3%, unwelcome sexual attention by 15.6% and sexual coercion by 5.3%. Girls reported more experiences of subjection to unwelcome attention and sexual coercion than boys (Table 1). Transgender identifying adolescents reported more commonly subjection to sexual coercion than cisgender youth (15.4% vs. 5.0%, p = 0.01).
Experiences of sexual coercion persisted as associated with transgender identity after controlling for age, sex and reported honesty of responding, but adding depression levelled out differences between transgender and cisgender identifying youth (Table 2).
Of all the respondents, 3.9% reported experiences of subjection to dating violence and 2.8% reported perpetration of dating violence, with no statistical difference between sexes (Table 1). Before controlling for confounding factors, perpetration of dating violence was borderline more common among transgender adolescents. After controlling for age, sex and reported honesty of responding, we found that transgender identity was associated with increased Odds Ratios for reporting perpetration of dating violence (Table 2). Finally, when depression was entered into the model the difference between gender identities in dating violence perpetration was levelled out.
Discussion
In our non-selected population, normative sexual experiences of transgender identifying youth did not systematically differ from those of mainstream, cisgender identifying youth. Transgender identifying adolescents presented with neither delayed nor with excessively advanced sexual experiences. This is in line with the findings of Veale et al. (19) but differed from those of Bungener et al. (18) and Kaltiala-Heino et al. (17), who found that transgender identifying adolescents had fewer sexual experiences, and of Korchmaros et al. (10), who reported that transgender adolescents had more sexual experiences than their cisgender counterparts. However, earlier research has focused on adolescents referred to mental health services due to symptoms of gender dysphoria (17, 18), activist cohorts (19) or they have not focused specifically on transgender identifying youth but rather on LGBTQ youth as whole (10), for example. Our novel contribution is that we compared sexual development between transgender and cisgender youth in a general population sample. Clinically referred adolescents with gender dysphoria often have concurrent mental health disorders (11-13), which could inhibit or intensify the accumulation of sexual experiences, for example due to sexual risk taking or insecurities related to gender dysphoria while transgender identifying individuals recruited through LGBQT networks do not represent the general population. Our results indicate that in terms of sexual development transgender identifying youth are equally well functioning and equally developing individuals as their cisgender peers in the general population.
Several studies have suggested that transgender adolescents are excessively subjected to sexual harassment, more than both cisgender males and females of any sexual orientation (22, 46-48). However, these studies were not based on non-selected population samples, and they did not distinguish between different types of sexual harassment. In our data, transgender reported subjection to gender harassment and unwelcome sexual attention did not differ according to gender identity. This does not appear to fully support the assumption of sexually harassing interactions serving as a mechanism for maintaining heteronormativity in everyday communication.
When sex, age and honesty of responding were controlled for, gender minority youth nevertheless reported increased experiences of the most severe form of sexual harassment, namely sexual coercion. However, when depression was controlled for, transgender adolescents were not statistically significantly more likely to report experiences of sexual coercion. This warrants further study. Depression may originate from special challenges faced by gender minority youth but could also predispose to identity struggles (49). Gender minority youth could be more vulnerable to sexual coercion due to aggression originating from prejudice, but on the other hand, trauma such as sexual coercion could lead to depression. Additionally, depression could predispose to hostile attribution bias and increased reporting of traumatic experiences. In order to assess causal relationships, longitudinal studies are warranted.
Subjection to dating violence was equally common among transgender and cisgender youth. Instead, trans youth differed from their cisgender peers in reporting more commonly perpetration of dating violence, a finding that persisted when age, sex and honesty of reporting were controlled for. Dank et al. (21) reported similar findings with regard to perpetration of dating violence, but different from our results, found that transgender youth were also more frequently subjected to dating violence. The study by Dank et al. (21), however, was limited by a very small sample of transgender adolescents. Some studies have similarly observed that sexual minority youth engage in peer aggression perpetration more than mainstream youth (50, 51). This has been attributed to elevated peer victimization (50), when aggressive behaviour would be a coping mechanism, or self-protective behaviour hiding the perpetrator’s own vulnerability (52). However, these explanations no not appear feasible in our data, as gender minority youth did not systematically report increased subjection to peer aggression in the field of dating and sexuality. Finally, the association between transgender identity and dating violence perpetration levelled out when depression was added into the model. This suggests that it may rather be depression, common among gender minority youth (11-13), than transgender identity that is per se associated with dating violence perpetration.
A novel approach in the present study was to control for reliability of responding when comparing gender minority and mainstream youth. As previously discussed, some adolescents deliberately mispresent themselves in survey studies, exaggerating their belonging to minorities as well as their problem behaviours, symptoms and psychosocial problems. Such dishonesty in responding could skew the results, especially in studies where the proportion of gender minorities within the study sample is small. Our logistic regression models indicated that controlling for honesty decreased Odds Ratios across the board. However, none of the statistically significant findings in the bivariate associations exceeded the cut-point of p < 0.01 after controlling for honesty, which adds credibility to our findings.
Strengths and limitations of this study
Instead of a clinical or an activist sample, we used an unselected sample of adolescents representative of Finnish youth in the general population. In Finland, nine years attendance at comprehensive school is mandatory from the age of seven until sixteen, and over 99% of children of compulsory school age are enrolled in a publicly run comprehensive school. A large portion of earlier research focusing on the area of trans youth sexuality has used selected samples. Our sample was socio-demographically representative of the 9th graders of comprehensive school in the whole country (53). We focused specifically on gender identity and compared transgender and cisgender adolescents. Many earlier studies have drawn conclusions about trans youth development based on findings concerning LGBTQ youth as whole. Thus, our study adds specifically to the knowledge of gender minority youth.
Even though our sample was not small, the small number of transgender adolescents (n = 43) limited us to analysing only transgender youth as a whole rather than separating non-binary and opposite sex identification in our study, which we concede to be a weakness.
We identified trans youth with two separate questions. Such a two-step method to identify gender identity has been recommended by Eisenberg et al. (54) and Reisner et al. (55). The first question (“What is your sex?”), which also opened the survey, focused explicitly on the responder’s sex as indicated in official documents, such as birth certificate or ID card. Gender identity was then later assessed with a separate question (“Do you perceive yourself to be…”) in a section assessing health, after questions about perceived health, height and weight. Same method has been previously used with Finnish adolescent population samples (31).
It has previously been shown that the presentation of a survey item may affect or influence respondents’ answers. For example, eliciting “sexual harassment” may affect respondents’ ability to recall events related to the subject of the question (56). In order to avoid bias, the term “sexual harassment” was omitted from the survey and experiences of sexual harassment were instead measured by asking if respondents’ had experienced certain behaviours. However, in contrast to dating violence, the survey unfortunately did not include questions about perpetration of sexual harassment, so we were unable to examine potential differences or similarities between trans and cis youth in this respect.
Controlling for honesty of responding is a strength of the present study. Adolescents deliberately mispresent themselves in survey studies, exaggerating their belonging to minorities as well as their problem behaviours, symptoms and psychosocial problems (39-41), and this distorts our understanding of minorities. Honesty screening question, like in the present study, has been shown a valid method for controlling such bias (40, 41). Gender identity has been shown to be a topic vulnerable to facetious responding (31).
Controlling for age is important, because during adolescence even small differences in age may have a large impact on all aspects of development, especially on sexuality (35, 57).
In studying associations between transgender identity and sexuality related issues among adolescents, sex needs to be controlled for as sex differences have been reported in both aspects of normative sexual development (15) and traumatic sexual experiences (58), as well as in reporting transgender identity (31) and gender dysphoria (12) in adolescence.
We consider controlling for depression a strength of the present study as transgender identity is associated with depression (59) and depression is related to sexual behaviour (15) as well as to traumatic sexual experiences such as sexual harassment (58). Previous studies on the subject of gender minority youth and sexuality have not taken depression into consideration. In our data, approximately 16% of the pupils reported depression that could be classified as intermediate or severe depression. This is a typical finding in a Finnish population-based adolescent sample (60) and adds to the generalizability of our results.
Conclusion
Transgender identifying adolescents in the general population are developing similarly to their cisgender identifying peers in the accumulation and nature of normative sexual experiences. Transgender identity need not entail problems in sexual development and, on the other hand, problems or delays in emotional and physical sexual closeness are not a result of transgender identity but regardless of gender identity may indicate a need for counselling. This is important for professionals working with adolescents, for example in schools and in health care. Transgender youth seem to be more susceptible to subjection to sexual harassment and dating violence perpetration than their cisgender peers. However, this association could in fact relate more closely to depression, a prevalent phenomenon among trans youth, than transgender identity itself. Additional studies, especially longitudinal ones, are warranted in order to better understand the interplay between gender identity, sexuality related violence, and depression among adolescents.
Author disclosure statement
The authors have no conflicts of interest.
1. Dalenberg WG, Timmerman MC, van Geert, Paul LC. Dutch Adolescents’ everyday expressions of sexual Bbhavior trajectories over a 2-year period: a mixed-methods study. Arch Sex Behav 2018;47(6):1811-23.
2. Cacciatore R, Korteniemi-Poikela E, Kaltiala R. The steps of sexuality—a developmental, emotion-focused, child-centered model of sexual development and sexuality education from birth to adulthood. Int J Sex Health 2019;31(3):319-38.
3. Manning WD, Longmore MA, Giordano PC. Adolescents’ involvement in non-romantic sexual activity. Soc Sci Res 2005;34(2):384-407.
4. Adelson SL. Practice parameter on gay, lesbian, or bisexual sexual orientation, gender nonconformity, and gender discordance in children and adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2012;51(9):957-74.
5. Hembree WC, Cohen-Kettenis PT, Gooren L, Hannema SE, Meyer WJ, Murad MH, et al. Endocrine treatment of gender-dysphoric/gender-incongruent persons: An Endocrine Society Clinical Practice Guideline. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2017;102(11):3869-903.
6. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 2013.
7. de Vries, Annelou LC, McGuire JK, Steensma TD, Wagenaar ECF, Doreleijers TAH, Cohen-Kettenis P. Young adult psychological outcome after puberty suppression and gender reassignment. Pediatrics 2014;134(4):696-704.
8. DeHaan S, Kuper LE, Magee JC, Bigelow L, Mustanski BS. The interplay between online and offline explorations of identity, relationships, and sex: a mixed-methods study with LGBT youth. J Sex Res 2013;50(5):421-34.
9. Doorduin T, van Berlo W. Trans people’s experience of sexuality in the Netherlands: a pilot study. J Homosex 2014;61(5):654-72.
10. Korchmaros JD, Ybarra ML, Mitchell KJ. Adolescent online romantic relationship initiation: differences by sexual and gender identification. J Adolesc 2015;40:54-64.
11. Holt V, Skagerberg E, Dunsford M. Young people with features of gender dysphoria: Demographics and associated difficulties. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry 2016;21(1):108-18.
12. Kaltiala-Heino R, Sumia M, Työläjärvi M, Lindberg N. Two years of gender identity service for minors: overrepresentation of natal girls with severe problems in adolescent development. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health 2015;9:9.
13. Olson J, Schrager SM, Belzer M, Simons LK, Clark LF. Baseline physiologic and psychosocial characteristics of transgender youth seeking care for gender dysphoria. J Adolesc Health 2015;57(4):374-80.
14. Kaltiala-Heino R, Fröjd S, Marttunen M. Depression, conduct disorder, smoking and alcohol use as predictors of sexual activity in middle adolescence: a longitudinal study. Health Psychol Behav Med 2015;3(1):25-39.
15. Savioja H, Helminen M, Fröjd S, Marttunen M, Kaltiala-Heino R. Sexual experience and self-reported depression across the adolescent years. Health Psychol Behav Med 2015;3(1):337-47.
16. Savioja H, Helminen M, Fröjd S, Marttunen M, Kaltiala-Heino R. Delinquency and sexual experiences across adolescence: does depression play a role? Eur J Contracept Reprod Health Care 2017;22(4):298-304.
17. Kaltiala-Heino R, Työläjärvi M, Lindberg N. Sexual experiences of clinically referred adolescents with features of gender dysphoria. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry 2019;24(2):365-78.
18. Bungener SL, Steensma TD, Cohen-Kettenis PT, de Vries ALC. Sexual and romantic experiences of transgender youth before gender-affirmative treatment. Pediatrics 2017;139(3):e20162283.
19. Veale J, Watson RJ, Adjei J, Saewyc E. Prevalence of pregnancy involvement among Canadian transgender youth and its relation to mental health, sexual health, and gender identity. Int J Transgend 2016;17(3-4):107-13.
20. Cerwenka S, Nieder TO, Cohen-Kettenis P, Cuypere G, Hebold Haraldsen IR, Kreukels B, et al. Sexual behavior of gender-dysphoric individuals before gender-confirming interventions: a European multicenter study. J Sex Marital Ther 2014;40:457-71.
21. Dank M, Lachman P, Zweig JM, Yahner J. Dating violence experiences of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth. J Youth Adolesc 2014;43(5):846-57.
22. Mitchell KJ, Ybarra ML, Korchmaros JD. Sexual harassment among adolescents of different sexual orientations and gender identities. Child Abuse Negl 2014;38(2):280-95.
23. Kaltiala-Heino R, Lindberg N, Fröjd S, Haravuori H, Marttunen M. Adolescents with same-sex interest: experiences of sexual harassment are more common among boys. Health Psychol Behav Med 2019;7(1):105-27.
24. Chesir-Teran D. Conceptualizing and assessing heterosexism in high schools: a setting-level approach. Am J Community Psychol 2003;31(3):267-79.
25. Pina A, Gannon T, Saunders B. An overview of the literature on sexual harassment: perpetrator, theory, and treatment issues. Aggress Violent Behav 2009;14:126-38.
26. Street AE, Gradus JL, Stafford J, Kelly K. Gender differences in experiences of sexual harassment: Data from a male-dominated environment. J Consult Clin Psychol 2007;75(3):464-74.
27. Nuttbrock LA, Hwahng SJ. Ethnicity, sex work, and incident HIV/STI among transgender women in New York City: a three year prospective study. AIDS Behav 2017;21(12):3328-35.
28. Brennan J, Kuhns LM, Johnson AK, Belzer M, Wilson EC, Garofalo R; Adolescent Medicine Trials Network for HIV/AIDS Interventions. Syndemic theory and HIV-related risk among young transgender women: the role of multiple, co-occurring health problems and social marginalization. Am J Public Health 2012;102(9):1751-7.
29. Wilson EC, Garofalo R, Harris RD, Herrick A, Martinez M, Martinez J, Belzer M; Transgender Advisory Committee and the Adolescent Medicine Trials Network for HIV/AIDS Interventions. Transgender female youth and sex work: HIV risk and a comparison of life factors related to engagement in sex work. AIDS Behav 2009;13(5):902-13
30. Dean L, Meyer IH, Robinson K, Sell RL, Sember R, Silenzio VMB, et al. Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender health: findings and concerns. J Gay Lesbian Med Assoc 2000;4(3):102-51.
31. Kaltiala-Heino R, Lindberg N. Gender identities in adolescent population: methodological issues and prevalence across age groups. Eur Psychiatry 2019;55:61-6.
32. Kaltiala-Heino R, Kosunen E, Rimpelä M. Pubertal timing, sexual behaviour and self-reported depression in middle adolescence. J Adolesc 2003;26(5):531-45.
33. Fitzgerald LF, Gelfand MJ, Drasgow F. Measuring sexual harassment: theoretical and psychometric advances. Basic Appl Soc Psych 1995;17(4):425-45.
34. Steinberg L. Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci 2005 Feb;9(2):69-74.
35. Dahl RE, Allen NB, Wilbrecht L, Suleiman AB. Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective. Nature 2018;554(7693):441-50.
36. Fechner PY. Gender differences in puberty. J Adolesc Health 2002;30(4):44-8.
37. Berkout OV, Young JN, Gross AM. Mean girls and bad boys: recent research on gender differences in conduct disorder. Aggress Violent Behav 2011;16(6):503-11.
38. Kaltiala-Heino R, Fröjd S, Marttunen M. Sexual harassment victimization in adolescence: associations with family background. Child Abuse Negl 2016;56:11-9.
39. Robinson-Cimpian J. Inaccurate estimation of disparities due to mischievous responders: several suggestions to assess conclusions. Educ Res 2014;43(4):171-85.
40. Fan X, Miller B, Park K, Winward B, Christensen M, Grotevant H, et al. An exploratory study about inaccuracy and invalidity in adolescent self-report surveys. Field Methods 2006;18(3):223-44.
41. Cornell D, Klein J, Konold T, Huang F. Effects of validity screening items on adolescent survey data. Psychol Assess 2012;24(1):21-35.
42. Marchiano L. Outbreak: on transgender teens and psychic epidemics. Psychol Perspect 2017;60(3):345-66.
43. Raitasalo R. Mielialakysely: Suomen oloihin Beckin lyhyen depressiokyselyn pohjalta kehitetty masennusoireilun ja itsetunnon kysely [Mood survey: Depression symptom and self-esteem survey developed in Finland based on Beck’s short depression survey]. Helsinki: Kela; 2007.
44. Beck AT, Beck RW. Screening depressed patients in family practice. A rapid technic. Postgrad Med 1972;52(6):81-5.
45. Kaltiala-Heino R, Laippala P, Rimpelä M, Rantanen P. Finnish modification of the 13-item Beck Depression Inventory in screening an adolescent population for depressiveness and positive mood. Nord J Psychiatry 1999;53(6):451-7.
46. Toomey RB, McGuire JK, Russell ST. Heteronormativity, school climates, and perceived safety for gender nonconforming peers. J Adolesc 2012;35(1):187-96.
47. Ybarra ML, Mitchell KJ, Palmer NA, Reisner SL. Online social support as a buffer against online and offline peer and sexual victimization among U.S. LGBT and non-LGBT youth. Child Abuse Negl 2015;39:123-36.
48. Devís-Devís J, Pereira-García S, Valencia-Peris A, Fuentes-Miguel J, López-Cañada E, Pérez-Samaniego V. Harassment patterns and risk profile in Spanish trans persons. J Homosex 2017;64(2):239-55.
49. Testa RJ, Habarth J, Peta J, Balsam K, Bockting W. Development of the gender minority stress and resilience measure. Psychol Sex Orientat Gend Divers 2015;2(1):65-77.
50. Berlan ED, Corliss HL, Field AE, Goodman E, Bryn Austin S. Sexual orientation and bullying among adolescents in the growing up today study. J Adolesc Health 2010;46(4):366-71.
51. Eisenberg ME, Gower AL, McMorris BJ, Bucchianeri MM. Vulnerable bullies: perpetration of peer harassment among youths across sexual orientation, weight, and disability status. Am J Public Health 2015;105(9):1784-91.
52. Eisenberg ME, Gower AL, McMorris BJ. Emotional health of lesbian, gay, bisexual and questioning bullies: does it differ from straight bullies? J Youth Adolesc 2016;45(1):105-16.
53. Knaappila N, Marttunen M, Fröjd S, Lindberg N, Kaltiala-Heino R. Socioeconomic trends in school bullying among Finnish adolescents from 2000 to 2015. Child Abuse Negl 2018;86:100-8.
54. Eisenberg ME, Gower AL, McMorris BJ, Rider GN, Shea G, Coleman E. Risk and protective factors in the lives of transgender/gender nonconforming adolescents. J Adolesc Health 2017;61(4):521-26.
55. Reisner SL, Conron KJ, Tardiff LA, Jarvi S, Gordon AR, Austin SB. Monitoring the health of transgender and other gender minority populations: validity of natal sex and gender identity survey items in a U.S. national cohort of young adults. BMC Public Health 2014;14:1224.
56. Galesic M, Tourangeau R. What is hexual Harassment? It depends on who asks! Framing effects on survey responses. Appl Cognit Psychol 2007;21:189-202
57. Laursen B, Hartl AC. Understanding loneliness during adolescence: developmental changes that increase the risk of perceived social isolation. J Adolesc 2013;36(6):1261-8.
58. Kaltiala R, Savioja H, Fröjd S, Marttunen M. Experiences of sexual harassment are associated with the sexual behavior of 14- to 18-year-old adolescents. Child Abuse Negl 2018;77:46-57.
59. Kaltiala-Heino R, Bergman H, Työläjärvi M, Frisén L. Gender dysphoria in adolescence: current perspectives. Adolesc Health Med Ther 2018;9:31-41.
60. Knaappila Noora. Are emotional symptoms increasing in adolescent population? [Submitted for publication]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2020. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Sexuality is a major facet of development during adolescence. Apace with normal sexual development, sexual experiences become more common and intimate. Recent research reports mixed results as to whether this is the case among transgender identifying adolescents. Recent research also suggests that trans youth experience negative sexual experiences (such as dating violence and sexual harassment) more often than their cisgender identifying peers. However, most studies have had clinical or selected samples.
The aim of this study is to compare the normative as well as negative sexual experiences of trans youth with their cisgender peers in the general population.
Our study included 1386 pupils of the ninth year of comprehensive school in Finland, mean age (SD) 15.59 (0.41) years. We compared sexual experiences, sexual harassment and dating violence among trans youth and their cisgender identifying peers. Distributions of the outcome variables were calculated among the whole sample and by sex. Next, multivariate associations were studied using logistic regression adjusting for age, sex, honesty of responding and depression. Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) are given.
After adjusting for age, sex, honesty of responding and ultimately for depression, normative sexual experiences of trans youth did not differ systematically from those of the mainstream, cisgender identifying youth. After adjusting for sex, age and honesty, transgender youth had increased Odds Ratios for experiences of sexual coercion and dating violence perpetration. In the final models however, no statistically significant differences were detected in the negative sexual experiences between transgender and cisgender youth.
Transgender identifying adolescents presented neither with delayed nor with excessively advanced sexual experiences. However, transgender youth seem to be more susceptible to subjection to sexual coercion and, unexpectedly, dating violence perpetration than their cisgender peers. However, these associations may in fact relate more closely to depression, a prevalent phenomenon among trans youth, than transgender identity itself.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 University of Tampere, Faculty of Medicine and Health Technology, Tampere, Finland
2 University of Tampere, Faculty of Social Sciences, Tampere, Finland
3 University of Helsinki and Helsinki University Hospital, Adolescent Psychiatry, Helsinki, Finland
4 University of Tampere, Faculty of Medicine and Health Technology, Tampere, Finland; Tampere University Hospital, Department of Adolescent Psychiatry, Tampere, Finland; anha Vaasa Hospital, Vaasa, Finland