- FC
- functional connectivity
- GCIPL
- ganglion cell-inner plexiform layer complex
- GM
- graymatter
- LCVA
- low contrast visual acuity
- MRI
- magnetic resonance imaging
- OCT
- optical coherence tomography
- PD
- Parkinson's disease
- ROI
- region of interest
- VAPS
- visual attention and processing speed
- VCON
- visual construction
- VMEM
- visual memory
- VPER
- visual perception
Abbreviations
Introduction
Interest in visual research in Parkinson's disease (PD) has grown due to evidence suggesting that visual dysfunction is an early clinical feature [1, 2] and a predictive biomarker of cognitive deterioration [3, 4]. Research on visual dysfunction in PD dates back several decades, with early studies by Bodis-Wollner in the late 1970s demonstrating deficits in visual evoked potentials [5], highlighting the involvement of both retinal and cortical pathways in PD-related visual impairments. Since then, numerous studies have explored both basic and higher-order visual deficits in PD. Work by Cronin-Golomb and colleagues has extensively documented impairments in visuospatial function, mental rotation, facial recognition, and facial emotion processing [6–10]. Similarly, Harris, Lee, and Amick have provided evidence of deficits in contrast sensitivity, optic flow perception, biological motion detection, visual scanning, and bistable perception in PD [11–15]. Wagenaar and collaborators have further demonstrated impairments in navigation and spatial perception, linking these deficits to abnormal processing of optic flow stimuli [16–19]. Jaywant et al. have investigated the relationship between visual and motor symptoms, showing how disruptions in visual perception can contribute to gait disturbances and postural instability [20–25]. Collectively, these studies emphasize that visual dysfunction in PD extends beyond contrast sensitivity deficits and involves complex interactions between visuoperceptual, visuospatial, attentional, and cognitive domains.
The retina has been a primary focus of visual dysfunction studies in PD. It is now well established that in PD early on, even from pre-motor phases [26–28], neurodegeneration of inner retinal cells occurs, involving the amacrine dopaminergic cells and the retinal ganglion cells of the parafovea [26, 29–32]. Retinal abnormalities, confirmed through optical coherence tomography (OCT), correlate with visual deficits [28]. Dopamine deficits observed in retinal cells explain the partial reversibility of some symptoms, such as contrast sensitivity, with L-Dopa therapy in early stages [33]. However, visual disturbances in PD cannot be fully explained by retinal changes [28], suggesting that brain alterations also play a key role and could help better explain the early and persistent visual deficits in PD.
While primary visual cortex atrophy occurs in late stages [34, 35], early-stage patients show occipital lobe hypometabolism [36], blood hypoperfusion [37], and reduced N-acetylaspartate levels [38]. These changes are linked to increased risks of cognitive decline and dementia, possibly driven by altered functional connectivity (FC) of the occipital cortex with other brain regions affected early in PD [39], such as cortical and subcortical hubs participating in ventral or dorsal associative visual networks. Most previous brain imaging studies addressing visual abnormalities in PD have focused on visual hallucinations or cognitive impairment but only a few have specifically evaluated the relationship between visual impairment and structural and functional brain alterations [40–47]. However, variations in imaging analysis techniques, cognitive tests, and task designs have resulted in divergent results.
In this study, we evaluated idiopathic PD patients and controls by combining multidomain psychophysical visual tests, multimodal brain imaging, and retinal OCT to investigate the structural and functional alterations underlying visual dysfunction in PD. Through a novel methodological approach that integrates voxel-level association maps across four visual domains, we looked for the common neuroanatomical substrate of visual disability in PD, exploring its impact on brain FC and evaluating its relationship with retinal neurodegeneration and disease progression stages.
Methods
Study Design and Participants
We conducted a cross-sectional evaluation of 42 patients with idiopathic PD (iPD) and 29 controls, recruited from Cruces University Hospital. Patients met the UK Brain Bank criteria for PD diagnosis [48] and were assessed in on-medication condition. We excluded participants with neurological, ophthalmological or systemic diseases other than PD that could affect study outcomes, as described before [49]. Exclusions included neurological, ophthalmological, or systemic diseases potentially affecting outcomes, as well as genetic forms of PD identified through routine clinical testing (LRRK2, PARK2, SNCA mutations: genetic testing was performed in individuals with more than one first or second-degree relative affected by the disease or those under 50 years of age). Controls were excluded if they had more than one first-degree relative with PD or PD-like symptoms. The Basque Clinical Research Ethics Committee approved the protocol (ID: PI2014154).
Demographical Features and
Age, sex, and years of education were recorded. For iPD patients, disease duration, Hoehn & Yahr Scale score, Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) score, and Levodopa equivalent daily dose (LEDD) were documented.
Visual and Cognitive Outcome Assessment
Binocular high-contrast and low-contrast visual acuity (LCVA) was measured using ETDRS and Sloan 2.5% charts, respectively, at 4 m with best correction and recorded as the total number of correct letters. LCVA was chosen as a marker of primary visual function due to its strong correlation with retinal atrophy and cognitive impairment in PD [3, 4, 27]. General cognition was evaluated with the Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA). Visual cognition was assessed through a neuropsychological battery grouped into three domains, as described previously [28]: (1) visual attention and processing speed (VAPS), including Salthouse Perceptual Comparison Test, Trail Making Test—part A, and Symbol Digit Modalities Test; (2) visual perception (VPER), including Picture Completion subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV, Benton Judgement of Line Orientation Test, and Number Location and Cube Analysis tests of the Visual Object and Space Perception battery; (3) visual construction (VCON), consisting of Clock Drawing Test. Composite scores were calculated for LCVA, VAPS, VPER, and VCON. Visual hallucinations were screened using the North-East Visual Hallucinations Interview (NEVHI) [50].
Retinal
Macular volumetric images were obtained with a Spectralis spectral-domain OCT system (Heidelberg Engineering, Heidelberg, Germany). Images met OSCAR-IB quality criteria [51] and the built-in software segmented macular layers (HRA Spectralis Viewing Module version 6.0.9.0). The thickness of the parafoveal ganglion cell-inner plexiform layer complex (GCIPL) was analyzed, as it is the most affected measure in PD and strongly relates to visual outcomes [26, 28]. Measurements from both eyes were averaged, following APOSTEL guidelines [52].
Brain Imaging Acquisition and Analyses
MRI was performed using a Philips 3-Tesla scanner with a 32-channel head coil, including T1-weighted and resting-state functional sequences. T1 images were processed with FSL's (version 6.0.1) voxel-based morphometry (VBM) protocol, including brain extraction, MNI152 registration, modulation, and smoothing. Functional images were preprocessed using the FC CONN (v18b) toolbox [53], with the default pipeline for volume-based analyses that included denoising and band-pass filtering [54]. Group comparisons accounted for head size using FSL-SIENAX (see Supporting Information for a full description of image acquisition parameters and preprocessing steps).
Calculation of the
VBM was used to examine the relation between gray matter volume in specific areas of the brain and visual function in both iPD patients and healthy controls. We looked at the four different measures of visual function and controlled for factors like age and head size while analyzing the data separately for patients and controls. Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement (TFCE) with 5000 iterations was used for multiple comparison correction. The four VBM corrected-maps were then intersected to detect common ROIs whose volume had a significant association with all the four visual outcomes, defining the so-called visualROI. The significance of the maps was adjusted to achieve a non-null intersection, that occurred at equal to 0.8, the smallest threshold that guaranteed a non-null intersection across all maps. The automated anatomical labeling atlas (AAL) was used to anatomically describe the visualROI.
Functional Connectivity of the
Seed-based FC analysis was performed using the visualROI as the seed. ROI-to-ROI analyses compared FC in iPD and controls, and the iPD<control contrast was conducted. The “CONN network cortical ROIs atlas” defined 31 ROIs grouped into 8 functional networks including the visual network, which is composed of 4 ROIs: medial visual (anatomically corresponding to cuneus, lingual, and fusiform gyrus), occipital visual (pericalcarine and lateral occipital cortex), left lateral visual, and right lateral visual (inferior temporal, middle temporal, superior temporal and supramarginal gyrus, and inferior parietal cortex) (see Supporting Information for a detailed description). Age was included as a confounding factor in the regression analyses. Connectivity circular graphs were created with false discovery rate (FDR) corrected p < 0.05 to binarize the 33 × 33 resulting resting-state FC matrices.
Calculation of the
VBM identified brain regions associated with GCIPL thickness (retinalROI) in iPD patients. The intersection of retinalROI and visualROI determined regions associated with both retinal thinning and visual dysfunction. Finally, with the aim of identifying models that better explain the relationship between neurodegeneration of the retina, amygdala, and brain, we analyzed the volumetric correlation of the different anatomical components of retinalROI. We extracted gray matter partial volume estimates from brain regions associated with parafoveal GCIPL thickness (retinalROI) and calculated the covariance between these regional volumes to identify clusters of regions exhibiting simultaneous atrophy, potentially indicative of distinct disease stages. Hierarchical clustering was used to define these clusters. We compared the identified clusters with Braak stages for alpha-synuclein pathology to determine which cluster/disease stage was most strongly associated with retinal volume loss.
Statistical Analysis of Data
Analyses were performed in R (v3.6.1) and Python's statsmodels toolbox. Normality was assessed with the Shapiro-Wilks test. Continuous variables were compared using Mann–Whitney U or Kruskal–Wallis tests, with Dunn's post hoc test for multiple comparisons. Categorical variables were analyzed with Fisher's exact test. Correlations were evaluated with Spearman coefficients, and p-values were FDR-corrected (p < 0.05).
Data Sharing
Datasets are not publicly available publicly due to patient privacy regulations but may be requested from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Results
Demographics and Clinical Characteristics of Study Population
Table 1 summarizes demographic and clinical characteristics of participants. No statistically significant differences were found in age or education years between iPD and controls, although the percentage of males was significantly lower in the latter group. In the patients, the median disease duration was 6.1 years, their median Hoehn & Yahr score was 2.0, and the median UPDRS III score was 26.9. In the majority, the disease onset occurred between 55 and 65 years of age, although 10 patients (23.8%) were early onset iPD (disease onset between 21 and 50 years of age) with negative genetic testing (see Section 2). As expected, iPD patients showed a significantly lower cognitive score (MoCA) and worse visual outcomes than controls, especially in LCVA and visual construction tests. A total of seven patients (16.7%) had a history of visual hallucinations.
TABLE 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics of study participants.
iPD | Controls | p | |
N | 42 | 29 | |
Demographics | |||
Age (years) | 60.3 (8.3) | 59.8 (7.0) | 0.781 |
Males, n (%) | 28 (66.7) | 12 (41.4) | 0.035 |
Education, years | 10.5 (4.2) | 12.2 (4.2) | 0.108 |
PD-related features | |||
Disease duration, years | 6.1 (3.6) | — | — |
Age at disease onset, years | 54.2 (7.8) | — | — |
LEDD, mg/day | 595.6 (354.6) | — | — |
UPDRS I | 2.1 (1.7) | — | — |
UPDRS II | 11.3 (5.7) | — | — |
UPDRS III | 26.9 (11.3) | — | — |
UPDRS IV | 3.9 (3.5) | — | — |
Hoehn & Yahr | 2.0 (1.0 to 3.0) | — | — |
Presence of visual hallucinations, n (%) | 7 (16.7%) | — | — |
Cognitive status | |||
MoCA total score | 24.6 (3.1) | 26.6 (3.2) | 0.009 |
Visual outcomes | |||
High-contrast VA, no. of letters | 59.2 (5.9) | 61.6 (4.2) | 0.061 |
Low-contrast VA, no. of letters (LCVA) | 29.1 (10.8) | 38.0 (6.2) | < 0.001 |
Visual attention and processing speed (VAPS) | −0.1 (0.7) | 0.2 (0.6) | 0.046 |
Visual perception (VPER) | −0.1 (0.7) | 0.3 (0.6) | 0.013 |
Visual construction (VCON) | −0.1 (0.9) | 0.4 (0.6) | 0.009 |
Amygdala Atrophy as a Central Role for Visual Disability in i
The common intersection of the four association maps for visual outcomes and gray matter volume, the visualROI, is represented in Figure 1. A full anatomical characterization of the visualROI is given in Table 2. Remarkably, visualROI was preferably located in ventral regions of the brain, rather than primary visual-processing areas. Approximately, 50% of the visualROI volume was found in the amygdala bilaterally (32% left and 15% right) and 20% in the left olfactory cortex, hippocampus, and superior temporal gyrus. In fact, the visualROI occupied a large part of the volume of the amygdala, especially on the left hemisphere (63% in the left and 25% in the right amygdala).
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
TABLE 2 VisualROI in iPD patients.
AAL label | Voxels (n) | Volume (mm3) | MNI X (mm) | MNI Y (mm) | MNI Z (mm) | AAL label percentage (%) | VisualROI percentage (%) |
Amygdala_L | 137 | 1096 | −25 | −2 | −17 | 62.27 | 32.31 |
Amygdala_R | 62 | 496 | 25 | 1 | −15 | 25.00 | 14.62 |
Olfactory_L | 29 | 232 | −21 | 6 | −16 | 10.36 | 6.84 |
Hippocampus_L | 23 | 184 | −28 | −7 | −18 | 2.47 | 5.42 |
Templ_Pole_Sup_L | 21 | 168 | −28 | 5 | −22 | 1.63 | 4.95 |
Interestingly, when we computed the associations between disease duration and brain volume, we found that disease duration was associated with several, mainly temporal-sided regions, including the left temporal superior, middle, and supramarginal lobes but not with the amygdala volume—the main structure within the visualROI.
The voxel-level association maps for the four visual domains (LCVA, VPERC, VAPS, and VCONS) demonstrated significant correspondence with traditional visual regions, including the thalamus, calcarine, fusiform, lingual, and inferior, middle, and superior occipital cortices (see detailed results in Tables S1–S4). For example, VCONS displayed a notable overlap with the lingual (29.94%), occipital inferior (34.35%), and fusiform (23.96%) regions, while VAPS was strongly associated with the calcarine (27.22%) and lingual (14.84%) regions. LCVA also showed substantial contributions from the thalamus (31.05%) and lingual (16.29%) regions. These findings confirm that traditional visual regions are represented in the voxel-level association maps of all four visual domains. However, when analyzing the intersection of the four voxel-level association maps, the resulting visualROI did not include any of these traditional visual regions. Instead, the visualROI primarily comprised subcortical and limbic structures, such as the amygdala, olfactory cortex, and superior temporal gyrus. This highlights the distinct role of these regions in integrating visual functions in PD and underscores the amygdala's central role in early visual dysfunctions.
Disruption of Functional Connectivity Between Amygdala and Visual Networks in i
We next analyzed the FC of visualROI with the main brain networks and their corresponding connectome layout (Figure 2). In controls, visualROI presented a strong positive FC with lateral sensorimotor networks bilaterally and a moderate positive FC with medial and lateral (left and right) visual networks, language networks (left and rightposterior superior temporal gyri and right inferior frontal gyrus), salience networks (left and right supramarginal gyri, anterior insula, and anterior cingulate cortex), and dorsal attention network (left frontal eye field) (Figure 2A). In the visualROI connectome layout of iPD, there were important differences compared to controls (Figure 2B). First, we observed a disruption of the FC of the visualROI with both medial and lateral visual networks bilaterally, with the dorsal attention network (frontal eye field left), with the language network (left and right posterior superior temporal gyri), and with a part of the salience network (left and right supramarginal gyri). Second, in iPD, significant connections of the visualROI emerged with the default mode network (medial prefrontal cortex) and salience network (anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula left and right). More importantly, we determined that the reduced connectivity between visualROI and visual networks (medial and both lateral) in iPD was the only significant connectivity difference (Figure 2C) compared to controls after correcting for multiple comparisons.
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
Retinal and Amygdala Atrophy in i
After family wise error (FEW)-correction, a statistically significant positive association was found in iPD and not in controls between the parafoveal GCIPL thickness and the volume of several gray matter regions of the frontal and ventral brain, mainly subcortical structures (retinalROI) (Figure 3). More specifically, retinalROI mainly coincided with bilateral basal ganglia and thalamus (17.9% and 17.5% of retinalROI volume, respectively) to a lesser extent with frontal cortical regions (12.8%), hippocampus and parahippocampus (9.4%), and cingulum (6.1%) and with the primary visual cortex (calcarine sulcus) (4.1%) and visual associative cortex (fusiform and lingual gyri) (4.0%) (Figure 3 and Table S5). More specifically, among the structures of the basal ganglia within the retinalROI, the caudate nuclei stood out (10.6%), followed by the amygdala (5.2%) and the putamen (2.0%). Of note, approximately, 50% of the amygdala volume was included in the resulting retinalROI (56.8% of left and 45.6% of right amygdala). To investigate to what extent visualROI was anatomically related to retinalROI, we computed the intersection between both ROIs and confirmed that there was a partial but clear overlap, especially with the left and right amygdalae, since 54% of retinalROI–visualROI intersection corresponded to amygdala (Table S5). Finally, analysis of covariance of the retinalROI regions revealed three primary clusters: (1) thalamic nuclei, (2) amygdala and visual regions (visual lateral and occipital), and (3) frontal regions (mesocortex (ACC) and frontal attention networks (dlPFC)) (Figure 4A). According to this analysis, retinal thinning was most strongly associated with gray matter volume loss in the frontal cluster. In contrast, amygdala atrophy was most closely associated with volume loss in lateral visual and occipital visual regions. These findings support the hypothesis of a two-phase progression model aligned with Braak's stages (Figure 4B). In the early phase (Braak 1–3), amygdala degeneration leads to disconnection from the ventral visual pathway and extra-geniculate visual pathway, impairing conscious and non-conscious visual processing. In the later phase (Braak 4–5), degeneration extends to the frontal regions and the retina, collectively exacerbating visual dysfunctions and highlighting the amygdala's central role in integrating retinal and cortical pathways. This observation does not imply propagation of pathology from the retina to frontal regions but rather suggests that the timing of significant retinal atrophy aligns with the neurodegeneration of frontal regions occurring in Braak stages 4–5.
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
Discussion
This study comprehensively evaluates the neuroanatomical basis of visual dysfunction in PD by integrating structural MRI, resting-state functional MRI, and retinal OCT with a detailed assessment of visual function. Although visual outcomes in PD were associated with atrophy in various cortical and subcortical regions, including classic primary and associative visual areas such as the calcarine cortex and medial temporal cortex, our findings highlight amygdala atrophy (visualROI) as the common denominator across all visual domains explored. FC analyses further revealed statistically significant disruptions between the amygdala and visual networks in PD compared to controls, underscoring the amygdala's central role in the genesis of visual disability. Moreover, the observed relationships between retinal atrophy, amygdala degeneration, and other brain regions support a two-phase progression model of visual impairments in PD, aligned with Braak's stages. In this amygdala-centric model, early amygdala degeneration disrupts visual pathways, including the ventral visual and extrageniculate networks, impairing both conscious and unconscious visual processing (Braak 1–3). In more advanced stages (Braak 4–5), disconnection between the amygdala and frontal regions, combined with retinal neurodegeneration, exacerbates visual disability. This integrative model highlights the amygdala's critical role in the progression of PD-related visual dysfunction and establishes a framework for understanding its structural and functional underpinnings.
While many previous imaging studies focused on visual hallucinations or cognitive impairment, few have examined structural and functional alterations underlying visual dysfunction in PD. Prior studies reported associations between visuoperceptual and visuospatial deficits in PD and thinning in different cortical regions, including the superior parietal cortex and precuneus [40], lateral parietal and temporal cortices, [42] and left superior temporal parahippocampal and lingual cortices [43]. Building on previous studies, our voxel-level association maps revealed significant correlations in PD between visual outcomes (LCVA, VCONS, VAPS, and VPERC) and atrophy in various primary and associative visual regions, including the lingual gyrus, fusiform gyrus, inferior occipital cortex, calcarine cortex, and thalamus (see Figure 1 and Tables S1–S4). A key distinction of our study, however, is the identification of the amygdala as a shared region of interest (visualROI) across multiple visual domains. This integrative finding underscores the amygdala's central role in PD-related visual impairments, providing a unifying neuroanatomical basis for deficits spanning diverse visual functions.
Combined structural and diffusion tensor imaging studies have demonstrated microstructural changes in optic radiations connecting the lateral geniculate nucleus and V2 as well as reduced visual cortical volume and chiasmatic area [44]. However, due to the absence of diffusion imaging in our study, we could not evaluate the relationship between visual tract microstructural damage and visual outcomes. Resting-state fMRI studies in PD consistently show reduced FC between visual networks and other major brain networks [55, 56]. Task-based fMRI has provided further insights into visual dysfunction mechanisms. For example, impairments in optic flow processing, linked to gait disturbances, are associated with reduced activation in the MT+ region and altered activity in the cingulate sulcus [45]. Abnormal wide-field optic flow processing in PD has also been linked to reduced visual cortex activation and increased connectivity between V5, the pre-supplementary motor area, and the cerebellum [47]. Additionally, visuospatial attention tasks reveal diminished cortical connectivity modulation in regions such as the superior frontal gyrus, opercular cortex, and parieto-occipital cortex [57], suggesting that impaired interactions between attentional and visual networks contribute to visual dysfunction in PD. Disruptions in contrast luminance perception have also been associated with abnormal superior colliculus responses [46], implicating the extrageniculate visual pathway and its connections to the amygdala.
While these imaging findings have advanced our understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying visual dysfunction in PD, inconsistencies across studies reveal significant methodological challenges. Variations in imaging analysis techniques, cognitive tests, and task designs have led to divergent results. In some cases, tests evaluating visuospatial and visuoperceptual functions were categorized under other cognitive domains, potentially obscuring associations with visual processing. Unlike prior studies that primarily focused on the anatomical and functional correlates of single visual functions or tasks, our study adopted a more integrative approach. This approach allowed us to identify a more comprehensive and generalizable neuroanatomical substrate for visual disability in PD, offering insights that go beyond isolated visual functions. By integrating voxel-level association maps across four visual domains, we identified the amygdala (visualROI) as a shared hub consistently linked to visual impairments. This finding, coupled with disrupted ventral visual network connectivity, highlights the amygdala's central role in PD-related visual dysfunction, providing a more comprehensive and generalizable neuroanatomical framework. The amygdala serves as a hub for visual-emotional integration, modulating attention, prioritizing emotionally relevant stimuli, and contributing to associative learning [58–60]. It connects extensively with both the cortical “high road” (LGN-striate cortex-occipitotemporal cortex-amygdala) and the subcortical “low road” (superior colliculus-pulvinar-amygdala), enabling both conscious and subconscious processing of visual stimuli [61, 62]. This duality reflects the amygdala's evolutionary role in mediating emotional and fear responses. Neurons in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) play a key role in visual learning and attention [61], while the centromedial nucleus focuses awareness on emotionally salient stimuli [63]. Evidence from blindsight patients and studies of non-mammalian vertebrates further supports the critical role of the superior colliculus and pulvinar in automatic, non-conscious emotional visual processing [64, 65].
Unlike PD, visual deficits in healthy older adults are primarily linked to age-related declines in the retina or the visual pathway (up to the occipital cortex), with relatively modest amygdala involvement [66]. Previous studies of aging cohorts have reported limited volumetric reductions in the amygdala [67] and postmortem measurements based on histological staining reveal no significant effect of age on amygdala volume [68]. From a functional point of view, fMRI studies in healthy population support that the amygdala functions similarly in healthy older adults as it does in younger adults [69].
In PD, the amygdala is an early target of Lewy pathology, with neurodegeneration occurring from prodromal stages [70]. Reduced amygdala volume has been observed even in de novo PD patients [71], and the density of Lewy bodies in the amygdala has been found to correlate with the presence of visual alterations in non-demented PD patients [72]. In line with our findings, other studies also found that the degree of neurodegeneration of the amygdala (neuron loss or atrophy) was not associated with the duration of the disease [72, 73]. All of this supports the idea that amygdala neurodegeneration in PD is an early phenomenon that does not further progress later on. The BLA is particularly vulnerable in PD, showing hypoconnectivity with temporal and frontal regions, including the fusiform gyrus and ventromedial prefrontal cortex [74]. These disruptions are linked to cognitive, emotional, and olfactory deficits, underscoring the BLA's role in ventral visual network integration. The BLA serves as a critical hub for multimodal sensory integration, receiving inputs from temporal lobe structures and neuromodulatory systems, including noradrenergic, dopaminergic, and cholinergic pathways originating from the locus coeruleus, ventral tegmental area, and basal forebrain, respectively [75, 76]. Noradrenergic activation of the BLA, triggered by emotionally arousing stimuli, plays a key role in strengthening long-term memory and facilitating object recognition memory, which primarily relies on cortical structures. Notably, the locus coeruleus is among the earliest structures affected in PD, and its degeneration may contribute to early BLA dysfunction, providing a mechanistic explanation for the amygdala's involvement in visual deficits observed in PD.
Our findings align with the “blind to blindsight” hypothesis, which posits that PD patients experience impaired unconscious visual processing and emotional recognition despite preserved conscious vision [77]. This contrasts with classical blindsight, where unconscious visual perception remains intact despite cortical damage [64]. This hypothesis highlights dysfunction in two parallel circuits: (1) the retino-colliculo-thalamo-amygdala pathway, mediating non-conscious emotional processing, and (2) the retino-geniculo-extrastriate pathway, supporting conscious visual identification and spatial awareness. Early amygdala degeneration in PD disrupts both pathways, leading to deficits in motion detection, emotional face recognition, and visual localization.
Thus, our findings reinforce the concept of a ‘silent amygdala’ in PD, where Lewy pathology leads to degeneration and hypoconnectivity, disrupting both conscious and non-conscious visual processing pathways.
The selective vulnerability of the amygdala in PD differentiates it from normal aging patterns, underscoring its critical role in early disease-related changes [72]. The results align with Braak's staging, suggesting that the amygdala is affected in early PD, consistent with the brain-first subtype. As a target of Lewy pathology, early amygdala degeneration disrupts both structural and functional connections with ventral visual stream (supporting conscious object recognition and discrimination), extrageniculate pathway (mediating non-conscious emotional responses and reflexive visual behaviors), higher-order visual networks (including occipital, medial, and lateral areas, which handle complex visual processing) and attention networks (such as the anterior cingulate cortex and precuneus, critical for higher-order tasks like visual attention).
This study has some limitations. Most patients were on dopaminergic and non-dopaminergic medications, which may influence visual outcomes. While dopaminergic effects were controlled by assessing patients in the ON state, non-dopaminergic influences were not fully accounted for. Additionally, while we observed associations between retinal thinning and amygdala atrophy, the underlying mechanisms remain unclear, warranting further investigation. Finally, visual and neuropsychological tests used in this study extend beyond isolated visual assessments, involving multiple cognitive domains, including language processing. Reduced connectivity between the amygdala and the posterior superior temporal gyrus—a key language region—further underscores the impact of multimodal integration deficits in PD. These findings suggest that visual tasks often engage broader cognitive and sensory networks, which are disrupted by PD-related neurodegeneration.
Overall, this study supports a novel amygdala-centric two-phase model of visual dysfunction in PD, aligned with Braak's stages. In the early phase (Braak 1–3), amygdala degeneration disrupts its connections with ventral visual and extrageniculate pathways, impairing conscious and unconscious visual processing. In the later phase (Braak 4–5), degeneration extends to the retina and frontal regions, exacerbating visual deficits. Reduced FC between the amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex, superior temporal gyrus, and other frontal areas further highlights the impact of amygdala-frontal disconnection in advanced stages. Our model also emphasizes a parallel retinal-brain neurodegeneration axis, wherein shared susceptibility between retinal and subcortical structures contributes to visual impairment. The observed correlation between parafoveal GCIPL thinning and amygdala atrophy supports this hypothesis. The early degeneration of the amygdala likely reflects its vulnerability to Lewy pathology, consistent with the brain-first subtype of PD.
Author Contributions
Asier Erramuzpe: data analysis, drafting, editing final version of manuscript. Ane Murueta-Goyena: data analysis, drafting, editing final version of manuscript. Antonio Jimenez-Marin: data analysis; Marian Acera: data acquisition. Sara Teijeira-Portas: data acquisition. Rocío Del Pino: data acquisition. Tamara Fernández-Valle: data acquisition. Ibai Diez: data analysis. Unai Sainz: data analysis. Naroa Ibarretxe-Bilbao: editing of final version of manuscript. Unai Ayala: data analysis, editing final version of manuscript. Maitane Barrenechea: data analysis, editing final version of manuscript. Alberto Cabrera-Zubizarreta: data acquisition. Jesús Cortés: data analysis, editing final version of manuscript. Juan Carlos Gómez-Esteban: editing final version of manuscript. Iñigo Gabilondo: conception/design, drafting, editing final version of manuscript.
Acknowledgments
The authors want to thank all the patients and participants involved in the study.
Conflicts of Interest
This study was cofunded by the Michael J. Fox Foundation (RRIA [Rapid Response Innovation Awards] 2014 Program, grant ID: 10189), the Carlos III Health Institute, and the European Union (ERDF/ESF, “A Way to Make Europe”/“Investing in Your Future”) through the projects PI14/00679 and PI16/00005; and the Department of Health of the Basque Government through the projects 2016111009 and 2020333033. Iñigo Gabilondo contract was funded by an Ikerbasque Fellow contract from Ikerbasque, the Basque Foundation for Science, and by a Juan Rodes grant JR15/00008 from the Carlos III Health Institute. Asier Erramuzpe contract was funded by The Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation, grant “RYC2021-032390-I” “RYC2022-035429-I” and Ikerbasque, the Basque Foundation for Science. Antonio Jimenez-Marin contract was funded by a predoctoral grant from the Basque Government “PRE_2019_1_0070”. Jesús Cortés was funded by Ikerbasque, the Basque Foundation for Science. For the rest of authors, none have received any funding from any institution, including personal relationships, interests, grants, employment, affiliations, patents, inventions, honoraria, consultancies, royalties, stock options/ownership, or expert testimony for the last 12 months. The authors report no competing interests.
Data Availability Statement
The data and the source-code of the analysis that support the findings of this study will be available after publication from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.
W. H. Whitfield, G. Q. Barr, M. J. Khayata, et al., “Contrast Sensitivity Visual Acuity in REM Sleep Behavior Disorder: A Comparison with and Without Parkinson Disease,” Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine 16, no. 3 (2020): 385–388.
Y. Li, H. Zhang, W. Mao, et al., “Visual Dysfunction in Patients with Idiopathic Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Behavior Disorder,” Neuroscience Letters 709 (2019): [eLocator: 134360].
L. A. Leyland, F. D. Bremner, R. Mahmood, et al., “Visual Tests Predict Dementia Risk in Parkinson Disease,” Neurology Clinical Practice 10, no. 1 (2020): 29–39.
A. Murueta‐Goyena, R. Del Pino, M. Galdós, et al., “Retinal Thickness Predicts the Risk of Cognitive Decline in Parkinson Disease,” Annals of Neurology 89 (2020): 165–176.
I. Bodis‐Wollner and M. D. Yahr, “Measurements of Visual Evoked Potentials in Parkinson's Disease,” Brain 101, no. 4 (1978): 661–671.
A. Cronin‐Golomb and A. E. Braun, “Visuospatial Dysfunction and Problem Solving in Parkinson's Disease,” Neuropsychology 11, no. 1 (1997): 44–52.
M. M. Amick, A. Cronin‐Golomb, and G. C. Gilmore, “Visual Processing of Rapidly Presented Stimuli is Normalized in Parkinson's Disease When Proximal Stimulus Strength is Enhanced,” Vision Research 43, no. 26 (2003): 2827–2835.
S. Davidsdottir, A. Cronin‐Golomb, and A. Lee, “Visual and Spatial Symptoms in Parkinson's Disease,” Vision Research 45, no. 10 (2005): 1285–1296.
M. M. Amick, H. E. Schendan, G. Ganis, and A. Cronin‐Golomb, “Frontostriatal Circuits are Necessary for Visuomotor Transformation: Mental Rotation in Parkinson's Disease,” Neuropsychologia 44, no. 3 (2006): 339–349.
U. S. Clark, S. Neargarder, and A. Cronin‐Golomb, “Specific Impairments in the Recognition of Emotional Facial Expressions in Parkinson's Disease,” Neuropsychologia 46, no. 9 (2008): 2300–2309.
J. P. Harris, J. E. Calvert, and O. T. Phillipson, “Processing of Spatial Contrast in Peripheral Vision in Parkinson's Disease,” Brain 115, no. Pt 5 (1992): 1447–1457.
S. A. McDowell and J. P. Harris, “Visual Problems in Parkinson's Disease: A Questionnaire Survey,” Behavioural Neurology 10, no. 2 (1997): 77–81.
A. C. Lee, J. P. Harris, and J. E. Calvert, “Impairments of Mental Rotation in Parkinson's Disease,” Neuropsychologia 36, no. 1 (1998): 109–114.
J. G. Smith, J. P. Harris, S. Khan, E. A. Atkinson, M. S. Fowler, and R. P. Gregory, “Perceptual Bias for Affective and Nonaffective Information in Asymmetric Parkinson's Disease,” Neuropsychology 24, no. 4 (2010): 443–456.
M. M. Amick, I. N. Miller, S. Neargarder, and A. Cronin‐Golomb, “Web‐Based Assessment of Visual and Visuospatial Symptoms in Parkinson's Disease,” Parkinsons Disease 2012 (2012): [eLocator: 564812].
S. Davidsdottir, R. Wagenaar, D. Young, and A. Cronin‐Golomb, “Impact of Optic Flow Perception and Egocentric Coordinates on Veering in Parkinson's Disease,” Brain 131, no. Pt 11 (2008): 2882–2893.
D. E. Young, R. C. Wagenaar, C. C. Lin, et al., “Visuospatial Perception and Navigation in Parkinson's Disease,” Vision Research 50, no. 23 (2010): 2495–2504.
C. C. Lin, R. C. Wagenaar, D. Young, et al., “Effects of Parkinson's Disease on Optic Flow Perception for Heading Direction During Navigation,” Experimental Brain Research 232, no. 4 (2014): 1343–1355.
C. C. Lin and R. C. Wagenaar, “The Impact of Walking Speed on Interlimb Coordination in Individuals with Parkinson's disease,” Journal of Physical Therapy Science 30, no. 5 (2018): 658–662.
D. J. Norton, A. Jaywant, X. Gallart‐Palau, and A. Cronin‐Golomb, “Normal Discrimination of Spatial Frequency and Contrast Across Visual Hemifields in Left‐Onset Parkinson's Disease: Evidence Against Perceptual Hemifield Biases,” Vision Research 107 (2015): 94–100.
A. Jaywant, T. D. Ellis, S. Roy, C. C. Lin, S. Neargarder, and A. Cronin‐Golomb, “Randomized Controlled Trial of a Home‐Based Action Observation Intervention to Improve Walking in Parkinson Disease,” Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 97, no. 5 (2016): 665–673.
A. Jaywant, V. Wasserman, M. Kemppainen, S. Neargarder, and A. Cronin‐Golomb, “Perception of Communicative and Non‐Communicative Motion‐Defined Gestures in Parkinson's Disease,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 22, no. 5 (2016): 540–550.
A. Jaywant, M. Shiffrar, S. Roy, and A. Cronin‐Golomb, “Impaired Perception of Biological Motion in Parkinson's Disease,” Neuropsychology 30, no. 6 (2016): 720–730.
R. D. Salazar, K. L. M. Moon, S. Neargarder, and A. Cronin‐Golomb, “Spatial Judgment in Parkinson's Disease: Contributions of Attentional and Executive Dysfunction,” Behavioral Neuroscience 133, no. 4 (2019): 350–360.
C. C. Wu, B. Cao, V. Dali, et al., “Eye Movement Control During Visual Pursuit in Parkinson's Disease,” PeerJ 6 (2018): [eLocator: e5442].
J. Y. Lee, A. Martin‐Bastida, A. Murueta‐Goyena, et al., “Multimodal Brain and Retinal Imaging of Dopaminergic Degeneration in Parkinson Disease,” Nature Reviews. Neurology 18, no. 4 (2022): 203–220.
V. Polo, M. Satue, M. J. Rodrigo, et al., “Visual Dysfunction and its Correlation with Retinal Changes in Patients with Parkinson's Disease: An Observational Cross‐Sectional Study,” BMJ Open 6, no. 5 (2016): [eLocator: e009658].
A. Murueta‐Goyena, R. Del Pino, P. Reyero, et al., “Parafoveal Thinning of Inner Retina is Associated with Visual Dysfunction in Lewy Body Diseases,” Movement Disorders 34, no. 9 (2019): 1315–1324.
I. Bodis‐Wollner, “Visual Electrophysiology in Parkinson's Disease: PERG, VEP and visual P300,” Clinical Electroencephalography 28, no. 3 (1997): 143–147.
L. Huang, D. Zhang, J. Ji, Y. Wang, and R. Zhang, “Central Retina Changes in Parkinson's Disease: A Systematic Review and Meta‐Analysis,” Journal of Neurology 268, no. 12 (2020): 4646–4654.
L. Veys, M. Vandenabeele, I. Ortuno‐Lizaran, et al., “Retinal Alpha‐Synuclein Deposits in Parkinson's Disease Patients and Animal Models,” Acta Neuropathologica 137, no. 3 (2019): 379–395.
I. Ortuño‐Lizarán, X. Sánchez‐Sáez, P. Lax, et al., “Dopaminergic Retinal Cell Loss and Visual Dysfunction in Parkinson Disease,” Annals of Neurology 88, no. 5 (2020): 893–906.
J. T. Hutton, J. L. Morris, and J. W. Elias, “Levodopa Improves Spatial Contrast Sensitivity in Parkinson's Disease,” Archives of Neurology 50, no. 7 (1993): 721–724.
D. Weintraub, J. Doshi, D. Koka, et al., “Neurodegeneration Across Stages of Cognitive Decline in Parkinson Disease,” Archives of Neurology 68, no. 12 (2011): 1562–1568.
E. J. Burton, I. G. McKeith, D. J. Burn, E. D. Williams, and J. T. O'Brien, “Cerebral Atrophy in Parkinson's Disease with and Without Dementia: A Comparison with Alzheimer's Disease, Dementia with Lewy Bodies and Controls,” Brain 127, no. Pt 4 (2004): 791–800.
D. Garcia‐Garcia, P. Clavero, C. Gasca Salas, et al., “Posterior Parietooccipital Hypometabolism May Differentiate Mild Cognitive Impairment from Dementia in Parkinson's Disease,” European Journal of Nuclear Medicine and Molecular Imaging 39, no. 11 (2012): 1767–1777.
P. Borghammer, “Perfusion and Metabolism Imaging Studies in Parkinson's Disease,” Danish Medical Journal 59, no. 6 (2012): B4466.
K. Nie, Y. Zhang, B. Huang, et al., “Marked N‐Acetylaspartate and Choline Metabolite Changes in Parkinson's Disease Patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment,” Parkinsonism & Related Disorders 19, no. 3 (2013): 329–334.
H. Braak, K. Del Tredici, U. Rub, R. A. de Vos, E. N. Jansen Steur, and E. Braak, “Staging of Brain Pathology Related to Sporadic Parkinson's disease,” Neurobiology of Aging 24, no. 2 (2003): 197–211.
J. B. Pereira, P. Svenningsson, D. Weintraub, et al., “Initial Cognitive Decline is Associated with Cortical Thinning in Early Parkinson disease,” Neurology 82, no. 22 (2014): 2017–2025.
E. Mak, J. Zhou, L. C. Tan, W. L. Au, Y. Y. Sitoh, and N. Kandiah, “Cognitive Deficits in Mild Parkinson's Disease are Associated with Distinct Areas of Grey Matter Atrophy,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 85, no. 5 (2014): 576–580.
A. I. Garcia‐Diaz, B. Segura, H. C. Baggio, et al., “Structural Brain Correlations of Visuospatial and Visuoperceptual Tests in Parkinson's Disease,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 24, no. 1 (2018): 33–44.
A. I. Garcia‐Diaz, B. Segura, H. C. Baggio, et al., “Cortical Thinning Correlates of Changes in Visuospatial and Visuoperceptual Performance in Parkinson's Disease: A 4‐Year Follow‐Up,” Parkinsonism & Related Disorders 46 (2018): 62–68.
A. Arrigo, A. Calamuneri, D. Milardi, et al., “Visual System Involvement in Patients with Newly Diagnosed Parkinson Disease,” Radiology 285, no. 3 (2017): 885–895.
D. Putcha, R. S. Ross, M. L. Rosen, et al., “Functional Correlates of Optic Flow Motion Processing in Parkinson's Disease,” Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 8 (2014): [eLocator: 57].
E. Bellot, L. Kauffmann, V. Coizet, S. Meoni, E. Moro, and M. Dojat, “Effective Connectivity in Subcortical Visual Structures in De Novo Patients with Parkinson's Disease,” Neuroimage: Clinical 33 (2022): [eLocator: 102906].
A. van der Hoorn, R. J. Renken, K. L. Leenders, and B. M. de Jong, “Parkinson‐Related Changes of Activation in Visuomotor Brain Regions During Perceived Forward Self‐Motion,” PLoS One 9, no. 4 (2014): [eLocator: e95861].
A. J. Hughes, S. E. Daniel, L. Kilford, and A. J. Lees, “Accuracy of Clinical Diagnosis of Idiopathic Parkinson's Disease: A Clinico‐Pathological Study of 100 cases,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 55, no. 3 (1992): 181–184.
A. Murueta‐Goyena, R. Del Pino, M. Galdos, et al., “Retinal Thickness Predicts the Risk of Cognitive Decline in Parkinson Disease,” Annals of Neurology 89, no. 1 (2021): 165–176.
U. P. Mosimann, D. Collerton, R. Dudley, et al., “A Semi‐Structured Interview to Assess Visual Hallucinations in Older People,” International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 23, no. 7 (2008): 712–718.
P. Tewarie, L. Balk, F. Costello, et al., “The OSCAR‐IB Consensus Criteria for Retinal OCT Quality Assessment,” PLoS One 7, no. 4 (2012): [eLocator: e34823].
A. Cruz‐Herranz, L. J. Balk, T. Oberwahrenbrock, et al., “The APOSTEL Recommendations for Reporting Quantitative Optical Coherence Tomography Studies,” Neurology 86, no. 24 (2016): 2303–2309.
S. Whitfield‐Gabrieli and A. Nieto‐Castanon, “Conn: A Functional Connectivity Toolbox for Correlated and Anticorrelated Brain Networks,” Brain Connectivity 2, no. 3 (2012): 125–141.
A. Nieto‐Castanon and S. Whitfield‐Gabrieli, “CONN Functional Connectivity Toolbox: RRID SCR_009550. Version version. 2022;22”.
M. Gottlich, T. F. Munte, M. Heldmann, M. Kasten, J. Hagenah, and U. M. Kramer, “Altered Resting State Brain Networks in Parkinson's Disease,” PLoS One 8, no. 10 (2013): [eLocator: e77336].
M. Filippi, E. Sarasso, and F. Agosta, “Resting‐State Functional MRI in Parkinsonian Syndromes,” Movement Disorders Clinical Practice 6, no. 2 (2019): 104–117.
D. Vereb, M. A. Kovacs, S. Antal, et al., “Modulation of Cortical Resting State Functional Connectivity During a Visuospatial Attention Task in Parkinson's Disease,” Frontiers in Neurology 13 (2022): [eLocator: 927481].
D. Bzdok, A. R. Laird, K. Zilles, P. T. Fox, and S. B. Eickhoff, “An Investigation of the Structural, Connectional, and Functional Subspecialization in the Human Amygdala,” Human Brain Mapping 34, no. 12 (2013): 3247–3266.
G. Gainotti, “Unconscious Processing of Emotions and the Right Hemisphere,” Handbook of Clinical Neurology 183 (2021): 27–46.
E. E. Benarroch, “The Amygdala: Functional Organization and Involvement in Neurologic Disorders,” Neurology 84, no. 3 (2015): 313–324, [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000001171].
D. J. Kravitz, K. S. Saleem, C. I. Baker, L. G. Ungerleider, and M. Mishkin, “The Ventral Visual Pathway: An Expanded Neural Framework for the Processing of Object Quality,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17, no. 1 (2013): 26–49.
M. Tamietto, P. Pullens, B. de Gelder, L. Weiskrantz, and R. Goebel, “Subcortical Connections to Human Amygdala and Changes Following Destruction of the Visual Cortex,” Current Biology 22, no. 15 (2012): 1449–1455.
S. Duncan and L. F. Barrett, “The Role of the Amygdala in Visual Awareness,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11, no. 5 (2007): 190–192.
S. Ajina and H. Bridge, “Blindsight and Unconscious Vision: What They Teach Us about the Human Visual System,” Neuroscientist 23, no. 5 (2016): 529–541.
L. Pessoa and R. Adolphs, “Emotion Processing and the Amygdala: From a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of Evaluating Biological Significance,” Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 11, no. 11 (2010): 773–783.
D. Garzone, R. P. Finger, M. M. Mauschitz, et al., “Visual Impairment and Retinal and Brain Neurodegeneration: A Population‐Based Study,” Human Brain Mapping 44, no. 7 (2023): 2701–2711.
J. S. Allen, J. Bruss, C. K. Brown, and H. Damasio, “Normal Neuroanatomical Variation Due to Age: The Major Lobes and a Parcellation of the Temporal Region,” Neurobiology of Aging 26, no. 9 (2005): 1245–1260, discussion 79–82.
J. Brabec, A. Rulseh, B. Hoyt, et al., “Volumetry of the Human Amygdala—An Anatomical Study,” Psychiatry Research 182, no. 1 (2010): 67–72.
K. Nashiro, M. Sakaki, and M. Mather, “Age Differences in Brain Activity During Emotion Processing: Reflections of Age‐Related Decline or Increased Emotion Regulation?,” Gerontology 58, no. 2 (2012): 156–163.
P. Borghammer, J. Horsager, K. Andersen, et al., “Neuropathological Evidence of Body‐First vs. Brain‐First Lewy Body Disease,” Neurobiology of Disease 161 (2021): [eLocator: 105557].
Y. Zeighami, S. M. Fereshtehnejad, M. Dadar, et al., “A Clinical‐Anatomical Signature of Parkinson's Disease Identified with Partial Least Squares and Magnetic Resonance Imaging,” NeuroImage 190 (2019): 69–78.
A. J. Harding, E. Stimson, J. M. Henderson, and G. M. Halliday, “Clinical Correlates of Selective Pathology in the Amygdala of Patients with Parkinson's Disease,” Brain 125, no. Pt 11 (2002): 2431–2445.
M. Qu, B. Gao, Y. Jiang, et al., “Atrophy Patterns in Hippocampus and Amygdala Subregions of Depressed Patients with Parkinson's Disease,” Brain Imaging and Behavior 18, no. 3 (2024): 475–484.
J. Wang, L. Sun, L. Chen, et al., “Common and Distinct Roles of Amygdala Subregional Functional Connectivity in Non‐Motor Symptoms of Parkinson's Disease,” NPJ Parkinsons Disease 9, no. 1 (2023): 28.
Y. F. Chen, Q. Song, P. Colucci, et al., “Basolateral Amygdala Activation Enhances Object Recognition Memory by Inhibiting Anterior Insular Cortex Activity,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 119, no. 22 (2022): [eLocator: e2203680119].
A. J. McDonald, “Functional Neuroanatomy of the Basolateral Amygdala: Neurons, Neurotransmitters, and Circuits,” Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience 26 (2020): 1–38.
N. J. Diederich, G. Stebbins, C. Schiltz, and C. G. Goetz, “Are Patients with Parkinson's Disease Blind to Blindsight?,” Brain 137, no. Pt 6 (2014): 1838–1849.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2025. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ (the "License"). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
ABSTRACT
Objective
Visual disability in Parkinson's disease (PD) is not fully explained by retinal neurodegeneration. We aimed to delineate the brain substrate of visual dysfunction in PD and its association with retinal thickness.
Methods
Forty‐two PD patients and 29 controls underwent 3‐Tesla MRI, retinal spectral‐domain optical coherence tomography, and visual testing across four domains. Voxel‐level associations between gray matter volume and visual outcomes were used to define a visual impairment region (visualROI). Functional connectivity of the visualROI with brain networks was analyzed. Covariance analysis of brain regions associated with retinal thinning (retinalROI) was conducted using hierarchical clustering to develop a model of retinal and brain neurodegeneration linked to disease progression.
Results
The amygdala was the primary component of the visualROI, comprising 32.3% and 14.6% of its left and right volumes. Functional connectivity analysis revealed significant disruptions between the visualROI and medial/lateral visual networks in PD. Covariance analysis identified three clusters within retinalROI: (1) the thalamic nucleus, (2) the amygdala and lateral/occipital visual regions, and (3) frontal regions, including the anterior cingulate cortex and frontal attention networks. Hierarchical clustering suggested a two‐phase progression: early amygdala damage (Braak 1–3) disrupting visual network connections, followed by retinal and frontal atrophy (Braak 4–5) exacerbating visual dysfunction.
Interpretation
Our findings support a novel, amygdala‐centric two‐phase model of visual dysfunction in PD. Early amygdala degeneration disrupts visual pathways, while advanced‐stage disconnection between the amygdala and frontal regions and retinal neurodegeneration contributes to further visual disability.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
















1 Computational Neuroimaging Group, Biobizkaia Health Research Institute, Barakaldo, Spain, Bilbao, Spain
2 Neurodegenerative Diseases Group, Biobizkaia Health Research Institute, Barakaldo, Bizkaia, Spain, Department of Neurosciences, Faculty of Medicine and Nursery, University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU), Leioa, Spain
3 Computational Neuroimaging Group, Biobizkaia Health Research Institute, Barakaldo, Spain
4 Neurodegenerative Diseases Group, Biobizkaia Health Research Institute, Barakaldo, Bizkaia, Spain
5 Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Deusto, Bilbao, Spain
6 Biomedical Engineering Department, Faculty of Engineering, Mondragon University, Mondragon, Spain
7 OSATEK Magnetic Resonance Imaging Unit, Galdakao Hospital, Galdakao, Spain
8 Computational Neuroimaging Group, Biobizkaia Health Research Institute, Barakaldo, Spain, Bilbao, Spain, Department of Cell Biology and Histology, University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU), Leioa, Spain
9 Bilbao, Spain, Neurodegenerative Diseases Group, Biobizkaia Health Research Institute, Barakaldo, Bizkaia, Spain