Background: Older adults show a high variability in cognitive performance that cannot be explained by aging alone. Although research has linked air pollution and noise to cognitive impairment and structural brain alterations, the potential impact of air pollution and noise on functional brain organization is unknown.
Objective: This study examined the associations between long-term air pollution and traffic noise with measures of functional brain organization in older adults. We hypothesize that exposures to high air pollution and noise levels are associated with age-like changes in functional brain organization, shown by less segregated brain networks.
Methods: Data from 574 participants (44.1% female, 56-85 years of age) in the German 1000BRAINS study (2011-2015) were analyzed. Exposure to particulate matter (PMio, PM2.5, and PM2.5 absorbance), accumulation mode particle number (PNam)> and nitrogen dioxide (NO2) was estimated applying land-use regression and chemistry transport models. Noise exposures were assessed as weighted 24-h (Lden) and nighttime (Lmght) means. Functional brain organization of seven established brain networks (visual, sensorimotor, dorsal and ventral attention, limbic, frontoparietal and default network) was assessed using resting-state functional brain imaging data. To assess functional brain organization, we determined the degree of segregation between networks by comparing the strength of functional connections within and between networks. We estimated associations between air pollution and noise exposure with network segregation, applying multiple linear regression models adjusted for age, sex, socioeconomic status, and lifestyle variables.
Results: Overall, small associations of high exposures with lesser segregated networks were visible. For the sensorimotor networks, we observed small associations between high air pollution and noise and lower network segregation, which had a similar effect size as a 1-y increase in age [e.g., in sensorimotor network, -0.006 (95% CI: -0.021, 0.009) per 0.3 Xl0"5/m increase in PM2.5 absorbance and -0.004 (95% CI: -0.006, -0.002) per 1-y age increase].
Conclusion: High exposure to air pollution and noise was associated with less segregated functional brain networks, https://doi.org/10.1289/ EHP97372015). Given that cognitive decline and reduced brain volume are also characteristic of normal neuronal aging, it has been suggested that air pollution may accelerate brain aging (Clifford et al. 2016; Fougere et al. 2015).
Chronic ambient noise is another growing environmental health problem, stemming from similar air pollution sources (e.g., traffic, transport, industry) (WHO 2011). Chronic noise can lead to annoyance, sleep disturbance, and stress, arousing the autonomous nervous and endocrine system, thereby leading to adverse health outcomes, most prominently cardiovascular diseases (Basner et al. 2014; WHO 2011). In addition, research has linked chronic noise to impaired cognitive function (Fuks et al. 2019; Jafari et al. 2019; Tzivian et al. 2016) and possible changes in brain structure (Cheng et al. 2019; NuBbaum et al. 2020) in adults. However, the precise mechanisms by which noise may affect the brain remain largely unknown.
Besides morphological changes, neurological studies on the aging brain also showed changes on the level of functional brain organization related to impaired cognitive function (Betzel et al. 2014; Ramanoel et al. 2018; Armstrong et al. 2020; Stumme et al. 2020). Although brain structure and functional organization are different aspects of the brain that require different analytical methods, they are both important to understand how environmental exposures may affect cognitive function. Therefore, to gain a better understanding of the complex relationship between environmental exposures, brain structure, and cognitive function, studies on how air pollution and noise may influence functional brain organization in the whole brain are needed. The brain is a complex system consisting of different brain regions interconnected with each other for information processing (Biswal et al. 2010).
Introduction
Every day, people are involuntarily exposed to a harmful mixture of gases and particles called air pollution that increases their risk for cardiovascular and respiratory diseases (Thurston et al. 2017). Air pollution may also affect the brain by inducing oxidative stress and chronic neuroinflammation, possibly leading to impaired cognition (as reviewed by Allen et al. 2017) and increasing incidence of neuronal diseases (Casanova et al. 2016; Jung et al. 2015; Peters et al. 2019). These observations lead to the question whether alterations in brain structure might explain the reported impaired cognitive performance. Anatomical neuroimaging studies have shown associations between high air pollution and lower brain volume (Casanova et al. 2016; Chen et al. 2015; NuBbaum et al. 2020; Power et al. 2011; Wilker et al.
Functional connectivity (FC) describes the strength of connectivity between these different brain regions (Friston et al. 1996). Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during a resting state (Biswal et al. 1995), the brain can be grouped into seven functionally specialized networks (i.e., visual, sensorimotor, dorsal and ventral attention, limbic, frontoparietal, default network) (Schaefer et al. 2018). Within each network, brain regions are highly interconnected, whereas there are few connections between brain regions from different networks (Bassett and Bullmore 2006) (Figure 1A). Therefore, functional brain organization is characterized by a balance between densely connected regions within networks together with few long-range connections between more remote regions and networks (Wig 2017).
From childhood to adulthood, the brain undergoes various age-related changes in FC. Although there are many open questions regarding the maturation of functional networks, it is believed that networks are already to some extent established in infants, showing a coarser network structure compared with adults (Grayson and Fair 2017). With age, this network structure is refined with increasing connections between functionally related brain regions and the strengthening of long-range connections between functionally related, but anatomically distant, brain regions (Grayson and Fair 2017). Thereby a pattern of segregation (decrease in connectivity strength) between anatomically close brain regions and integration (increase in connectivity strength) between functionally related brain regions emerges, and an organized, widespread system of networks is formed (Bassett and Bullmore 2006) (Figure IB).
Neuroimaging studies investigating the association between age and functional brain organization in healthy adults reported decreasing segregation with age in various networks (Betzel et al. 2014; Chan et al. 2014; Geerligs et al. 2015; Malagurski et al. 2020; Stumme et al. 2020). Furthermore, decreasing segregationwas also linked to declining cognitive performance in adults, such as poor memory function (Chan et al. 2014), reduced learning rates (Iordan et al. 2018), and low processing speed (Ng et al. 2016), supporting the suggestion that preserving segregated brain networks with age is important for good cognitive functioning.
One study investigated the impact of indoor household incense burning on cognitive functions and brain health in older adults (Wong et al. 2020). They reported associations between regular incense burning with poorer cognitive performance and decreased FC within regions of the default mode network. Other studies exploring the impact of environmental pollutants on functional brain organization have only been conducted in children or adolescents and reported slower functional brain maturation (Pujol et al. 2016) or lower FC strength within brain regions for emotion processing and regulation (de Water et al. 2018), as well as for motor control (de Water et al. 2019). However, the brain is not susceptible to environmental factors only during early brain development, but also in later life when progressive neurocogni-tive degeneration starts (Park and Reuter-Lorenz 2009; Wig 2017) and compensatory mechanisms begin to falter owing to aging (Grossman 2014). Although average cognitive performance declines with age, this is not the case for all older adults, and there is a large variability among older adults that has yet to be explained (Nelson and Dannefer 1992). To our knowledge, there have been no studies on long-term outdoor air pollution and functional brain organization in older adults.
Related to noise, few studies have investigated the impact of environmental noise on functional organization in the adult brain, and these point to a less segregated network structure for high levels of short-term ambient noise exposure (Zou et al. 2019) and neuronal loss together with reduced neuronal activity under chronic noise and stress (Cheng et al. 2019). Because air pollution and environmental noise often occur at the same timeowing to their common sources (e.g., motorized traffic), it is important to consider both exposures simultaneously, especially if they may have synergistic or antagonistic effects.
In this population-based 1000BRAINS study, we therefore aimed to examine whether long-term exposure to ambient air pollution and road traffic noise are associated with global functional brain organization by creating whole-brain functional networks from resting-state MRI data in a cohort of people 55-85 years of age. Based on our prior work showing associations of air pollution with age-like morphological changes in the brain (NuBbaum et al. 2020), we hypothesized that global functional brain organization shows age-like alterations, namely a decrease in segregation index, under high exposure to air pollution and noise. With this study, we want to contribute to the understanding of how ubiquitous external factors, such as air pollution and noise, act on brain function, possibly mediating the already observed adverse effects on cognition.
Materials and Methods
Study Design
This analysis was conducted using data from participants of the 1000BRAINS study (age range: 56-85 y), a cohort study examining interindividual variability of structure, function, and connectivity in the aging brain in relation to genetic and environmental influencing factors (Caspers et al. 2014). For the 1000BRAINS study, participants from the German Heinz Nixdorf Recall (HNR) study and the HNR MultiGeneration Study were recruited (Figure SI). This analysis includes only participants recruited from the HNR study because air pollution and noise exposure data were not available for participants from the HNR MultiGeneration study. The HNR study is a prospective cohort study of randomly selected men and women living in three adjacent cities (Bochum, Essen, and Mulheim) in the densely populated Ruhr area (Schmermund et al. 2002). Between December 2000 and August 2003,4,814 people (age range: 45-75 y) attended the baseline examination, where data from self-administered questionnaires, face-to-face interviews along with anthropometric measurements, and laboratory tests were collected. A 5-y follow-up examination was conducted between 2006 and 2008 (" = 4,157), with repeated assessments of medical condition and risk factor status. HNR participants attending the 10-y follow-up (2011-2015; n = 3,087) were invited to participate in the 1000BRAINS study. Participants had to be physically able and have no medical contraindications to undergo MRI. Each participant underwent MRI scans along with neuropsychological and motor testing (Caspers et al. 2014). Compliance with prompts during MRI was assessed by post-MRI questionnaires. For this analysis, eligible functional MRI data from 685 participants were available. Both the HNR and 1000B RAINS study were approved by the ethics committee of the University Hospital Essen. Participants gave informed consent and all study procedures complied with the Declaration of Helsinki (WMA 2013).
Air Pollution. To model point-specific residential long-term exposure concentrations of particulate matter (PM) of aerodynamic diameter of <2.5 urn (PM2.5), <10 urn (PM10), and soot [PM2.5 absorbance (PM2.5abs)L as well as nitrogen dioxide (NO2), we used the European Study of Cohorts for Air Pollution Effects land-use regression (ESCAPE-LUR) model with region-specific land use data. For detailed description of the model, see Beelen et al. (2007). In short, PM was monitored at 20 sites and nitrogen oxides at 40 sites in the Ruhr area in three different 2-wk periods between October 2008 and October 2009 to reflect different seasons (Beelen et al. 2013; Eeftens et al. 2012). The LUR model was created using annual averages of measured pollutant concentrations from background and traffic-related monitoring sites andpredictor variables from Europe-wide and local Geographic Information System databases. To model long-term exposure, participants were assigned point-specific exposure concentrations based on their address at HNR baseline examination (2000-2003), ~ 10 y prior to their 1000BRAINS examination.
In addition, we estimated participants' exposure to accumulation mode particle number concentration (PNam), a measure of quasi-ultrafine particles, using the validated, spatiotemporal, three-dimensional EURopean Air Pollution Dispersion (EURAD) model (Buns et al. 2012; Hass et al. 1993; Memmesheimer et al. 2004). The multilayer model simulates transport, chemical transformation, and deposition of tropospheric constituents. Ambient air concentrations were assigned to North Rhine-Westphalia and the Ruhr area using a sequential nesting grid of 1 km2 (Buns et al. 2012). The long-term exposure was calculated as average concentration from 2001 to 2003 and assigned to each participant according to the 1-km2 grid in which they lived at the time of the HNR baseline.
Traffic Noise. Long-term traffic noise was modeled at facade points according to the European Directive 2002/49/EC (European Commission 2002) as weighted 24-h (Lden) and nighttime (2200-0600 hours) mean noise (Lnight) for the year 2006. Individual residential exposures were assigned using the maximum noise value in a 10-m buffer around each participant's HNR baseline address in 2000-2003. Based on the assumption that mean noise levels have remained mostly constant over time in regard to spatial distribution and exposure level in the HNR study area, these noise exposures reflect long-term exposure. In addition, we examined indoor noise during the daytime (In-L^en) and at night (In-L^t), derived from outdoor noise levels, and took into account noise-reducing factors, such as room orientation and noise shielding (Foraster et al. 2014; Ohlweinetal. 2019).
Residential Traffic Density. To estimate exposure to traffic on a small spatial scale, we used the distance (in meters) from the HNR baseline home address to the nearest high-traffic roads (Disttrafroad), defined as roads with a traffic count of >26,000 vehicles/d, categorized into three groups (<100 m, 100-200 m, >200 m). The necessary data was acquired from the State Office for Nature, Environment and Consumer Protection of North Rhine-Westphalia.
Image Acquisition and Image Preprocessing
Imaging was performed using a 3T Siemens Tim-TRIO magnet resonance scanner with a 32-channel head coil. For resting-state fMRI, 300 functional images were acquired using a gradient-echo planar imaging sequence (36 slices, slicethickness = 3.1 mm, repetition time = 2,200 ms, echo time = 30 ms, field of view = 200 X 200 mm2, voxel resolution 3.1 X 3.1 X 3.1 mm3) (Caspers et al. 2014). Resting-state fMRI scans of 11-min duration were performed while eyes were closed, light switched off, and with the instruction to let the mind wander without thinking of anything in particular and not to fall asleep. A post-scan debriefing was performed to verify compliance with these instructions.
In the preprocessing, the first four gradient-echo planar imaging scans were removed for each participant. A two-pass procedure was applied to corrected functional images for head movements [Statistical Parametric Mapping 12 (Friston et al. 2007)]. In the first pass, all scans were aligned to the first image. In the second pass, an average image was estimated using affine registration, and all images were then aligned to the average image as a reference. To normalize all images to the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 template (Holmes et al. 1998), the unified segmentation approach (Ashburner and Friston 2005) was used. Compared with the often-applied normalization based on Tl weighted images, the unified segmentation approach increasesaccuracies of registration (Calhoun et al. 2017; Dohmatob et al. 2018). To minimize the impact of motion artifacts on measurements of FC, recent findings recommend a combined use of independent component analysis (ICA)-based Automatic Removal Of Motion Artifacts (ICA-AROMA) (Pruim et al. 2015) with global signal regression (Burgess et al. 2016; Ciric et al. 2017). Therefore, the present study included both AROMA and global signal regression. Finally, all resting-state fMRI images were bandpass filtered (0.01-0.1 Hz).
Resting-State FC. To explore the brain's global intrinsic functional organization, we used resting-state fMRI data available in the 1000BRAINS study. Because resting-state fMRI allows indirect measurement of neuronal activity at rest (task-negative state), it is possible to analyze and visualize interactions between regions across the whole brain, instead of only specific brain regions as in task-based fMRI. The resting-state fMRI depicts low frequency changes in the blood-oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signals, which arise from changes in blood flow due to an increasing neuronal metabolism resulting from neuronal activity (Fox and Raichle 2007). Functionally related brain regions are synchronously activated and therefore show a contemporaneous increase in neuronal metabolism. This FC between two brain regions can then be measured by the temporal correlation of the BOLD signals of both brain regions (Biswal et al. 1995; Fox and Raichle 2007; Margulies et al. 2007; Smith et al. 2009) (Figure 1 A). In general, regions with high values of FC (i.e., high temporal correlations of their BOLD signals) are grouped into large-scale functional networks called resting-state networks (Damoiseaux et al. 2006; Schaefer et al. 2018; Yeoetal. 2011).
To define large-scale brain networks, we used Schaefer's cerebral cortex parcellation with 400 distinct brain regions partitioned into seven large-scale networks: visual, sensorimotor, dorsal and ventral attention, limbic, frontoparietal, and default network (Schaefer et al. 2018). To calculate FC for these large-scale brain networks, we used a graph-theory approach, where functional brain networks are graphically represented by nodes (i.e., brain regions) and edges (i.e., connections between two brain regions; Figure 1A) (Biswal et al. 2010; Bullmore and Sporns 2009, 2012; Rubinov and Sporns 2010), allowing FC within regions belonging to one network and FC between regions of different networks to be assessed.
To estimate FC between all predefined 400 brain regions, for each brain region a BOLD mean time series over 300 time points was extracted from the preprocessed fMRI data for each of the 400 brain regions (i.e., nodes). Next, correlations between these 400 BOLD mean times series were calculated using Pearson's product-moment correlation, resulting in a 400x400 (node-to-node) correlation matrix (Biswal et al. 1995). Then, these correlations were converted into z-values using Fisher's equation (Fisher 1915) to estimate three FC metrics.
To estimate the strength of FC within each network, intra-network FC was calculated as the mean z-transformed FC value between all pairs of nodes belonging to the same network (zjntrj (Figure 1A, dotted lines) between nodes of one network). Internetwork FC, which quantifies how strongly a network is connected to all other networks, was calculated as the mean z-transformed FC value from one network to all other networks (zji,ter) (Figure 1A, solid lines). To assess the relation between intra- and internetwork FC in one quantity, we calculated a ratio of intra- and internetwork FC called the segregation index (Chan et al. 2014):compared with its connections to all other networks (low internetwork FC). Negative values express higher brain network integration, with connections within the same network being weaker than connections to all other networks (Figure IB). The maximal value of the segregation index is 1, which displays an isolated network with large connections within and no connections to other networks.
When calculating the mean z-transformed FC value, a distinction between positive and negative correlations must be made, otherwise they cancel out each other. Moreover, interpretation of negative correlations is still unclear (Chai et al. 2012; Fornito et al. 2013; Murphy and Fox 2017). In addition, it is suggested that negative correlations may be strengthened during the preprocessing of functional imaging (Fox et al. 2009; Murphy et al. 2009; Murphy and Fox 2017). Therefore, we only used positive correlations in the main analysis and set negative correlations to zero.
Covariates. Except for age (in years), where age at time of 1000B RAINS examination (2011-2015) was taken, only covariates from the HNR baseline examination (2000-2003) were used in all analyses. Socioeconomic status (SES) and lifestyle variables were assessed by questionnaire. Individual SES was defined based on the International Standard Classification of Education as total years of formal education, combining primary schooling and vocational or professional education (UNESCO 1997) and categorized into four groups (<10, 11-13, 14-17, and >18 years of age). To assess neighborhood SES, unemployment rates (units of percentage) between 2001 and 2003 were provided by local census authorities for each residential neighborhood according to administrative boundaries (median size: 11,263 inhabitants) (Dragano et al. 2009). Smoking status was categorized into current, former (>1 y since quitting), and never smoker. Cumulative smoking exposure (pack-years) was assessed for former and current smokers and accounted for periods of nonsmoking. Environmental tobacco smoke exposure (ETS; Yes/No) reflected regular passive exposure to smoke at home, work, or another location. Physical activity (Yes/No) was defined by regular sporting activities at least once a week for a minimum of 30 min. Alcohol intake was obtained and grouped into five categories (0, 1-3, >3-6, >6-14, >14 drinks per week). A dietary pattern index ranging from 0 to 26, with 26 representing an ideal diet (Winkler and Doring 1998), was divided into quantiles (<10, 11-12, 13-15, >15). Anthropometric measurements (height, weight) were conducted according to standard protocols and body mass index (BMI) was calculated as kilograms per meter squared.
Statistical Analyses
We used multiple linear regression models to evaluate the association between long-term air pollution and noise exposure at baseline (2000-2003) with FC metrics at 1000BRAINS examination (2011-2015) for seven brain networks, using each single exposure as an independent variable and the segregation index for each network as the main outcome. We chose a period of ~ 10 y between exposure and outcome measurement because of the long latency of effects of external exposures on the brain. To improve the understanding of changes in the segregation index, we performed additional regression models with intra- and internetwork FC as dependent variables.
We first conducted an unadjusted model (model 0) for each brain network, where we performed separate crude linear regression models. In model 1, we added to model 0, adjustments for sex and age at time of fMRI scan. Using a directed acyclic graph (DAG; Figure S2) (Textor et al. 2011), we chose the minimal adjustment set, including BMI, smoking status, physical activity, alcohol consumption and diet, which were then included in model
2. In model 3 (main model), we further adjusted model 2 for individual SES and neighborhood SES. In the main model, air pollution exposure models were further adjusted for 24-h outdoor noise whereas noise models were additionally adjusted forM,
5abs- Age and BMI were modeled using restricted cubicsplines with three knots. We examined the residuals for nonnor-mality and nonconstant variance of the error terms for model fit. To reduce the influence of measurement inaccuracies on noise levels at the lower end, we modeled noise as a partially bound continuous variable with a lower cutoff value of 50 dB(A) for Lden and 45 dB(A) for L"ight, with all noise values lower than the defined threshold being set to the threshold value following (NuBbaum et al. 2020). For noise, we estimated model parameters per 10-dB(A) increase in noise level. All air pollution exposures were modeled as continuous variables, and air pollution exposure model parameters were computed per interquartile range (IQR) increase to allow comparable relative increases across exposures.
For comparison, we additionally examined the relationship between age as an independent variable and FC metrics as outcomes for each of the seven networks. The regression models were adjusted for sex, BMI, diet, physical activity, smoking status, cumulative smoking, environmental tobacco smoke exposure, alcohol consumption, and individual and neighborhood SES.
To summarize the results in one graphic, we use an algorithm (see "R-code to Categorize Confidence Limits for Summarization Plot" in the Supplemental Material) that categorized all confidence intervals (CIs) into 10 categories by considering the direction of the effect size and the precision of estimation (width of the CI). For this, the direction of the effect size and the precision of estimation (width of the CI) were considered. First, for each estimate, CIs were divided into 8 equally sized segments. Depending on the location of the zero value in segments, these 8 segments plus 2 categories for confidence limits completely above or below the zero value, formed 10 categories. Then, each category was assigned a color from blue to green displaying possible decrease (CI mostly of <0) to increase (CI mostly of >0). To avoid graphically overestimating effects, confidence limits with effect estimates very close to zero (categories 4-7) were grouped into 1 category (white) (Figure S3).
Sensitivity Analyses
For the noise models, we performed sensitivity analyses using different noise threshold values (45 dB(A) for Lden and 35 dB (A) for L"ight). To account for possible exposure misclassifica-tion in noise (e.g., due to house structure, ventilation habits), we also evaluated indoor noise measures with threshold values for indoor Lden and L"ight of 35dB(A) and 10dB(A) following Ohlwein et al. (2019). Because ~ 10% of values in indoor noise levels were missing, these analyses were performed in a smaller sample of 522 participants. In another sensitivity analysis, we used inverse probability weighting (IPW) to account for selection bias introduced by the recruitment of participants for MRI toward a more male, younger, and better-educated study population compared with the general population. Given that distributional assumptions have to be made for IPW with continuous exposures, we followed recent recommendations and applied a distribution-free method of quantile binning, with 20 distinct bins, using the SAS code from Naimi et al. (2014). Because we hypothesized that potential environmental effects on FC have a long latency period, we used exposure data from the baseline examination (2000-2003) in the main analysis. In a sensitivity analysis, we used exposure data from the 5-y follow-up HNR examination (2006-2008) instead. Because of the yet ambiguous meaning of negative correlations (Chai et al. 2012; Fornitoet al. 2013; Fox et al. 2009; Murphy et al. 2009; Murphy and Fox 2017), the main analysis included only positive correlations. Statistical analyses were performed using R (version 3.6.2; R Development Core Team).
Results
Study Population
Of 685 participants of the 1000B RAINS examination (2011-2015) with exposure assessment (2000-2003), fMRI data were available for 592 (Figure S4). Of these, 18 participants were excluded owing to incomplete exposure data (n = 2) or covariate data (n = 16), resulting in a final study population of 574 participants (44.1% female) of middle age at HNR baseline examination (mean = 56.1 y) and ~ 10 y older at 1000BRAINS examination (mean = 67.4 y) (Table 1).
Participants excluded from the analysis were more likely nonsmokers, slightly less highly educated, and were less physically active compared with included participants (Table SI). Compared with HNR participants who did not participate in the 1000B RAINS study, HNR participants who participated in the 1000B RAINS study were on average younger, more likely male, drank more alcohol, and more highly educated (Table SI).
Mean concentrations of PM2.5, PM10, and NO2 were 18.3, 27.5, and 29.5 ug/m3 (Table 2). The IQRs for PM2.5, PM10, and NO2 were 1.4, 2.0, and 5.2 ug/m3. Mean outdoor noise levels were 53.0 dB(A) for 24-h values and 44.1 dB(A) for nighttime values. Some air pollution and noise exposures had a right-skewed distribution and showed a small correlation (Spearman correlation coefficient = 0.18-0.39) (Table 3).
On average, FC within networks was comparatively larger (higher intra-network FC) than between networks (lowerinternetwork FC) for all networks (Table 4), resulting in a segregation index ranging from -0.17 to 0.93. Across all networks, the mean segregation index took on positive values, reflecting lager connections within networks (higher intra-network FC) compared with smaller connections between networks (lower internetwork FC). We observed the highest mean segregation (0.60) in the visual network, and the lowest mean segregation (0.32) in the frontoparietal network. Although mean internetwork FC was quite constant across networks (ranging from 0.23 to 0.27), mean intra-network FC showed more variation across networks. For instance, regions belonging to the visual network showed large intra-network FC (mean = 0.62), whereas regions of the frontoparietal network showed smaller functional connections (mean = 0.37).
Air Pollution and FC
Only results from the main model (model 3) are described in detail here given that the results for the other models do not differ considerably (Figure S5; Table S2). For several pollutants, a tendency toward a small association of higher air pollution with less segregated networks (a lower segregation index) was observed, with estimates ranging from -0.018 (95% CI: -0.037, 0.001) for a 613 ra/mL increase in PNam in the ventral attention network to 0.013 (95% CI: -0.011, 0.036) per 1.4-ug/m3 increase in PM2.5 in the visual network (Figures 2 and 3; Table S2). In comparison, changes in the segregation index seen for 1 y of additional age with estimates ranged from -0.004 (95% CI: -0.006, -0.002) to -0.001 (95% CI: -0.003, 0.002) (Table S3). In particular, for an increase of 0.3 X 10~5/m in PM2.5 absorbance, the segregation index decreased by 0.006 (95% CI: -0.021, 0.009) in the sensorimotor network. In comparison, a 1-y increase in age showed a decreasing segregation index by 0.004 (95% CI: -0.006, -0.002) in the same network. Thus, for PM2.5 absorbance we observed an effect size comparable to a 1-y increase in age in the sensorimotor network (Table S3).
A pattern of small, but overall positive, associations of higher air pollution with comparatively larger connections between networks (higher internetwork FC) was most noticeable for soot (PM2.5abs) [e.g., 0.004 (95% CI: -0.004, 0.012) per 0.3 X 10-5/m PM2.5 absorbance in the ventral attention network] and distance of <100 m to the nearest high-traffic roads [e.g., 0.031 (95% CI: -0.003, 0.065) in the visual network] (Figures 3 and 4; Table S2). On the other hand, no consistent pattern of larger or smallerconnections within networks (intra-network FC) was detectable (Figures 3 and 4; Table S2). Although we saw some possible associations between higher pollutants and higher intra-network FC, smaller connections within networks, especially in the ventral attention [e.g., -0.012 (95% CI: -0.026, 0.001) per 2.0 ug/m3 PM10] and sensorimotor network [e.g., -0.009 (95% CI: -0.026, 0.008) per 2.0-ug/m PM10], were also observable. For the default network, only small associations between distance to the nearest high-traffic roads and decreased segregation [e.g., -0.051 (95% CI: -0.127, 0.025) for <100-m vs. >200-m distance to high-traffic roads] were observed, with no clear pattern of increase or decrease in intra- or internetwork FC.
Traffic Noise and FC
For outdoor noise exposures, we observed small associations between high noise exposure and low network segregation (Figures 3 and 4; Table S2). These were most noticeable in the frontoparietal and visual network [-0.020 (95% CI: -0.037, -0.003) and -0.028 (95% CI: -0.049, -0.006) per 10-dB(A) increase in L,ien, respectively] and slightly smaller in the defaultand sensorimotor network [-0.013 (95% CI: -0.033, 0.006) and -0.014 (95% CI: -0.032, 0.004) per 10-dB(A) increase in Lden, respectively].
Decreased intra-network FC [-0.029 (95% CI: -0.057, -0.002) per 10-dB(A) increase in Lden in the visual network] and increased internetwork FC [e.g., 0.005 (95% CI: -0.004, 0.014) per 10-dB(A) increase in Lden in the dorsal attention network] were observable for higher noise levels (Figures 3 and 4; Table S2).
Sensitivity Analysis
Our results were robust to different noise thresholds [45 dB(A) for Lden and 35 dB(A) for Lnight (Figure S6; Table S4)]. In line with the main analysis, higher indoor and outdoor noise exposures were associated with smaller segregation in the frontoparietal and visual network in the subsample of 522 participants with indoor noise exposure values (Figure S7; Table S5). Moreover, across all noise variables, smaller connections within regions of the visual network were observable for high outdoor and indoor noise levels. In addition, high indoor noise values showed small associations with more pronounced network segregation of the dorsal attention and possibly limbic networks, whereas this was not apparent for outdoor noise values. No associations between outdoor or indoor noise levels and internetwork FC were visible compared with the main model.
Using IPW, we observed a more pronounced pattern of less network segregation for high particle (PMio, PM2.5, PM2.sabs, PNam), and NO2 concentration than in the main analysis (Figure S8; Table S6). For intra-network FC, we continued to see no clear pattern of increase or decrease, but for high particle concentrations and high noise levels, we observed smaller connections within regions belonging to the ventral attention network and the visual network, respectively.
In another sensitivity analysis using exposure data from 2006 to 2008 (5 y before the fMRI scans), we observed patterns similar to those seen in the main analysis, with associations between higherexposure and mostly higher segregation and lower internetwork FC (Figure S9; Table S7). For intra-network FC, a mixture of higher and lower intra-network FC with high exposure was seen.
Discussion
The present study suggests that long-term exposure to ambient air pollution and road traffic noise is associated with altered global intrinsic functional organization of the brain. Our findings appear to support the hypothesis that exposure to long-term ambient air pollution and road traffic noise may lead to less segregated and more integrated brain networks. Therefore, high air pollution and noise levels might favor an age-like change in functional brain organization.
Functional Organization and Brain Aging
The healthy brain is intrinsically organized into functionally distinct and specialized brain networks that process different types of information (Wig 2017). The resulting structure of segregated networks for specialized functions (e.g., fast processing visual input) alongside integrated networks for higher cognitive functions (e.g., memory function) is characteristic of functional brain organization. The degree of network segregation is used to assess functional network organization (Chan et al. 2014). Neuroimaging studies in adults have reported decreasing segregation with increasing age in various networks (Chan et al. 2014; Iordan et al. 2018; Malagurski et al. 2020; Stumme et al. 2020). These changes were further linked to decreased cognitive function in adults (Chan et al. 2014; Cohen and D'Esposito 2016; Damoiseaux 2017; Marques et al. 2016; Stumme et al. 2020; Wig 2017). In particular, evidence indicates that lower functional segregation (a more integrated network structure) is linked to worse episodic memory (Chan et al. 2014), declines in processing speed (Ng et al. 2016), and slower learning rates (Iordan et al. 2018).
Two aging theories try to link the altered functional brain organization to cognition, and it is suggested that they are notmutually exclusive and may even influence each other (Gazzaley and D'Esposito 2003; Stumme et al. 2020). The compensatory theory suggests that connections between networks increase with age to compensate for age-related decreased FC within networks to maintain cognitive performance (Gazzaley and D'Esposito 2003). The dedifferentiation theory states that a reduced specification of networks leads to increased neuronal noise, reduced processing speed, and increased energy consumption (Gazzaley and D'Esposito 2003). Eventually, this leads to cognitive decline (Geerligs et al. 2015). It is proposed that the reduced specification of networks results from of a series of age-related neuro-biological processes, beginning with the reduced concentrations of neurotransmitters (e.g., dopamine), with lower neuronal responsiveness and increased neuronal noise (Li et al. 2001).
To maintain performance, brain regions are activated in a nonspecific (random) fashion, which eventually results in diffuse patterns of functional connections (Fornito et al. 2015). Although cognitive ability is maintained at first, the progressive diffusion of networks leads to less distinct activation patterns of different stimuli, such as visual perception (Bennett et al. 2007; Goh et al. 2010; Park et al. 2004) or motor action (Carp et al. 2011), and may subsequently result in impaired cognitive function (Koen and Rugg 2019).
Air Pollution
To date, studies investigating the impact of air pollution on FC are scarce. Only one study examined the impact of indoor airpollution on cognitive function and brain health in older adults (Wong 2020). They reported associations between regular incense burning with poorer cognitive performance. Although no structural brain alterations were visible for regular incense burning, decreased FC within regions of the default network were observed. The insufficient suppression of the default network has been linked to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Mowinckel et al. 2017), autism (Padmanabhan et al. 2017), and cognitive impairment (Lee et al. 2016). Therefore, Wong et al. (2020) suggested that indoor incense burning alters FC, possibly leading to increased vulnerability to future cognitive decline.
Other studies on air pollution and FC have been conducted only in children. In a cohort of children (8-12 years of age), Pujol et al. (2016) found older age was associated with higher functional connection within the default network along with smaller connections to other networks. In contrast, they observed smaller connections within the default network in children with higher exposure to air pollution (elemental carbon and NO2). Because with increasing age, stronger connections are built between functionally related brain regions in normal brain maturation, they concluded that exposure to higher air pollution may decelerate brain maturation. Similarly, in a pilot study, de Water et al. (2018) found that prenatal exposure to manganese, which is neurotoxic at high levels, was associated with reduced FC within brain regions involved in emotions and regulation processing in childhood (6-7 years of age). In addition, in a group of 14 adolescents (12-18 years of age) they observed associations between early postnatal exposure to manganese and decreased functional connections within brain regions for motor function (de Water et al. 2019). Thus, research indicates that children's functional organization may be altered by environmental exposures, possibly slowing brain maturation. To our knowledge, there have been no prior studies examining the association of outdoor air pollution and FC in adults. Therefore, it is unclear how adult FC may be affected by outdoor air pollution.
In line with several neurological studies on healthy aging and functional organization in adults, overall patterns of slightly lower network segregation with higher air pollution exposure were visible in the present study. This suggests that high air pollution might be associated with altered functional network organization (less segregated and more integrated network structure). Overall, the less segregated and more integrated network structure seems to be related to increasing connectivity between networks rather than decreasing connectivity within networks. In line with de Water et al. (2019), we observed a tendency toward smaller connections with higher air pollution exposure within regions of the sensorimotor network, which is responsible for motor function, and the ventral attention network, which is associated with detecting unexpected stimuli and shifting attention. For other networks, no clear pattern was detectable for intra-network FC. Contrary to the results of other studies, we did not find a general decrease in FC between regions of the default network. We did, however, observe reduced segregation in the default network. Whether this comes from reduced FC within the network or from stronger functional connections to other networks is not clear. Our results were even more pronounced using IPWs, indicating that effects may be stronger in a more general population than those observed in our study population. Moreover, our findings are consistent with previous neurological research on age-related changes in FC and correspond to the dedifferentia-tion theory (loss of functional specification) in healthy aging (Goh2011).
Traffic Noise
Up to now, the association between noise and functional brain organization has rarely been studied. In a small experimental studywith 30 participants, Zou et al. (2019) reported that short-term noise levels were associated with a less segregated and more integrated brain network structure. A recent study investigated the impact of long-term aircraft noise exposure on various brain outcomes in fighter jet pilots using a matched study design (Cheng et al. 2019). They reported impaired working memory function and neuronal loss, as well as reduced neuronal activity in the hippocampus and other brain regions important for cognition, under exposure to chronic noise and stress.
Similar to our findings for air pollutants, we observed patterns of less segregated and more integrated brain networks with increasing long-term outdoor noise exposure, especially for the visual and frontoparietal network. Although these changes seem to result from lower intra-network FC in the visual network, higher internetwork FC seems to drive the lower segregation in the frontoparietal network.
The associations with outdoor noise seemed to indicate that increasing integration arises from increased connections between distinct networks. Similar to the dedifferentiation theory, higher noise levels were associated with more integrated networks. Although these results were also present for indoor noise values, we additionally observed lager network segregation for the dorsal attention network (associated with orientation of attention) and the limbic network (associated with emotion regulation and social interaction). This may be a result of lager connections between regions within these networks. However, the reduced sample size may have influenced the results. In addition, indoor noise values were calculated based on questionnaire data but were not validated, and thus they might be biased. Nevertheless, these results show that noise may affect the intrinsic organization of the brain and be a vital piece in unraveling how functional organization may be affected by external factors.
Potential Biological Mechanisms
The biological mechanisms through which air pollution is associated with neurocognitive impairments are still not fully understood. It is assumed that particles reach the brain through inhalation either indirectly from the lungs into the bloodstream and crossing the blood-brain barrier or directly from the nose through the olfactory nerve (Geiser et al. 2005; Oberdorster et al. 2004). Accumulated particles can then cause chronic brain inflammation, oxidative stress and microglia activation, which are linked to cognitive impairment and neurodegeneration (Block et al. 2012). With experimental studies showing that microglia activation amplifies dopaminergic neurotoxicity (Block et al. 2004), it is hypothesized that neurotransmitter systems are weakened and that synaptic plasticity, important for learning processes or recovery from brain lesions, is reduced (Allen et al. 2017). At this level, long-term air pollution could adversely affect FC and lead to reorganized functional connections between brain regions, as seen in the age-related dedifferentiation process. These functional changes could, in turn, due to neuronal death, lead to structural changes as reduced brain volume and lead to impaired cognitive function.
Chronic noise is assumed to affect the brain through different, but possibly interrelated, biological mechanisms as air pollution. Noise leads to increased annoyance and stress, thereby arousing the autonomous nervous system/hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA-axis) (Jafari et al. 2019). Animal studies suggest that chronic activation of the HPA-axis may lead to reduced neurogenesis (production of neurons) and altered synaptic plasticity, followed by functional and structural changes (Cheng et al. 2019). However, it remains to be investigated precisely how noise can affect brain functionality.
Strengths and Limitations
Although the size of the study is relatively large compared with other MRI studies, the power to detect small environmental effects might not be sufficient, and single statistically significant results can occur by chance only. Therefore, further studies with larger sample sizes should follow. Moreover, potential selection bias occurred with younger, better educated, and healthier study participants compared with the general population. Although we performed IPW analysis to compensate for this, residual selection bias could still occur. Because we used functional scans from one time point, we were unable to establish the exposure-related change in FC over time. Although having a relatively small exposure variability compared with other, specifically national studies, this study is based on a contiguous study area that is relatively homogeneous in terms of primary and secondary pollutant sources and living conditions. Thus, there is less random and systematic variation compared with studies covering different parts of a country. Exposure contrast is primarily based on small-scale intra-urban differences, which have been shown to be more important for health effects than between-area variability in many studies (Zemp 1999; Miller 2007; Atkinson 2013).
To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the association of long-term air pollution and chronic traffic noise with functional brain organization in older adults. Later in life, the brain is very vulnerable, as progressive neurocognitive degeneration starts (Park and Reuter-Lorenz 2009; Wig 2017) and compensatory mechanisms begin to falter (Grossman 2014). Given the increasing life expectancy, it is important to understand how cognitive function can be preserved (Park and Reuter-Lorenz 2009). Furthermore, air pollution and noise are worldwide issues, and with globally increasing life expectancy, it is of high importance to understand how environmental exposures affect the brain. Because of the age range of the 1000BRAINS study (55-85 y), we were able to investigate effects during this vulnerable period. Moreover, only a limited number of studies on functional brain organization and noise exposure exist to date. Our study contributes new insights into how long-term noise exposures may influence brain function.
Conclusions
Overall, high exposure to air pollution and noise was associated with less segregated functional brain networks. To our knowledge, this is the first study examining associations between exposure to long-term air pollution and chronic traffic noise and FC in older adults. Although we observed only small associations, for high air pollution and noise exposure, we noticed possible trends toward more diffuse patterns of FC, which are also seen in normal brain aging. With continuously increasing global life expectancy, maintaining cognitive function is essential for good quality of life and independence in older age, for individuals and society. As such, it is an important public health issue to understand how modifiable external factors affect the brain and cognition. In this study, we observed associations comparable to a 1-y increase in age. Thus, long-term exposure to air pollution and noise might favor age-like changes in FC, possible mediating adverse effects on cognition. Further studies are needed to understand the complex way in which air pollution and noise may affect the brain.
Acknowledgments
We thank the North Rhine-Westphalia State Agency for Nature, Environment and Consumer Protection for providing emission and land use data for North Rhine-Westphalia. We are indebted to the investigative group and the study personnel of the Heinz Nixdorf
Recall (HNR) study and the 1000BRAINS study. We also thank the Heinz Nixdorf Foundation for the generous support of this study. The HNR study was supported by grants from the German Research Council (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft; grants ER 155/6-1, ER 155/6-2, SI 236/8-1, and SI 236/9-1) and the Kulturstiftung Essen, Germany. The exposure assessment was funded through the European Community's Seventh Framework Program (FP7/2007-2011; grant 211250) and by the German Research Council (HO 3314/4-3). Further, this project was partially funded by the 1000B RAINS Study of the Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, Research Centre Jiilich, Germany, and has received funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme under grant agreement no. 785907 (HBP SGA2; S.C.) as well as from the Initiative and Networking Fund of the Helmholtz Association (S.C).
References Achard S, Bullmore E. 2007. Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks. PLoS Comput Biol 3(2):e17, PMID: 17274684, https://doi.org/10.1371/ journal.pcbi.0030017. Allen JL, Klocke C, Morris-Schaffer K, Conrad K, Sobolewski M, Cory-Slechta DA. 2017. Cognitive effects of air pollution exposures and potential mechanistic underpinnings. Curr Environ Health Rep 4(2):180 191, PMID: 28435996, https://doi.org/10.1007/s40572-017-0134-3.
Armstrong NM, An Y, Shin JJ, Williams 0A, Doshi J, Erus G, et al. 2020. Associations between cognitive and brain volume changes in cognitively normal older adults. Neurolmage 223:117289, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117289. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. 2005. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage 26(3):839-851, PMID: 15955494, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018.
Atkinson RW, Carey IM, Kent AJ, van Staa TP, Anderson HR, Cook DG. 2013. Long-term exposure to outdoor air pollution and incidence of cardiovascular diseases. Epidemiology 24(1):44-53, PMID: 23222514, https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE. 0b013e318276ccb8.
Basner M, Babisch W, Davis A, Brink M, Clark C, Janssen S, et al. 2014. Auditory and non-auditory effects of noise on health. Lancet 383(9925):1325-1332, PMID: 24183105, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61613-X.
Bassett DS, Bullmore E. 2006. Small-world brain networks. Neuroscientist 12(6):512-523, PMID: 17079517, https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858406293182. Beelen R, Hoek G, Fischer P, van den Brandt PA, Brunekreef B. 2007. Estimated long-term outdoor air pollution concentrations in a cohort study. Atmos Environ 41 (7):1343-1358, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.atmosenv.2006.10.020.
Beelen R, Hoek G, Vienneau D, Eeftens M, Dimakopoulou K, Pedeli X, et al. 2013. Development of N02 and N0x land use regression models for estimating air pollution exposure in 36 study areas in Europe-the ESCAPE project. Atmos Environ 72:10-23, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.atmosenv.2013.02.037. Bennett PJ, Sekuler R, Sekuler AB. 2007. The effects of aging on motion detection and direction identification. Vision Res 47(6):799-809, PMID: 17289106, https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.visres.2007.01.001.
Betzel RF, Byrge L, He Y, Gohi J, Zuo XN, Sporns 0. 2014. Changes in structural and functional connectivity among resting-state networks across the human lifespan. Neuroimage 102(pt 2):345-357, PMID: 25109530, https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.067. Biswal BB, Mennes M, Zuo XN, Gohel S, Kelly C, Smith SM, et al. 2010. Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107(10):4734-4739, PMID: 20176931, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0911855107.
Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. 1995. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 34(4):537-541, PMID: 8524021, https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910340409. Block ML, Elder A, Auten RL, Bilbo SD, Chen H, Chen JC, et al. 2012. The outdoor air pollution and brain health workshop. Neurotoxicology 33(5):972-984, PMID: 22981845, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuro.2012.08.014.
Block ML, Wu X, Pei Z, Li G, Wang T, Qin L, et al. 2004. Nanometer size diesel exhaust particles are selectively toxic to dopaminergic neurons: the role of microglia, phagocytosis, and NADPH oxidase. FASEB J 18(13):1618-1620, PMID: 15319363, https://doi.org/10.1096/fj.04-1945fje. Bullmore E, Sporns 0. 2009. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(3):186 198, PMID: 19190637, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2575.
Bullmore E, Sporns 0. 2012. The economy of brain network organization. Nat Rev Neurosci 13(5):336-349, PMID: 22498897, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3214. Buns C, Klemm 0, Wurzler S, Hebbinghaus H, Steckelbach I, Friesel J, et al. 2012. Comparison of four years of air pollution data with a mesoscale model. Atmos Res 118:404-417, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.atmosres.2012.07.009.
Burgess GC, Kandala S, Nolan D, Laumann TO, Power JD, Adeyemo B, et al. 2016. Evaluation of denoising strategies to address motion-correlated artifacts in resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data from the Human Connectome Project. Brain Connect 6(9):669-680, PMID: 27571276, httnc7/Hni nrn/10 mSQ/hrain 901R nd^R
II LLpb.//UUI.UI y/ 1 U. 1 UOD/UI dlll.ZU 1 D.U40D. Calhoun VD, Wager TD, Krishnan A, Rosch KS, Seymour KE, Nebel MB, et al. 2017. The impact of T1 versus EPI spatial normalization templates for fMRI data analyses. Hum Brain Mapp38(11):5331-5342, PMID: 28745021, https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23737.
Carp J, Park J, HebrankA, Park DC, Polk TA. 2011. Age-related neural dedifferentia-tion in the motor system. PLoS One 6(12):e29411, PMID: 22216274, https://doi.org/ 10.1371/journal.pone.0029411. Casanova R, Wang X, Reyes J, Akita Y, Serre ML, Vizuete W, et al. 2016. A voxel-based morphometry study reveals local brain structural alterations associated with ambient fine particles in older women. Front Hum Neurosci 10:495, PMID: 27790103, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00495.
Caspers S, Moebus S, Lux S, Pundt N, Schiitz H, Miihleisen TW, et al. 2014. Studying variability in human brain aging in a population-based German cohort-rationale and design of 1000 BRAINS. Front Aging Neurosci 6:149, PMID: 25071558, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00149. Chai XJ, Castahon AN, Ongiir D, Whitfield-Gabrieli S. 2012. Anticorrelations in resting state networks without global signal regression. Neuroimage 59(2):1420-1428, PMID: 21889994, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.048.
Chan MY, Park DC, Savalia NK, Petersen SE, Wig GS. 2014. Decreased segregation of brain systems across the healthy adult lifespan. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111(46):E4997-E5006, PMID: 25368199, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1415122111. Chen JC, Wang X, Wellenius GA, Serre ML, Driscoll I, Casanova R, et al. 2015. Ambient air pollution and neurotoxicity on brain structure: evidence from Women's Health Initiative Memory Study. Ann Neurol 78(3):466-476, PMID: 26075655, https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24460.
Cheng H, Sun G, Li M, Yin M, Chen H. 2019. Neuron loss and dysfunctionality in hippocampus explain aircraft noise induced working memory impairment: a resting-state fMRI study on military pilots. Biosci Trends 13(5):430-440, PMID: 31611544, https://doi.Org/10.5582/bst.2019.01190. Ciric R, Wolf DH, Power JD, Roalf DR, Baum GL, Ruparel K, et al. 2017. Benchmarking of participant-level confound regression strategies for the control of motion artifact in studies of functional connectivity. Neuroimage 154:174-187, PMID: 28302591, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.020.
Clifford A, Lang L, Chen R,AnsteyKJ, Seaton A. 2016. Exposure to air pollution and cognitive functioning across the life course-a systematic literature review. Environ Res 147:383-398, PMID: 26945620, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.envres.2016.01.018. Cohen JR, D'Esposito M. 2016. The segregation and integration of distinct brain networks and their relationship to cognition. J Neurosci 36(48): 12083-12094, PMID: 27903719, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2965-15.2016.
Damoiseaux JS. 2017. Effects of aging on functional and structural brain connectivity. Neuroimage 160:32-40, PMID: 28159687, https://doi.0rg/IO.IOI6/j.neuroimage. 2017.01.077. Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SARB, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, et al. 2006. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103(37):13848-13853, PMID: 16945915, https://doi.org/10.1073/ pnas.0601417103.
de Water E, Papazaharias DM, Ambrosi C, Mascara L, lannilli E, Gasparotti R, et al. 2019. Early-life dentine manganese concentrations and intrinsic functional brain connectivity in adolescents: a pilot study. PLoS One 14(8):e0220790, PMID: 31412061, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220790. de Water E, Proal E, Wang V, Medina SM, Schnaas L, Tellez-Rojo MM, et al. 2018. Prenatal manganese exposure and intrinsic functional connectivity of emotional brain areas in children. Neurotoxicology 64:85-93, PMID: 28610744, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuro.2017.06.006.
Dohmatob E, Varoquaux G, Thirion B. 2018. Inter-subject registration of functional images: do we need anatomical images? Front Neurosci 12:64, PMID: 29497357, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00064. Dragano N, Hoffmann B, Moebus S, Mohlenkamp S, Stang A, Verde PE, et al. 2009. Traffic exposure and subclinical cardiovascular disease: is the association modified by socioeconomic characteristics of individuals and neighbourhoods? Results from a multilevel study in an urban region. Occup Environ Med 66(9):628-635, PMID: 19293166, https://doi.org/10.1136/oem.2008.044032. Eeftens M, Beelen R, de Hoogh K, Bellander T, Cesaroni G, Cirach M, et al. 2012. Development of land use regression models for PM2.5, PM2.5 absorbance, PM10 and PMcoarsi, in 20 European study areas; results of the ESCAPE project. Environ Sci Technol 46(20):11195-11205, PMID: 22963366, https://doi.org/10.1021/es301948k.
European Commission. Directive 2002/49/EC of the European Parliament and of the Council of 25 June 2002 relating to the assessment and management of environmental noise. In: Communities OJotE. 2002. Fisher RA. 1915. Frequency distribution of the values of the correlation coefficient in samples from an indefinitely large population. Biometrika 10(4):507-521, https://doi.org/10.2307/2331838.
Foraster M, Kiinzli N, Aguilera I, Rivera M, Agis D, Vila J, et al. 2014. High blood pressure and long-term exposure to indoor noise and air pollution from road traffic. Environ Health Perspect 122(11): 1193-1200, PMID: 25003348, https://doi.org/10. 1289/ehp.1307156.
Fornito A, Zalesky A, Breakspear M. 2013. Graph analysis of the human connectome: promise, progress, and pitfalls. Neuroimage 80:426-444, PMID: 23643999, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.087. Fornito A, Zalesky A, Breakspear M. 2015. The connectomics of brain disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci 16(3):159-172, PMID: 25697159, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3901.
Fougere B, Vellas B, Billet S, Martin PJ, Gallucci M, Cesari M. 2015. Air pollution modifies the association between successful and pathological aging throughout the frailty condition. Ageing Res Rev 24(pt B):299-303, PMID: 26462883, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.arr.2015.09.004.
Fox MD, Raichle ME. 2007. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 8(9):700-711, PMID: 17704812, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2201. Fox MD, Zhang D, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME. 2009. The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. J Neurophysiol 101(6):3270-3283, PMID: 19339462, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90777.2008.
Friston KJ, Ashburner JT, Kiebel SJ, Nichols TE, Penny WD. 2007. Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images. Burlington, MA: Academic Press. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Fletcher P, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RSJ. 1996. Functional topography: multidimensional scaling and functional connectivity in the brain. Cereb Cortex 6(2):156-164, PMID: 8670646, https://doi.Org/10.1093/cercor/6.2.156.
Fuks KB, Wigmann C, Altug H, Schikowski T. 2019. Road traffic noise at the residence, annoyance, and cognitive function in elderly women. Int J Environ Res Public Health 16(10):1790, PMID: 31137595, https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16101790. GazzaleyA, D'Esposito M. 2003. The contribution of functional brain imaging to our understanding of cognitive aging. Sci Aging Knowledge Environ 2003(4):PE2, PMID: 12844554, https://doi.Org/10.1126/sageke.2003.4.pe2.
Geerligs L, Renken RJ, Saliasi E, Maurits NM, Lorist MM. 2015. A brain-wide study of age-related changes in functional connectivity. Cereb Cortex 25(7):1987-1999, PMID: 24532319, https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu012. Geiser M, Rothen-Rutishauser B, Kapp N, Schiirch S, Kreyling W, Schulz H, et al. 2005. Ultrafine particles cross cellular membranes by nonphagocytic mechanisms in lungs and in cultured cells. Environ Health Perspect 113(11 ):1555- 1560, PMID: 16263511, https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.8006. Goh JOS. 2011. Functional dedifferentiation and altered connectivity in older adults: neural accounts of cognitive aging. Aging Dis 2(1):30-48, PMID: 21461180, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.107.
Goh JO, Suzuki A, Park DC. 2010. Reduced neural selectivity increases fMRI adaptation with age during face discrimination. Neuroimage 51(1):336-344, PMID: 20139012, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.107. Grayson DS, Fair DA. 2017. Development of large-scale functional networks from birth to adulthood: a guide to the neuroimaging literature. Neuroimage 160:15-31, PMID: 28161313, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.079. Grossman E. 2014. Time after time: environmental influences on the aging brain. Environ Health Perspect 122(9):A238-A243, PMID: 25181706, https://doi.org/10. 1289/ehp/122-A238.
Hass H, Ebel A, Feldmann H, Jakobs HJ, Memmesheimer M. 1993. Evaluation studies with a regional chemical-transport model (EURAD) using air-quality data from the EMEP monitoring network. Atmos Environ A Gen Topics 27(6):867-887, https://doi.Org/10.1016/0960-1686(93)90007-L Holmes CJ, Hoge R, Collins L, Woods R, Toga AW, Evans AC. 1998. Enhancement of MR images using registration for signal averaging. J Comput Assist Tomogr 22(2):324-333, PMID: 9530404, https://doi.org/10.1097/00004728-199803000-00032.
lordan AD, Cooke KA, Moored KD, Katz B, Buschkuehl M, Jaeggi SM, et al. 2018. Aging and network properties: stability over time and links with learning during working memory training. Front Aging Neurosci 9:419, PMID: 29354048, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00419. Jafari MJ, Khosrowabadi R, Khodakarim S, Mohammadian F. 2019. The effect of noise exposure on cognitive performance and brain activity patterns. Open Access Maced J Med Sci 7(17):2924-2931, PMID: 31844459. Jung CR, Lin YT, Hwang BF. 2015. Ozone, particulate matter, and newly diagnosed Alzheimer's disease: a population-based cohort study in Taiwan. J Alzheimers Dis 44(2):573-584, PMID: 25310992, https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-140855.
Koen JD, Rugg MD. 2019. Neural dedifferentiation in the aging brain. Trends Cogn Sci 23(7):547-559, PMID: 31174975, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.04. 012. Lee ES, Yoo K, Lee YB, Chung J, Lim JE, Yoon B, et al. 2016. Default mode network functional connectivity in early and late mild cognitive impairment: results From the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 30(4):289-296, PMID: 26840545, https://doi.org/10.1097/WAD. 0000000000000143.
Li SC, Lindenberger U, Sikstrom S. 2001. Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation. Trends Cogn Sci 5(11 ):479-486, PMID: 11684480, https://doi.org/ 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01769-1.
Malagurski B, Liem F, Oschwald J, MerillatS, Jancke L 2020. Functional dedifferenti-ation of associative resting state networks in older adults-a longitudinal study. Neuroimage 214:116680, PMID: 32105885, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage. 2020.116680.
Margulies DS, Kelly AMC, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. 2007. Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroimage 37(2):579-588, PMID: 17604651, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuro image.2007.05.019. Marques P, Moreira P, Magalhaes R, Costa P, Santos N, Zihl J, et al. 2016. The functional connectome of cognitive reserve. Hum Brain Mapp 37(9):3310-3322, PMID: 27144904, https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23242.
Memmesheimer M, Friese E, Ebel A, Jakobs HJ, Feldmann H, Kessler C, et al. 2004. Long-term simulations of particulate matter in Europe on different scales using sequential nesting of a regional model. Int J Environ Pollut 22(1 2): 108 132, https://doi.org/10.1504/IJEP.2004.005530. Miller KA, Siscovick DS, Sheppard L, Shepherd K, Sullivan JH, Anderson GL, et al. 2007. Long-term exposure to air pollution and incidence of cardiovascular events in women. N Engl J Med 356(5):447-458, PMID: 17267905, https://doi.org/ 10.1056/NEJMoa054409.
Mowinckel AM, Alnass D, Pedersen ML, Ziegler S, Fredriksen M, Kaufmann T, et al. 2017. Increased default-mode variability is related to reduced task-performance and is evident in adults with ADHD. Neuroimage Clin 16:369-382, PMID: 28861338, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.nicl.2017.03.008. Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker DA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA. 2009. The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? Neuroimage 44(3):893-905, PMID: 18976716, https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036.
Murphy K, Fox MD. 2017. Towards a consensus regarding global signal regression for resting state functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage 154:169-173, PMID: 27888059, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.052. Naimi Al, Moodie EEM, Auger N, Kaufman JS. 2014. Constructing inverse probability weights for continuous exposures: a comparison of methods. Epidemiology 25(2):292-299, PMID: 24487212, https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE. 0000000000000053. Nelson EA, Dannefer D. 1992. Aged heterogeneity: fact or fiction? The fate of diversity in gerontological research. Gerontologist 32(1):17-23, PMID: 1740251, https://doi.Org/10.1093/geront/32.1.17.
Ng KK, Lo JC, Lim JKW, Chee MWL, Zhou J. 2016. Reduced functional segregation between the default mode network and the executive control network in healthy older adults: a longitudinal study. Neuroimage 133:321-330, PMID: 27001500, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.029.
NuKbaum R, Lucht S, Jockwitz C, Moebus S, Engel M, Jockel KH, et al. 2020. Associations of air pollution and noise with local brain structure in a cohort of older adults. Environ Health Perspect 128(6):067012, PMID: 32539589, https://doi.org/10.1289/EHP5859. Oberdorster G, Sharp Z, Atudorei V, Elder A, Gelein R, Kreyling W, et al. 2004. Translocation of inhaled ultrafine particles to the brain. Inhal Toxicol 16(6-7):437^45, PMID: 15204759, https://doi.org/10.1080/08958370490439597.
Ohlwein S, Hennig F, Lucht S, Matthiessen C, Pundt N, Moebus S, et al. 2019. Indoor and outdoor road traffic noise and incident diabetes mellitus: results from a longitudinal German cohort study. Environ Epidemiol 3(1):e037, PMID: 33778334, https://doi.Org/10.1097/EE9.0000000000000037. Padmanabhan A, Lynch CJ, Schaer M, Menon V. 2017. The default mode network in autism. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 2(6):476-486, PMID: 29034353, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.04.004.
Park DC, PolkTA, Park R, Minear M, Savage A, Smith MR. 2004. Aging reduces neural specialization in ventral visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101(35):13091-13095, PMID: 15322270, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.040514 8101. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. 2009. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol 60:173-196, PMID: 19035823, https://doi.org/10.1146/ annurev.psych.59.103006.093656.
Peters R, Ee N, Peters J, Booth A, Mudway I, Anstey KJ. 2019. Air pollution and dementia: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis 70(suppl 1):S145-S163, PMID: 30775976, https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-180631. Power MC, Weisskopf MG, Alexeeff SE, Coull BA, Spiro A III, Schwartz J. 2011. Traffic-related air pollution and cognitive function in a cohort of older men. Environ Health Perspect 119(5):682-687, PMID: 21172758, https://doi.org/10. 1289/ehp.1002767.
Pruim RHR, Mennes M, van Rooij D, Llera A, Buitelaar JK, Beckmann CF. 2015. ICA-AROMA: a robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data. Neuroimage 112:267-277, PMID: 25770991, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j. neuroimage.2015.02.064.
Pujol J, Martinez-Vilavella G, Macia D, Fenoll R, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, Rivas I, et al. 2016. Traffic pollution exposure is associated with altered brain connectivity in school children. Neuroimage 129:175-184, PMID: 26825441, https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.036.
Ramanoel S, Hoyau E, Kauffmann L, Renard F, Pichat C, Boudiaf N, et al. 2018. Gray matter volume and cognitive performance during normal aging. A Voxel-based morphometry study. Front Aging Neurosci 10:235, PMID: 30123123, https://doi.org/ 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00235.
Rubinov M, Sporns 0. 2010. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage 52(3):1059-1069, PMID: 19819337, https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003. Schaefer A, Kong R, Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Zuo XN, Holmes AJ, et al. 2018. Local-global panellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cereb Cortex 28(9):3095 3114, PMID: 28981612, https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx179.
Schmermund A, Mohlenkamp S, Stang A, Gronemeyer D, Seibel R, Hirche H, et al. 2002. Assessment of clinically silent atherosclerotic disease and established and novel risk factors for predicting myocardial infarction and cardiac death in healthy middle-aged subjects: rationale and design of the Heinz Nixdorf RECALL study. Am Heart J 144(2):212 218, PMID: 12177636, https://doi.org/10. 1067/mhj.2002.123579. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, et al. 2009. Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(31):13040-13045, PMID: 19620724, https://doi.org/10. 1073/pnas.0905267106.
Stumme J, Jockwitz C, Hoffstaedter F, Amunts K, Caspers S. 2020. Functional network reorganization in older adults: graph-theoretical analyses of age, cognition and sex. Neuroimage 214:116756, PMID: 32201326, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j. neuroimage.2020.116756. Textor J, Hardt J, Kniippel S. 2011. DAGitty: a graphical tool for analyzing causal diagrams. Epidemiology 22(5):745, PMID: 21811114, https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE. 0b013e318225c2be.
Thurston GD, Kipen H, Annesi-Maesano I, Balmes J, Brook RD, Cromar K, et al. 2017. A joint ERS/ATS policy statement: what constitutes an adverse health effect of air pollution? An analytical framework. Eur Respir J 49(1 ):1600419, PMID: 28077473, https://doi.org/10.1183/13993003.00419-2016. Tzivian L, Dlugaj M, Winkler A, Weinmayr G, Hennig F, Fuks KB, et al. 2016. Long-term air pollution and traffic noise exposures and mild cognitive impairment in older adults: a cross-sectional analysis of the Heinz Nixdorf recall study. Environ Health Perspect 124(9):1361-1368, PMID: 26863687, https://doi.org/10. 1289/ehp.1509824.
UNESCO (United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization). 1997. ISCED 1997. International Standard Classification of Education. http://uis. unesco.org/sites/default/files/documents/international-standard-classification-of-education-1997-en_0.pdf [accessed 26 October 2021].
WHO (World Health Organization). 2011. Burden of Disease from Environmental Noise -Quantification of Healthy Life Years Lost in Europe, https://www.euro.who.in1/__ data/assets/pdf_file/0008/136466/e94888.pdf [accessed 26 October 2021]. Wig GS. 2017. Segregated systems of human brain networks. Trends Cogn Sci 21(12):981-996, PMID: 29100737, https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.006.
Wilker EH, Preis SR, Beiser AS, Wolf PA, Au R, Kloog I, et al. 2015. Long-term exposure to fine particulate matter, residential proximity to major roads and measures of brain structure. Stroke 46(5):1161-1166, PMID: 25908455, https://doi.org/ 10.1161/STRO KEAH A. 114.008348. Winkler G, Doring A. 1998. Validation of a short qualitative food frequency list used in several German large scale surveys. Z Ernahrungswiss 37(3):234-241, PMID: 9800314, https://doi.org/10.1007/pl00007377.
WMA (World Medical Association). 2013. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310(20):2191-2194, PMID: 24141714, https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2013. 281053. Wong A, Lou W, Ho KF, Yiu BKF, Lin S, Chu WCW, et al. 2020. Indoor incense burning impacts cognitive functions and brain functional connectivity in community older adults. Sci Rep 10(1):7090, PMID: 32341386, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-63568-6.
Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. 2011. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106(3):1125-1165, PMID: 21653723, https://doi.Org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011. Zemp E, Elsasser S, Schindler C, Kiinzli N, Perruchoud AP, Domenighetti G, et al. 1999. Long-term ambient air pollution and respiratory symptoms in adults (SAPALDIA study). Am J Respir Crit Care Med 159(4 pt 1):1257 1266, PMID: 10194174, https://doi.Org/10.1164/ajrccm.159.4.9807052.
Zou L, Wang H, Song G. 2019. Impact of road traffic noise on functional connectivity of human brain networks. I0P Conf Ser Earth Environ Sci 252(4):042026, https://doi.Org/10.1088/1755-1315/252/4/042026.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2022. This work is published under Reproduced from Environmental Health Perspectives (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Background: Older adults show a high variability in cognitive performance that cannot be explained by aging alone. Although research has linked air pollution and noise to cognitive impairment and structural brain alterations, the potential impact of air pollution and noise on functional brain organization is unknown. Objective: This study examined the associations between long-term air pollution and traffic noise with measures of functional brain organization in older adults. We hypothesize that exposures to high air pollution and noise levels are associated with age-like changes in functional brain organization, shown by less segregated brain networks. Methods: Data from 574 participants (44.1% female, 56-85 years of age) in the German 1000BRAINS study (2011-2015) were analyzed. Exposure to particulate matter (PMio, PM2.5, and PM2.5 absorbance), accumulation mode particle number (PNam)> and nitrogen dioxide (NO2) was estimated applying land-use regression and chemistry transport models. Noise exposures were assessed as weighted 24-h (Lden) and nighttime (Lmght) means. Functional brain organization of seven established brain networks (visual, sensorimotor, dorsal and ventral attention, limbic, frontoparietal and default network) was assessed using resting-state functional brain imaging data. To assess functional brain organization, we determined the degree of segregation between networks by comparing the strength of functional connections within and between networks. We estimated associations between air pollution and noise exposure with network segregation, applying multiple linear regression models adjusted for age, sex, socioeconomic status, and lifestyle variables. Results: Overall, small associations of high exposures with lesser segregated networks were visible. For the sensorimotor networks, we observed small associations between high air pollution and noise and lower network segregation, which had a similar effect size as a 1-y increase in age [e.g., in sensorimotor network, -0.006 (95% CI: -0.021, 0.009) per 0.3 Xl0"5/m increase in PM2.5 absorbance and -0.004 (95% CI: -0.006, -0.002) per 1-y age increase]. Conclusion: High exposure to air pollution and noise was associated with less segregated functional brain networks, https://doi.org/10.1289/ EHP97372015). Given that cognitive decline and reduced brain volume are also characteristic of normal neuronal aging, it has been suggested that air pollution may accelerate brain aging (Clifford et al. 2016; Fougere et al. 2015). Chronic ambient noise is another growing environmental health problem, stemming from similar air pollution sources (e.g., traffic, transport, industry) (WHO 2011). Chronic noise can lead to annoyance, sleep disturbance, and stress, arousing the autonomous nervous and endocrine system, thereby leading to adverse health outcomes, most prominently cardiovascular diseases (Basner et al. 2014; WHO 2011). In addition, research has linked chronic noise to impaired cognitive function (Fuks et al. 2019; Jafari et al. 2019; Tzivian et al. 2016) and possible changes in brain structure (Cheng et al. 2019; NuBbaum et al. 2020) in adults. However, the precise mechanisms by which noise may affect the brain remain largely unknown. Besides morphological changes, neurological studies on the aging brain also showed changes on the level of functional brain organization related to impaired cognitive function (Betzel et al. 2014; Ramanoel et al. 2018; Armstrong et al. 2020; Stumme et al. 2020). Although brain structure and functional organization are different aspects of the brain that require different analytical methods, they are both important to understand how environmental exposures may affect cognitive function. Therefore, to gain a better understanding of the complex relationship between environmental exposures, brain structure, and cognitive function, studies on how air pollution and noise may influence functional brain organization in the whole brain are needed. The brain is a complex system consisting of different brain regions interconnected with each other for information processing (Biswal et al. 2010).
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 Environmental Epidemiology Group, Institute of Occupational, Social and Environmental Medicine, Centre for Health and Society, Medical Faculty and University Hospital Diisseldorf, Heinrich Heine University Diisseldorf, Diisseldorf, Germany
2 Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, Research Centre Jiilich, Jiilich, Germany
3 Institute for Urban Public Health, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
4 Institute of Medical Informatics, Biometry and Epidemiology, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany