Correspondence to Dr Dominika M Pindus; [email protected]
Strengths and limitations of this study
The HIIT2SITLess study is a well-controlled randomised crossover pilot feasibility trial designed to isolate the effects of the intensity of short physical activity (PA) bouts to interrupt prolonged sitting on frontoparietal function in middle-aged and older adults.
The trial is designed based on the theory linking the activation of the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system with high-intensity exercise to frontoparietal brain function.
The trial employs rigorous neurophysiological and cognitive measures of frontoparietal function, inhibitory control and episodic memory.
This pilot feasibility trial recruits healthy middle-aged and older adults with a limited cardiovascular risk; hence, its generalisability to populations with an increased cardiovascular risk is limited.
The study focuses on acute but not the long-term benefits of interrupting prolonged sitting with PA on brain function, attentional control and episodic memory.
Introduction
The year 2020 has marked a dramatic shift in the ageing population worldwide, when the number of older adults exceeded the number of children.1 Most older adults aged ≥65 years experience normal age-related cognitive decline, characterised by a decreased ability to control distractions and correctly recall the details of information and events (ie, episodic memory).2–4 These cognitive functions are indispensable for everyday functioning, learning and decision-making.5 6 Given the ubiquity of normal age-related cognitive decline, there is an urgent need for effective approaches to improve cognitive and brain health during ageing.
Yet, effective and scalable interventions to improve cognitive and brain health in older adults are lacking. Traditional physical activity (PA) interventions (eg, a 20–40 min bout of moderate-intensity PA) show promise and can improve frontoparietal function and hippocampal-dependent episodic memory in seniors.7 However, they have limited impact because most older adults (70%) do not engage daily in moderate-intensity PA that lasts even 10 min.8 In contrast, the efficacy of PA interventions that use short (<10 min) but high-intensity PA to improve frontoparietal function and cognition in seniors is virtually unknown. Such interventions could boost PA adoption because they address critical barriers to PA participation in middle-aged and older adults: the lack of time and access to gyms.9
Traditional PA interventions designed to enhance neurocognitive function in older adults also do not reduce their excessive sedentary time (ST), amounting to 10 hours/day.10 Epidemiological evidence suggests that remaining sedentary for 10 hours/day or more increases the risk of Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) and AD-related dementias, even in physically active adults.11 Emergent observational studies indicate that ST and prolonged sitting, such as sitting continuously for 20 min or longer, may attenuate attentional control,12 13 episodic memory14 and frontoparietal function.15 For example, adults aged 21–45 years with more prolonged ST had poorer attentional control.12 Older adults engaging in more ST had poorer episodic memory.14 Pontifex et al15 found a decrease in P3b amplitude in young adults who sat for 20 min, suggesting a decrease in frontoparietal brain function. The P3b component of event-related brain potentials (ERPs) is a stimulus-locked positive-going waveform embedded in an electroencephalogram (EEG) signal, which appears approximately 250–700 ms after stimulus onset with a maximum over parietal electrodes.16 The amplitude of the P3b-ERP component increases proportionally with the attentional resources allocated towards the inhibition of neuronal activity extraneous to the task in order to facilitate the task-relevant attentional processing; its latency is thought to index the speed of stimulus evaluation.16 The P3b-ERP component is considered a marker of frontoparietal function because several of its cortical generators overlap with frontoparietal regions.17–19 Yet, it is unknown how prolonged sitting can affect frontoparietal function in middle-aged and older adults and if interrupting sitting with high-intensity PA could improve it.
Spatial patterns of coactivation between brain regions supporting cognitive performance are already observed at rest in correlated fluctuations of activity, known as intrinsic brain networks.20 One such network, the frontoparietal network (FPN; comprising hubs in the frontal cortex and intraparietal sulcus21), supports cognitive control functions, including attentional control.21–23 Higher functional connectivity (FC) at rest in this network predicts better attentional control in older adults.22 Yet, FC in the FPN declines with age24 25 and in AD,26 which predicts faster cognitive decline.25 Another network relevant to cognitive ageing is the default mode network (DMN; it comprises regions in the medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate cortices27 28), which supports episodic memory.29 FC in this network also declines with age,25 30 presaging faster cognitive decline.25 A decline in FC within the DMN has also been related to episodic memory decline in older adults.29 Accordingly, changes in FC in the FPN and the DMN can enhance our understanding of PA effects on brain functions that are susceptible to age-related and AD-related decline.
To be effective, PA interventions should target the mechanisms underlying the decreasing efficiency of the frontoparietal functions, attentional control and episodic memory decline during ageing. The locus coeruleus, a group of noradrenergic neurons in the pons,31 helps maintain the structural integrity of the FPN.32 Cerebral norepinephrine increases activation in the frontoparietal brain regions and optimises attentional control.33–35 It also binds to β-adrenoreceptors in the hippocampus, stimulating learning and memory,36 37 including episodic memory.38–40 Its effects may also extend to increased FC in the DMN.41 42 PA is thought to stimulate phasic norepinephrine release from the locus coeruleus31 43 and enhance frontoparietal function,44–46 attentional control47 and episodic memory48 via locus coeruleus projections to the prefrontal and parietal cortices49 50 and the hippocampus.51 Yet, the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system (LC-NE) is highly susceptible to ageing52 and AD.53 Thus, PA interventions designed to stimulate the LC-NE system could significantly impact the functional integrity of the ageing brain.
High-intensity interval training (HIIT) could stimulate the LC-NE system because it uses short high-intensity intervals (interspersed with brief periods of rest), which can rapidly enhance peripheral catecholamine release54 55 and stimulate the LC-NE system.56 57 In confirmation, experimental studies in young adults showed that a HIIT bout lasting ≤10 min can improve frontoparietal function and attentional control at a short 15–20 min delay.58 59 However, the effect of a single bout of HIIT on cognitive function declines after 20–30 min.47 Thus, a single bout cannot counteract the potential adverse effect of 5 hours of prolonged sitting that adults of all ages engage in daily60 on neurocognitive function. Whether regularly interrupting prolonged sitting with short (<10 min) bouts of HIIT could be leveraged to improve cognitive and brain function in middle-aged and older adults over several hours is unknown.
Several previous studies tested the effect of frequent but short PA (2–5 min) breaks to prolonged sitting of primarily light intensity on cognitive function relative to sitting alone.61–63 Yet, they were unsuccessful in improving cognitive functions. One reason for this null effect can be insufficient PA intensity (ie, light or moderate) to stimulate the LC-NE system within 2–5 min.64–66 As discussed above, adults spend a substantial proportion of the day in prolonged sitting (~48%), which increases with age. The proposed work overcomes these limitations by leveraging short HIIT bouts at the intensity and duration sufficient to increase peripheral catecholamines64 65 to enhance cognitive and brain functions.
Study aims and objectives
The lack of effective PA interventions to reduce prolonged sitting and enhance cognitive and brain function in middle-aged and older adults reflects a significant gap in our understanding of the detrimental effects of prolonged sitting on brain health and the necessary PA dose to counter its effects. The HIIT2SITLess study was designed to address this gap. The HIIT2SITLess study is a randomised crossover pilot feasibility trial designed to test three specific aims:
To assess the feasibility, acceptability, fidelity and safety of HIIT breaks to improve neurocognitive function.
To quantify the differences between conditions in a change in P3b amplitude and latency, a marker of frontoparietal function.
To explore the differences between conditions in attentional control, episodic memory and FC in the FPN and DMNs.
The study will test the following hypotheses:
HIIT interruptions to prolonged sitting will be feasible, acceptable and safe and can be implemented with fidelity to enhance neurocognitive function in middle-aged and older adults.
HIIT versus light intensity interval training (LIIT) bouts will result in greater changes in P3b amplitude and latency.
HIIT versus LIIT bouts will improve attentional control and episodic memory.
HIIT versus LIIT bouts will enhance FC in frontoparietal and DMNs.
Given the emergent evidence that acute responses to exercise can predict chronic adaptations in brain function and cognitive performance,67 the findings from this study can inform future acute and chronic PA interventions to reduce prolonged sitting and enhance brain health in middle-aged and older adults.
Methods and analysis
Study setting and design
HIIT2SITLess is a randomised crossover trial with two interventions lasting 3.5 hours each: prolonged sitting interrupted every 30 min with 6 min HIIT bouts active condition and prolonged sitting interrupted every 30 min with 6 min LIIT bouts control condition. The study is conducted over three consecutive visits. The participants will be recruited to the trial between February 2024 and March 2026. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Institutional Review Board at the University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign (see online supplemental table 1 for sponsor details).
Trial registration
The trial was registered on ClinicalTrials.gov No. NCT06243016 before the enrolment of the first participant. See online supplemental table 2 for trial registration details.
Participants
The study will enrol 54 (27 female) middle-aged (40–59 years) and older (60–75 years) cognitively healthy adults from Champaign County, IL, and the surrounding areas. This age range was chosen based on the proven safety of HIIT in similar age groups,68 a steeper decline in physical function after the age of 75 years,69 and previous exercise trials and cohort studies into cognitive and brain health in middle-aged70–72 and older adults.7 67
Eligibility
Our inclusion and exclusion criteria have been designed to enrol individuals who are sedentary, low or moderately physically active and can safely engage in acute high-intensity exercise. The criteria were developed to emphasise safety and generalisability of study outcomes. Table 1 outlines the study’s inclusion and exclusion criteria.
Table 1Study inclusion and exclusion criteria
Inclusion criteria | Exclusion criteria |
Age 40–75 years; including premenopausal, postmenopausal and perimenopausal women regardless of hormone therapy replacement. | Physical disability or musculoskeletal disease prohibitive to vigorous exercise |
BMI <40 kg/m2 | Learning disabilities |
Sedentary (≥6 hours/day sitting by a survey question) | Cognitive abilities below a 26-point cut-off on a MoCA |
The Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviour Questionnaire in the low and moderate physical activity range | Type 1 or 2 diabetes |
Capable of exercising vigorously based on the PARQ+ | Neurological condition (eg, MS, Parkinson, dementia, MCI) |
Has a medical clearance for maximal exercise and HIIT from a physician | Colour blindness |
Normotensive or participant’s blood pressure is controlled (ie, individuals who had previously been at or above the 140/90 mm Hg threshold before the initiation of treatment but are now below this threshold) | Brain injury (eg, traumatic brain injury, stroke) |
Intellectual ability no less than 1 SD relative to the population mean (ie, ≥85 where mean=100, SD=15) as measured with KBIT-2 | Presence of other health conditions that may be exacerbated by exercise |
No current or previous diagnosis of type 1 or type 2 diabetes confirmed by the participant’s physician | History of heart disease |
Fasting blood glucose <126 mg/dL or HbA1c <6.5% in the last 12 months | High cholesterol not controlled by medication |
Good or corrected vision (near vision 20/30) and hearing | Signs and symptoms indicative of underlying cardiovascular disease (based on General Health History Questionnaire) |
No significant abnormalities on the ECG during the maximal exercise test | A chronic pulmonary disease (eg, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease) |
No signs and symptoms that suggest an underlying cardiovascular disease as recorded during the maximal exercise test by a study physician | Emphysema |
No indications to prematurely stop the maximal exercise test as outlined by the ACSM’s Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription | Pulmonary embolus |
Concussion if more than 12 months before the study screening | Asthma |
History of cancer but in full remission for at least 12 months and no history of chemotherapy, signed off by the physician or an oncologist | History of renal disease |
History of seizures | |
A neuropsychiatric disorder (eg, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, schizophrenia) | |
Osteoporosis if it interferes with an individual’s ability to exercise | |
Severe back problems | |
Severe arthritis if it interferes with an individual’s ability to exercise | |
Thyroid disorder not controlled by medication | |
Polyneuropathy | |
Sleep disorders except for obstructive sleep apnoea | |
AIDS | |
Hepatitis C | |
History of long COVID-19 | |
Current or past smoking <12 months | |
Corticosteroid intake <31 days before screening | |
Opioids taken <6 months from screening | |
Anabolic androgens taken <31 days before screening | |
A serious illness or hospitalisation in the last 6 months | |
Currently taking medications that can affect the central nervous system (except for antidepressants and anxiolytics) | |
Current participation in an ongoing trial likely to influence exercise ability or cognitive function |
ACSM, American College of Sports Medicine; BMI, body mass index; HbA1c, glycated haemoglobin; HIIT, high-intensity interval training; KBIT-2, Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test-2; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; PARQ+, Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire+.
Blinding and randomisation
54 participants will be randomised to two condition sequences by a statistician following baseline assessments. Permuted block randomisation generated using the PROC PLAN procedure (SAS Institute)73 is used, where sequences are randomised within a block of six participants to minimise the possibility of group imbalances due to dropout. Participants are randomised to one of two condition sequences by a study statistician: (1) X=HIIT, LIIT breaks or Y=LIIT, HIIT breaks. Generated permuted block randomisation also ensures that blocks are balanced by cognitive task (ie, flanker (F), antisaccade (A) and mnemonic similarities task (M)) sequence (FAM, MFA and AMF). The principal and coinvestigators will be blinded to the sequence allocation. The sequence will be concealed until the participant’s enrolment. On enrolment, the study sequence will be verbally communicated to the study coordinator by a statistician. The coordinator will record the sequence number in Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap). The staff implementing the trial will be unblinded. Participants will be blinded as to the intervention order until their first intervention visit.
Recruitment and retention
Recruitment of participants began in February 2024 with planned completion of enrolment by June 2025. The participant recruitment occurs via local media outlets, the local buses, the University list-serve, social media campaigns, contacts to local faith congregations, the University Extension, organisations serving older adults in Champaign County, and flyers, and individual mailouts to adults aged 40–75 years in Champaign County. Recruitment and enrolment occur continuously. The researchers will send reminders and will call to remind participants about their appointments. In case of dropout, the research coordinator will follow up with questions about reasons for withdrawal.
Study procedures
A complete schedule of study assessments is presented in online supplemental table 3.
Screening procedure
Screening call
At the beginning of the screening call, participants will sign an informed consent to the screening process (online supplemental material 1). The screening call is designed to select participants based on age, English language fluency, independent living, physical function, self-reported sitting time, PA, ability to engage in vigorous cycling, disability status, vision and hearing and to screen out individuals with a history of stroke or transient ischaemic attack, long COVID-19 and smokers. A trained researcher will then administer the Telephone Interview of Cognitive Status-modified. Only individuals with a score <32 (a cut-off for mild cognitive impairment)74 will be included. If a participant qualifies based on these assessments, they will complete a General Health History questionnaire designed to screen out participants with an increased risk of cardiovascular disease,75 and pre-existing conditions as listed in the exclusion criteria (table 1). An individual will also fill in the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale.76 Individuals with anxiety and depression will be included due to the high prevalence of these disorders in the general population.77 78 Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale scores will be used to explore the potential confounding effect of these factors on the results. In addition to eligibility based on these assessments, the individual must be cleared by his/her primary care physician (PCP) for maximal and high-intensity exercise.
Physical Activity Questionnaires
The Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviour Questionnaire79 screens out highly physically active individuals who engage in 300 min or more of moderate-to-vigorous PA per week. The Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire+80 is used to identify individuals who may be at a greater risk of participating in high-intensity exercise. Table 2 lists all psychosocial assessments.
Table 2Psychosocial assessments
Name | Description |
The Activities Collected over Time over 24-hours139 | A 24-hour physical activity recall to measure participant’s previous day physical activity, sedentary behaviours and sleep. |
Beck Depression Inventory-2140 | A 21-item inventory to assess attitudes and symptoms of depression in adults aged 18–80 years. |
Cardiorespiratory Fitness Questionnaire141 | A 5-item questionnaire to assess the level of aerobic fitness based on questions about habitual aerobic exercise. |
The Epworth Sleepiness Scale142 | An 8-item scale to assess the levels of sleepiness defined as the chance of dozing off in 8 common circumstances. |
Florida Cognitive Activity Scale | A 23-item scale assessing the frequency that the participants engage in daily cognitively stimulating activities such as crossword puzzles, home repairs, playing chess etc. |
FDI DIS Abbreviated143 144 | An 8-item questionnaire to assess the frequency of engaging in common everyday activities such as visiting friends, taking care of finances, and the extent to which individuals feel limited in their ability to perform such activities. |
FDI FXN Abbreviated 144 145 | A 15-item questionnaire to assess the level of difficulty an individual has with specific activities of daily living such as running and walking 0.5 mile, preparing meals, going up a flight of stairs, etc. |
FXNSE without a device146 | A 15-item scale measuring individual’s confidence in completing specific functional activities unassisted. |
Geriatric Depression Scale147 | It is a 15-item scale that assesses the degree of depressive symptoms and anhedonia in older adults. |
Godin-Shephard Leisure Time Physical Activity Questionnaire148 149 | A 3-item assessment of habitual structured exercise in a typical week. |
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale76 | A 14-item questionnaire with questions about symptoms of depression, anxiety and psychological distress during the past week. |
The Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviour Questionnaire79 | A 7-item questionnaire assessing time spent on average in moderate-to-vigorous physical and muscle strengthening physical activity during a usual week, perceived aerobic fitness, time spent sedentary and frequency of interrupting prolonged sitting. |
Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test-276 | A standardised and normed intelligence test for ages 4–90 years. The test comprises one verbal and two non-verbal components used to compute verbal and non-verbal IQ scores and a general IQ score. |
Karolinska Sleepiness Scale95 | A one-item assessment of individual’s subjective experience of sleepiness over the past 5 min. |
Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)82 150 | MoCA is a screening tool for cognitive impairment. It comprises 13 items assessing 7 cognitive domains: visuospatial and executive function, naming, memory, language, abstraction, and orientation in time and place. |
Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire for Everyone80 | It is a 7-item screening tool recommended as pre-participation screening before a subject begins physical activity. Questions ask about diagnosis and signs and symptoms of cardiovascular disease, medication and bone, joint and soft tissue problems that may prevent an individual from physical activity. |
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index80 | A 9-item tool assessing sleep quality. |
Preference for Tolerance of the Intensity of Exercise Questionnaire151 | A 16-item scale to assess individual’s responses and preference for exercise intensity. |
Rosenberg Sedentary Behaviour Questionnaire152 | An 18-item questionnaire assessing the time individuals spent in various sedentary behaviours on weekdays and weekend days. |
Task Engagement Scale96–98 | A 9-item scale assessing the level of physical, emotional and cognitive engagement in a task. |
Mental Effort Scale99 | A single-item scale assessing the level of mental effort exerted during the task. |
FDI DIS, Late-Life Function and Disiability Instrument -Disability Component; FDI FXN, Late-Life Function and Disability Instrument: Function Component; FXNSE, Function Self Efficacy Scale.
Screening visit
Once the participant who qualified based on a screening call is medically cleared by his/her PCP, the participant will come to the laboratory for an in-person screening visit. Before the screening visit (as well as baseline and intervention visits), participants will be asked not to (1) exercise strenuously for 48 hours before the experimental visit, (2) drink caffeine or (3) alcohol in the 24 hours before the experimental visit. They will also come to the laboratory in the morning after the overnight fast. A trained researcher will measure their resting heart rate (HR) and blood pressure (BP). Only participants with systolic over diastolic BP (SBP/DBP) of less than 200/110 mm Hg on the screening day will undergo the maximal exercise test because higher values are a contraindication to a maximal exercise test.75 They also must have a confirmation from their physician on a medical clearance that their BP is within a normotensive range. The anthropometric measurements will follow to ensure that the participant’s body mass index (BMI) does not exceed 40 kg/m2 due to an increased cardiovascular risk.81 If the participant’s physician cannot confirm fasting glucose levels or glycated haemoglobin levels less than below diagnostic values for type 2 diabetes in the last 12 months, a trained researcher will collect a fasting capillary blood sample using a lancet device and a point-of-care glucometer to confirm that fasting glucose levels are below 126 mg/dL. Next, participants fill in demographic information and undergo neuropsychological testing.
Neuropsychological assessments
A trained researcher administers a Montreal Cognitive Assessment to screen out individuals with scores <26 suggestive of potential cognitive impairment .82 A standardised test of cognitive abilities (Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test-2)79 will be administered next, and individuals with a score <85 (ie, 1 SD below the age-matched population) will be excluded.
Psychosocial assessments
A set of psychosocial questionnaires will be administered to allow for more accurate assessments of depressive symptoms (table 2).
Cardiorespiratory fitness testing
Participants will undergo a maximal exercise test on a cycle ergometer (Excalibur, Lode, Groningen, the Netherlands) using a modified Astrand protocol83 84 with a 12-lead ECG. The test will be supervised by a study physician who is experienced in supervising graded maximal exercise tests in older adults. This test is conducted based on the recommendations from the American College of Sports Medicine (ACSM) to evaluate participants’ physiological responses to exercise.75 Its results will be used as an inclusion criterion to enhance the safety of acute high-intensity exercise. Three trained first aid and cardiopulmonary resuscitation (CPR)-certified experimenters will conduct the test in collaboration with the study physician. Participants’ resting BP, HR and ECG readings will be collected. They will then warm up for 2 min while pedalling at the same speed of 50 revolutions per minute. Next, the workload on the cycle ergometer will be increased depending on the participant’s sex, starting at 50 watts for females and increasing every 2 min by 25 watts. Males will start at 100 watts and exercise at 50 watts increments.83 84 The participant will cycle until volitional exhaustion.83 84 Their HR and ECG are continuously monitored, and BP will be monitored every 2 min during exercise by a physician. Every 2 min, the study staff will record ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) using the Borg scale.85 Relative peak oxygen consumption will be expressed in mL/kg/min and based on maximal effort as evidenced by at least two of the following criteria75: (1) respiratory exchange ratio ≥1.1, (2) failure of the HR to increase with increasing workload (ie, ≤10 bpm increase relative to age-predicted HRmax86) or (3) RPE>17. The test finishes with a 5 min cool-down. If there are no positive findings on the ECG as described in the indications to stopping the maximal exercise test in the ACSM’s Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription,75 the individual will be cleared for participation in the study. The HRmax achieved during the test will be used to determine exercise intensity for each individual.
Baseline
Baseline assessments were designed to familiarise participants with the main intervention procedures, including cognitive tasks and HIIT and LITT bouts. Participants will first sign an informed study consent (online supplemental material 2). The consent includes provisions for the deidentified data for use in future studies. A trained researcher will assess their resting BP to verify that SBP/DBP is <200/110 mm Hg, which is a counterindication to exercise.75 Participants will then practise two LIIT and two HIIT bouts every 25 min. During each 25 min block, they will complete a questionnaire battery and practice cognitive tasks described in detail under the intervention section.
LIIT bouts
Each LIIT bout will last 6 min and comprise a 1 min warm-up (cycling at 50 rpm with no resistance), followed by two low-intensity intervals, cycling at 57%–60% of their maximum HR lasting 2 min and separated by a 1 min passive recovery (sitting on a cycle ergometer). Research assistants will continuously monitor participants’ HR and prompt the participants to adjust speed to elicit the prescribed exercise intensity. RPE ratings will be collected every minute.
HIIT bouts
Each HIIT bout will last 6 min and comprise a 1 min warm-up (cycling at 50 rpm with no resistance), followed by two high-intensity intervals separated by a 1 min passive recovery (sitting on a cycle ergometer). High-intensity intervals comprise cycling for 2 min at, on average, 90% of the participant’s individual HRmax established during the maximal exercise test on the same cycle ergometer. The workload and speed will be continuously adjusted by a trained researcher to reach the 90% HRmax. Participants’ HR will be continuously monitored by the research staff in response to exercise and 2 min after exercise to confirm the drop in HR of at least 22 beats per minute, which indicates a normal HR response after exercise.87 88 Participant’s BP is also monitored 6 min after each bout of exercise to ensure that resting BP does not exceed the <200/110 mm Hg threshold.75 Between HIIT and LIIT bouts, participants will practice cognitive tasks (described in the intervention section) and complete questionnaires.
Cognitive task practice
Participants will complete two cognitive tasks of attentional control (a modified Eriksen flanker task and the antisaccade task) during baseline to minimise practice effects observed in previous studies.89 90 The Mnemonic Similarity Task (MST) uses two parallel versions to control for practice effects. The cognitive tasks are described in detail in the intervention section.
Psychosocial questionnaires
A battery of questionnaires will be administered to provide a descriptive characterisation of the study sample in relation to their habitual leisure-time exercise, types of sedentary behaviours they engaged in and their habitual cognitive activities (table 2). In addition, data on sleep quality and sleeping habits will be collected. All these factors are related to cognitive and brain functions and will provide contextual descriptive information for the study sample.
Physical function questionnaires
The data on physical function, physical function self-efficacy, gait and disability will be collected to provide important characteristics of the study sample to contextualise the feasibility and acceptability data from this pilot trial.
Accelerometry
At the end of the baseline visit, participants will be provided with two activity monitors: an activPAL 4 micro (PAL Technologies, Glasgow, Scotland) to wear on their right thighs to monitor sitting and sit-to-stand transitions, and a GT9XLink (ActiGraph, Pensacola, Florida, USA) to wear on their wrists, which monitor PA and sleep continuously 24/7 over 1 week. Both devices record raw acceleration from tri-axial accelerometers. The activPAL uses accelerometer-derived information about thigh position and acceleration to determine body posture. It provides information on sitting/lying down time, sit-to-stand transitions, sedentary patterning (bouts and breaks) and stepping cadence. The raw acceleration recorded by the GT9XLink is translated to average acceleration, energy expenditure, steps and PA intensities used to estimate ST, light, moderate and vigorous PA (min/day). The device also measures sleep latency, efficiency, and total sleep time. In addition, participants will keep a sleep diary to record times in and out of bed, sleep and wake-up times and complete a 24-hour PA recall for the day preceding each intervention condition.91 The information from accelerometers will be used as exploratory covariates in participants’ responses to the intervention. Participants will also wear the devices for 1 week preceding the second intervention day. The data from both weeks will be compared to assess consistency in free-living physical behaviours between intervention visits.
Intervention visits
Figure 1 illustrates an experimental design. At each experimental visit, participants will engage in the same protocol except for the intensity of PA. Participants are asked to come to the laboratory after an overnight fast. At the beginning of the visit, participants are outfitted with a chest HR monitor, an accelerometer and an activPAL to monitor intervention fidelity. On coming to the laboratory, participants are outfitted with a waist-worn GT9XLink accelerometer, an activPAL and an HR monitor and asked to sit quietly for 5 min. After the rest, their resting HR and BP are collected to verify that SBP/DBP is <200/110 mm Hg. Participants are then fitted with an EEG cap. During the cap preparation, participants are provided with a light, standardised meal calibrated to their recommended caloric intake based on MyPlate (https://www.myplate.gov/) recommendations specific to age group (40–50, 50–60, 60–70 and 70–75), gender and BMI and accounting for 22% of their recommended daily energy intake.92 93 After breakfast, they complete a 24-hour PA recall (Activities Completed over Time in 24 Hours),94 Karolinska Sleepiness Scale95 and their HR and BP are measured. The PA recall is collected to confirm compliance with not engaging in high-intensity exercise 24 hours before the visit. Karolinska Sleepiness Scale will be used to explore any differences in intervention effects based on self-perceived levels of sleepiness. Then, participants will begin a 6 min rest while the EEG signal is collected. After the resting state EEG data collection, they will complete three cognitive tasks in a randomised order while the EEG signal is simultaneously collected. Then, participants will complete one intervention at each visit. Each intervention comprises a 3.5-hour sitting time interrupted every 30 min with a 6 min interval training bout of light (LIIT, a control condition) or high intensity (HIIT, an active condition). The same specifications for LIIT and HIIT bouts are used as during the baseline visit. Participants will complete five bouts per intervention, totaling 30 min of LIIT or HIIT, depending on the condition. The order of the intervention conditions will be randomised across participants such that each participant will serve as his/her own control. HR and BP are monitored and recorded two and 6 min after each break, respectively, to ensure that participants show a normal physiological response to exercise. Participants will also complete one a modified Eriksen flanker task with simultaneous EEG recordings twice during a 3.5-hour sitting, 15 min after the first and the third PA bout (figure 1), to assess the acute and cumulative effects of HIIT versus LIIT bouts on cognitive and brain function. In the last 15 min of sitting, participants will receive another standardised meal identical to the one received at the pretest. After they consume the meal, participants engage in the exact same neurocognitive assessments as during the pretest. After neurocognitive assessments in experimental visit 1, participants will receive two activity monitors to wear for a week preceding the second intervention visit. Participants complete two study surveys designed to assess intervention acceptability at the end of the second intervention visit.
Figure 1. Study design. EEG, electroencephalogram; HIIT, high-intensity interval training; HR, heart rate; LIIT, low-intensity interval training; MST, Mnemonic Similarity Task. Created in: https://BioRender.com
Sedentary activities
During the 3.5-hour sitting, participants sit continuously except for HIIT/LIIT bouts and bathroom breaks. Participants are transported to the bathroom in a wheelchair. The frequency and duration of bathroom breaks are recorded. Participants sit at a table with a laptop in the same testing room as the cycle ergometer. They will complete a standardised set of home administrative tasks (eg, planning a holiday, a birthday party) and read a standardised set of popular science articles from the New York Times. Activities will change every 30 min. Two sets of sedentary activities were developed, and their order was randomised across participants. To control for cognitive and emotional arousal, participants are asked not to use their electronic devices during the intervention. Participants are provided with plain water to drink during the 3.5-hour sitting but no food except for the two standardised meals to control for energy intake.
Mental effort, cognitive engagement and fatigue
To monitor participants’ cognitive engagement and subjective task difficulty, they will fill in Task Engagement96–98 and Cognitive Effort99 scales before each HIIT or LIIT bout. These measures were included to control for cognitive stimulation during sedentary activities. To monitor participants’ psychological arousal, we will measure the levels of perceived fatigue and vigour, they will self-report their energy, vigour and fatigue on a validated Visual Analogue Fatigue Scale before every break.100 We will also monitor participants’ perceived enjoyment of PA during each condition with Physical Activity Enjoyment Scale to inform intervention acceptability.101
Cognitive tasks
Modified Eriksen Flanker task
Inhibitory control is measured using a modified Eriksen flanker task before, after and twice during 3-hour sitting.102 The modified Eriksen flanker task provides a measure of attentional control (an aspect of inhibitory control) by introducing a perceptual and response conflict. Participants are presented with a row of five 3 cm tall arrowheads appearing in the centre of the computer screen on a black background. A participant is required to respond to the directionality of the middle arrowhead, flanked by arrowheads pointing either in the same (congruent trials) or the opposite direction (incongruent trials). Incongruent flankers introduce a perceptual conflict that must be overcome to respond correctly. Congruency and directionality are random and equiprobable. Stimuli are presented for 83 ms, followed by a 1000 ms response window and a jittered inter-trial interval (ITI) of 1100, 1300 and 1500 ms. Participants will complete two blocks of 100 trials. Behavioural measures of reaction time (RT), RT variability and accuracy for each task condition will be used as secondary outcomes. This task is sensitive to modulation with acute exercise.44 In addition, the P3b component measured during this task has shown reliable responses to a single bout of acute exercise.103 Participants complete this task before, after and twice during the intervention (figure 1).
Antisaccade task
The antisaccade task is an accuracy-based measure of attentional control and was chosen as a complementary cognitive measure to the RT-based Eriksen flanker task. It also provides a psychometrically superior evaluation of attentional control.104 Participants first fixate on a crosshair. Next, a tone signals the beginning of the trial. An asterisk appears to either the left or right of the crosshair, followed by a letter Q or O displayed opposite to the asterisk side. The participant has to look away from the asterisk in the direction of the letter. Then, the letter is masked. The participant must identify which letter (O or Q) appeared with a corresponding button press. The number of correctly identified letters is the secondary outcome. Participants complete 2 blocks of 76 trials with set ITI to 5000 ms and varied fixation time (1000, 2000 ms). Participants complete this task before and after each intervention.
Mnemonic Similarity Task
Episodic memory is measured with a computerised MST.105 106 Performance on this task is a good marker of hippocampal function105 and is sensitive to the acute effects of PA in older adults.107 An encoding phase will be administered first. Participants study 64 coloured pictures of common objects, one at a time, for 2.0 s each with 0.5 s interstimulus interval. They then indicate whether the object was an ‘indoor’ or ‘outdoor’ item. An immediate retrieval phase follows, comprising repeats, lures (similar but new objects), and new objects. Participants will indicate if objects are old or new.108 They complete a set of 192 objects. A lure discrimination index (probability of ‘similar’/’novel’ judgements in response to a lure) is another secondary outcome. Participants complete the MST task before and after 3.5-hour sitting on each intervention day.
Electroencephalogram
One of the primary outcomes of the HIIT2SITLess study is to test the effects of HIIT interruptions to prolonged sitting on the P3b-ERP component during an inhibitory control modified flanker task. Accordingly, participants are fitted with an EEG cap throughout the intervention to measure the EEG signal before and after each 3.5-hour sitting time. The EEG is recorded during a 6 min rest at pretest and post-test, followed by EEG recordings simultaneous with cognitive tasks. In addition, the EEG is recorded while the flanker task is completed twice during the 3.5-hour sitting. The EEG is measured using a 64-electrode Quik-Cap Neo Net (Compumedics, Charlotte, North Carolina, USA) with four integrated bipolar electrodes for vertical and horizontal eye movements, arranged according to the 10–10 system.
Neurofunctional measures
The P3b: The main aim of the HIIT2SITLess study is to assess the effects of HIIT interruptions to sitting on the P3b component, which is an established marker of frontoparietal brain function embedded within the stimulus-locked ERP. Both the P3b amplitude and latency have been reliably modulated by acute exercise.103 However, its responses to prolonged sitting in older adults have not yet been investigated. Accordingly, the P3b-ERP component will be measured during the flanker task at four time points (before, after and twice during each intervention) and twice during the antisaccade task (before and after the intervention).
N2 and error-related negativity
The effects of the intervention on other ERP components related to cognitive control will also be explored.109 Cognitive control can be defined as a set of mental operations implicated in selection, scheduling and coordinating information processes involved in attention, memory and action in service of a goal.110 Attentional control is part of the cognitive control system.111 The stimulus-locked N2-ERP component109 is thought to represent conflict processing. The N2 is a small negative-going component, which appears within 200–350 ms following stimulus onset and reaches a maximum over the frontal Fz and FCz electrodes.109 Larger N2 amplitudes have been observed with successful conflict resolution and fewer commission errors.112 This ERP component has been modulated by a single bout of sitting lasting 20 min in preadolescents such that a more negative N2 amplitude was observed during the flanker task (suggesting greater conflict) after a bout of sitting compared with a bout of moderate-intensity walking.113 The error-related negativity (ERN) is a response-locked negative-going component that often appears in response to commission errors and is considered a marker of conflict monitoring mediated by the dorsal portion of the anterior cingulate cortex.114 The ERN can be modulated by acute exercise,115 but its response to prolonged sitting has not been investigated. Accordingly, neurofunctional responses underlying inhibitory control, which include conflict monitoring, are measured in the HIIT2SITLess study.
Frontal N400 (FN400) and late positive component
The HIIT2SITLess study will also explore the neuroelectric correlates of pattern separation (a measure of episodic memory) using the MST. Specifically, the study will explore the intervention effects on the difference waveforms in response to old and new items (an old-new effect) presented during the MST in the ERP components studied in the context of familiarity116–118 and recollection.119 120 For example, the anterior-central negative-going FN400 component appears approximately 400 ms after stimulus onset over frontal electrodes. The positive-going late positive component (LPC) appears posteriorly approximately 600 ms after stimulus onset.121 Anterior-central FN400 is thought to index familiarity judgments because it varies with self-reported recognition confidence ratings.116 In contrast, the parietal LPC is thought to index recollection because its amplitude varies with an individual’s ability to identify a source of memory120 but not with their recognition confidence.116 Correctly identified lure items in the MST are thought to represent pattern separation, the process that reduces overlap between memory representations. This process is involved in memory recollection. In contrast, incorrectly identifying a similar item as old (lure false alarms in the MST) is thought to index pattern completion, which can rely on partial or degraded memory traces for memory retrieval, akin to recognition memory. The amplitudes of the FN400 and LPC components will be examined in response to correctly identified lures and lure false alarms during the MST.
Directional connectivity
In addition to ERPs, this study will explore changes in FC patterns during rest and task engagement in response to the HIIT interruptions to sitting. We will reconstruct cortical sources and estimate non-directional and directional (ie, effective) FC in high temporal resolution,122 123 using the weighted Minimum Norm Estimation, a gold standard of source reconstruction, together with the Directed Transfer Function,124 125 a technique that uses multivariate autoregressive modelling to estimate network dynamics over time. The effects of the intervention on FC between the regions of the FPN and the DMN at rest and during the flanker and the MST tasks will be explored. These networks have been chosen because FC in these networks declines with age,126 127 but a single bout of PA can strengthen FC in both networks.128 FC in other cognitive networks will also be explored.
End-of-study questionnaire
The HIIT2SITLess study survey
An 18-item survey developed by researchers specifically for the study. The survey includes 12 questions with answers on a 7-point Likert scale asking participants to evaluate the time commitment required for the study, engagement in HIIT and LIIT, EEG and cognitive assessments, sitting duration and sedentary behaviours that participants engaged in during the intervention. The survey also includes six open-ended questions asking participants for an explanation of their ratings and any additional comments.
The HIIT breaks survey
A 24-item survey developed specifically for the study. The survey includes 14 items measured on a 7-point Likert scale and ten open-ended items asking about the participant’s experience with the HIIT breaks, including the dose (ie, duration, frequency, intensity) and how they compare to a single bout of moderate-intensity exercise. To evaluate the potential feasibility of participants adopting similar HIIT breaks at home, four questions focus on the likelihood of adopting such breaks. The remaining open-ended questions ask about participants’ preferences for the type of exercise, duration, intensity and frequency.
Follow-ups
In addition to monitoring for adverse events by research staff during the study visits, adverse events will be monitored for 30 days immediately following the last intervention day. A study coordinator will call a week after the intervention and approximately 30 days after the study. If an adverse event is recorded, the research coordinator will follow up with a participant until the event is resolved.
Primary outcomes
Our primary outcomes for feasibility, fidelity and acceptability of the intervention are listed in table 3. The primary outcomes related to intervention effects on brain function are the change in task-evoked brain activity. Specifically, the amplitude and the latency of the P3b difference during the modified Eriksen flanker task over four measurements at the pretest, after the first and third PA bout, and at the post-test. We will use the area under the curve (AUC) to measure change.
Table 3Feasibility, missingness, fidelity and acceptability outcomes
Description | |
Feasibility | |
Recruitment rates | N randomised/N screened5 |
Retention rates | N randomised who successfully completed all conditions/N randomised5 |
Cognitive and EEG data | % of participants with fully completed preintervention and postintervention EEG recordings with >50% of valid, correct trials for each task and task condition. |
Fidelity | |
HIIT | % of high-intensity intervals at 90% HRmax |
Sitting | % of time spent sitting during each condition (out of 180 min) |
Acceptability | |
Time commitment | Number and length of visits: 1-Unacceptable; 7-Fully acceptable, Number of dropouts due to time commitment. |
HIIT | Duration; (2) Intensity; (3) Frequency; (4) cycling 1-Not acceptable, would not implement at home; to 7-Fully acceptable and would implement at home. |
EEG/cognitive measures | 1-Unacceptable; 7-Fully acceptable, Number of dropouts due to EEG measurements. |
EEG, electroencephalogram; HIIT, high-intensity interval training.
Secondary outcomes
Our secondary outcomes related to aim one focus on safety. We will measure the frequency of serious adverse events and moderate severity adverse events. An adverse event in the HIIT2SITLess trial is defined as any occurrence of an undesirable and unintended, but not necessarily unexpected, result of the HIIT or LIIT intervention or study procedures. A moderate adverse event results in a low level of inconvenience or concern with the intervention or study procedures and may cause some interference with functioning. An example of a moderate adverse event is chest pain or injury with no fracture. A serious adverse event in the HIIT2SITLess trial is defined as an event that may be harmful to the participant and/or serious enough to warrant discontinuing the study due to its intolerability or potential harm to the participant. Any adverse event that meets the standard criteria outlined in the Code of Federal Regulations (21CFR 312.32) will be classified as a serious adverse event.129 The research staff are trained on expected adverse events such as muscle soreness due to high-intensity exercise, mild discomfort or bruising due to the use of a lancet device. The staff records these events on an adverse events form. In addition, the study physician observes for any adverse events during the graded maximal exercise test. The study staff monitors participants’ responses to exercise and observes for signs and symptoms of hypoglycaemia throughout each intervention session. A description of the event is recorded by attending staff and reviewed and classified by the PI.
The secondary outcomes related to aim 3 include the differences between conditions in:
Behavioural responses during the modified Eriksen flanker task.
Behavioural responses during the antisaccade task.
Behavioural responses during the MST task.
The amplitude and the latency of the N2-ERP component during the flanker task.
The amplitude and the latency of the P3b-ERP component during the antisaccade task.
The amplitude and the latency of the N2-ERP component during the antisaccade task.
All secondary outcomes will be measured at pretest and post-test except for behavioural and neuroelectric measures from the modified Eriksen flanker task, which are measured at four time points (before, after and twice during each intervention).
Exploratory outcomes
Exploratory outcomes related to aim 3 include:
The amplitude and the latency of the ERN component during the flanker task.
The amplitude and the latency of the ERN component during the antisaccade task.
The amplitude and the latency of the FN400 component during the MST task.
The amplitude and the latency of the LPC component during the MST task.
FC within the FPN.
FC within the DMN.
FC in other than FPN and DMN canonical brain networks.
Data monitoring
Access to person-identifiable information is restricted to a research coordinator, a graduate student and study technicians. Identifiable information is kept separate from the data and maintained on REDCap, a secure web application (capable of compliance with the Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act, HIPPA) for building and maintaining study infrastructure, including surveys, collecting informed consent and building databases. Part of the identifiable information (medical clearance) is maintained on the HIPPA-compliant cloud storage service Box for Protected Health Information. All research records will be retained for 6 years on completion of the study based on the HIPAA (45 CFR 164.530(j)). Data quality is promoted by staff training, and data completeness is verified by a senior team leader at the end of each session. REDCap also provides outcome-specific range restrictions as an additional data quality check. Protocol amendments are listed in online supplemental table 4. Any further amendments will be approved by the IRB at the University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign. The Data Safety and Monitoring Board was deemed unnecessary due to the small scale of the trial. The trial receives safety oversight from an independent Safety Officer appointed by the National Institute on Aging.
Sample size determination
The study was powered for the effect of HIIT versus LIIT bouts on the pre-to-post-condition change in the P3b amplitude on a working memory task (which relies on attentional control) based on two dependent sample t-tests using G*Power V.3.1.130 Our target sample of 42 adults is based on an acute effect of a single HIIT bout on the P3b amplitude relative to baseline in middle-aged and older adults reported by Tsai et al.131 Using a one-tail test, an ⍺=0.05, we will have 91% power to detect an effect size with Cohen’s d=0.5 on preintervention to postintervention comparison. Kamijo et al132 reported Cohen’s d=1.2 for the P3b latency in older adults using a single 20 min bout of moderate intensity (expected to increase peripheral catecholamines).65 133 We will have 80% power to detect an effect of d=0.78 based on independent samples t-test (one-tailed) comparisons. To account for 20% attrition, we will recruit 54 older adults to the study.
Statistical analyses
Missing data
We will verify whether the collected data meet the missing completely at random (MCAR) assumption using Little’s test of MCAR. If this assumption holds, to account for the missing data,134 we will fit general linear mixed-effects models.135 (A violation of the MCAR assumption will prompt an investigation to identify its causes and appropriate statistical solutions.) Mixed-effects models assume that data are missing at random and implicitly account for the missing values by modelling weighted averages of condition effects, one for complete cases and one for singletons. To allow for the intention-to-treat analysis, we will estimate sequence (two levels: X, Y), condition (two levels: HIIT Breaks, LIIT Breaks) and time (four levels: prebreak, after break #1, after break #3 and post-test), and the two-way and three-way interactions between these factors; no interim analyses will be performed. If the analyses suggest no sequence interaction with time and condition, we will estimate the two-way condition by time interactions. All analyses of primary outcomes for aim 2 will be conducted on pretest, after the first and the third PA bout, and post-test assessments.135 We will test intervention effects on secondary outcomes using the pretest and post-test assessments. We will also explore the intervention effects on FC at rest and during task engagement and neurofunctional correlates of attentional control, pattern separation and completion using two time points: pretest and post-test. We will include each outcome as a response variable, sequence, condition and time as fixed effects and a participant-specific random intercept. Carryover (ie, period) effects are assumed to be null based on the sufficient washout period of 1 week between treatments.136 Results will be presented as mean differences between conditions in the AUC with one-tailed 95% CI (primary outcome) and mean differences between conditions at post-test for secondary outcomes. We will present the results as the effect sizes (Cohen’s d).137
Data sharing
During the research period, access to the data will be restricted to the researchers directly working on the project. Data that support the conclusions of the project published in peer-reviewed scientific journals will be made available to other researchers on request. Only deidentified data that support a published manuscript will be shared. All investigators involved in the development of the trial will be coauthors of any subsequent publications resulting from the trial.
Reporting guidelines
The study was designed in accordance with Standard Protocol Items: Recommendations for Interventional Trials, and its details are provided in online supplemental material.
Publication
Publication of the results of this trial will be submitted for consideration in peer-reviewed scientific journals and will be made available to participants on request.
Patient and public involvement
Patients or the public were not involved in developing this trial protocol.
Ethics and dissemination
The study has been approved by the IRB at the University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign (IRB24-0010). All participants provide informed written consent to screening procedures and separately to the study procedures. Participants are provided with a copy of the consent document before the screening and in-person visit to allow them time to review the information (online supplemental materials 1 and 2). The data collected from participants will be used for research purposes. Deidentified data can be used for future studies and training purposes.
Discussion
The HIIT2SITLess randomised crossover pilot feasibility trial was designed to assess the feasibility of HIIT as brief interruptions to prolonged sitting to enhance cognitive and brain function in middle-aged and older adults. This is a carefully designed controlled study, where PA intensity is individually tailored and carefully monitored by the research staff. The study was designed based on a theoretical premise that brief but high-intensity PA breaks to prolonged sitting can enhance function by stimulating the cerebral norepinephrine system through the abdominal vagus nerve pathway.56 57 In contrast, light-intensity PA may not yield such improvements due to too low intensity of short (< 10 min) PA bouts. We hypothesise that implementing 6 min HIIT interruptions to prolonged sitting every 30 min will be feasible and acceptable over the 3.5-hour period to middle-aged and older adults. We also hypothesise that HIIT interruptions to prolonged sitting will enhance frontoparietal function as indicated by greater P3b amplitude and shorter P3b latency of the incongruent-congruent difference waveform during the flanker task measuring inhibitory control. Furthermore, this pilot trial will allow us to explore the intervention effects on behavioural measures of inhibitory control and episodic memory and their neuroelectric correlates. The HIIT2SITLess trial goes beyond the ERP markers of brain function and seeks to explore the effects of interrupting prolonged sitting with HIIT bouts on FC in FPN and the DMN using directional FC measures. As such, this trial is the first of its kind to test the effectiveness and feasibility of HIIT as a means to reduce prolonged sitting in the population of highly sedentary adults at risk of age-related cognitive decline.
As with every trial, this trial has several limitations. Although built on a theoretical premise, the study is not designed to test the changes in central or peripheral norepinephrine to directly test this theory. However, the P3b-ERP component is considered an index of phasic shifts in the locus coeruleus activity,138 the main source of cerebral norepinephrine, because the locus coeruleus can exert a neuromodulatory effect on the P3b through its efferent cortical projections,43 which overlap with cortical generators of the P3b.17–19 HIIT is physically demanding and, therefore, entails greater risk in individuals who are at an increased risk of cardiovascular disease and those with a cardiovascular disease history. Accordingly, participating individuals must have medical clearance and show no positive findings on the graded maximal exercise test. Our study is also not designed to test the differences in other than intensity components of the PA dose (ie, PA bout frequency and duration). Nonetheless, by including two neurofunctional measures after the first and the third PA bout, we will explore the cumulative benefit of three compared with a single bout of HIIT on brain function and inhibitory control. The outcomes from the HIIT2SITLess trial will inform mechanistic models (catecholamine-driven increase in phasic locus coeruleus activity) that may underpin the effectiveness of interrupting prolonged sitting with brief PA bouts on cognitive and brain functions. The feasibility outcomes will promote the clinical applications of interrupting prolonged sitting with HIIT in highly sedentary middle-aged and older populations. The results from this study can be used to support the development of chronic interventions to test the effectiveness of reducing prolonged sitting with HIIT on brain function, structure and the underlying biological mechanisms.
Ethics statements
Patient consent for publication
Not applicable.
Contributors DMP: conceptualisation, data curation, methodology, funding acquisition, project administration, resources, supervision, writing original draft; CH, AK and NK: conceptualisation, methodology, funding acquisition, resources; MW and TSL: methodology, software; SP, JSo, JSa and MK: methodology; JK: visualisation. FBQ, RS and TS: data acquisition. All coauthors reviewed, edited and approved the final draft. DMP is the guarantor of this work.
Funding Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R21AG080411.
Disclaimer The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Neither study sponsors nor funder had no role in the design of this study and will have no role in its execution, analyses, interpretation of the data or decision to submit results.
Competing interests None declared.
Patient and public involvement Patients and/or the public were not involved in the design, or conduct, or reporting, or dissemination plans of this research.
Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Supplemental material This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.
1 World Health Organization. Ageing and health. Geneva: World Health Organization, 2024. Available: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/ageing-and-health
2 Old SR, Naveh-Benjamin M. Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: a meta-analysis. Psychol Aging 2008; 23: 104–18. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.104
3 Greene NR, Naveh-Benjamin M. Adult age-related changes in the specificity of episodic memory representations: A review and theoretical framework. Psychol Aging 2023; 38: 67–86. doi:10.1037/pag0000724
4 Simpson S, Eskandaripour M, Levine B. Effects of Healthy and Neuropathological Aging on Autobiographical Memory: A Meta-Analysis of Studies Using the Autobiographical Interview. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2023; 78: 1617–24. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbad077
5 Del Missier F, Mäntylä T, Bruine de Bruin W. Executive functions in decision making: an individual differences approach. Think Reason 2010; 16: 69–97. doi:10.1080/13546781003630117
6 Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C, Ekstrom AD, et al. A contextual binding theory of episodic memory: systems consolidation reconsidered. Nat Rev Neurosci 2019; 20: 364–75. doi:10.1038/s41583-019-0150-4
7 Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2011; 108: 3017–22. doi:10.1073/pnas.1015950108
8 Zenko Z, Willis EA, White DA. Proportion of Adults Meeting the 2018 Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans According to Accelerometers. Front Public Health 2019; 7: 135. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2019.00135
9 Spiteri K, Broom D, Bekhet AH, et al. Barriers and Motivators of Physical Activity Participation in Middle-aged and Older-adults - A Systematic Review. J Aging Phys Act 2019; 27: 929–44. doi:10.1123/japa.2018-0343
10 Matthews CE, Carlson SA, Saint-Maurice PF, et al. Sedentary Behavior in U.S. Adults: Fall 2019. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2021; 53: 2512–9. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000002751
11 Raichlen DA, Aslan DH, Sayre MK, et al. Sedentary Behavior and Incident Dementia Among Older Adults. JAMA 2023; 330: 934–40. doi:10.1001/jama.2023.15231
12 Pindus DM, Edwards CG, Walk AM, et al. The relationships between prolonged sedentary time, physical activity, cognitive control, and P3 in adults with overweight and obesity. Int J Obes (Lond) 2021; 45: 746–57. doi:10.1038/s41366-020-00734-w
13 Pindus DM, Edwards CG, Walk AM, et al. Sedentary time is related to deficits in response inhibition among adults with overweight and obesity: An accelerometry and event-related brain potentials study. Psychophysiology 2021; 58: e13843. doi:10.1111/psyp.13843
14 Han X, Song L, Li Y, et al. Accelerometer-Measured Sedentary Behavior Patterns, Brain Structure, and Cognitive Function in Dementia-Free Older Adults: A Population-Based Study. J Alzheimers Dis 2023; 96: 657–68. doi:10.3233/JAD-230575
15 Pontifex MB, Parks AC, Henning DA, et al. Single bouts of exercise selectively sustain attentional processes. Psychophysiology 2015; 52: 618–25. doi:10.1111/psyp.12395
16 Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol 2007; 118: 2128–48. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019
17 Bledowski C, Prvulovic D, Hoechstetter K, et al. Localizing P300 generators in visual target and distractor processing: a combined event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 2004; 24: 9353–60. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1897-04.2004
18 Eichele T, Specht K, Moosmann M, et al. Assessing the spatiotemporal evolution of neuronal activation with single-trial event-related potentials and functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005; 102: 17798–803. doi:10.1073/pnas.0505508102
19 Volpe U, Mucci A, Bucci P, et al. The cortical generators of P3a and P3b: a LORETA study. Brain Res Bull 2007; 73: 220–30. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.03.003
20 Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SARB, Barkhof F, et al. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006; 103: 13848–53. doi:10.1073/pnas.0601417103
21 Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Miezin FM, et al. Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007; 104: 11073–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704320104
22 Campbell KL, Grady CL, Ng C, et al. Age differences in the frontoparietal cognitive control network: implications for distractibility. Neuropsychologia 2012; 50: 2212–23. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.025
23 Borst JP, Anderson JR. Using model-based functional MRI to locate working memory updates and declarative memory retrievals in the fronto-parietal network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2013; 110: 1628–33. doi:10.1073/pnas.1221572110
24 Andrews-Hanna JR, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, et al. Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron 2007; 56: 924–35. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.038
25 Buckley RF, Schultz AP, Hedden T, et al. Functional network integrity presages cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology (ECronicon) 2017; 89: 29–37. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000004059
26 Zhao Q, Sang X, Metmer H, et al. Functional segregation of executive control network and frontoparietal network in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 2019; 120: 36–48. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2019.04.026
27 Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008; 1124: 1–38. doi:10.1196/annals.1440.011
28 Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001; 98: 676–82. doi:10.1073/pnas.98.2.676
29 Staffaroni AM, Brown JA, Casaletto KB, et al. The Longitudinal Trajectory of Default Mode Network Connectivity in Healthy Older Adults Varies As a Function of Age and Is Associated with Changes in Episodic Memory and Processing Speed. J Neurosci 2018; 38: 2809–17. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3067-17.2018
30 Damoiseaux JS, Beckmann CF, Arigita EJS, et al. Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cereb Cortex 2008; 18: 1856–64. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm207
31 Sara SJ. The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10: 211–23. doi:10.1038/nrn2573
32 Bachman SL, Dahl MJ, Werkle-Bergner M, et al. Locus coeruleus MRI contrast is associated with cortical thickness in older adults. Neurobiol Aging 2021; 100: 72–82. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2020.12.019
33 Tomassini A, Hezemans FH, Ye R, et al. Prefrontal Cortical Connectivity Mediates Locus Coeruleus Noradrenergic Regulation of Inhibitory Control in Older Adults. J Neurosci 2022; 42: 3484–93. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1361-21.2022
34 Lee TH, Greening SG, Ueno T, et al. Arousal increases neural gain via the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system in younger adults but not in older adults. Nat Hum Behav 2018; 2: 356–66. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0344-1
35 Hernaus D, Casales Santa MM, Offermann JS, et al. Noradrenaline transporter blockade increases fronto-parietal functional connectivity relevant for working memory. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2017; 27: 399–410. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.02.004
36 Hansen N, Manahan-Vaughan D. Hippocampal long-term potentiation that is elicited by perforant path stimulation or that occurs in conjunction with spatial learning is tightly controlled by beta-adrenoreceptors and the locus coeruleus. Hippocampus 2015; 25: 1285–98. doi:10.1002/hipo.22436
37 Gibbs ME, Hutchinson DS, Summers RJ. Noradrenaline release in the locus coeruleus modulates memory formation and consolidation; roles for α- and β-adrenergic receptors. Neuroscience 2010; 170: 1209–22. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.07.052
38 Dahl MJ, Mather M, Düzel S, et al. Rostral locus coeruleus integrity is associated with better memory performance in older adults. Nat Hum Behav 2019; 3: 1203–14. doi:10.1038/s41562-019-0715-2
39 Yebra M, Galarza-Vallejo A, Soto-Leon V, et al. Action boosts episodic memory encoding in humans via engagement of a noradrenergic system. Nat Commun 2019; 10: 3534. doi:10.1038/s41467-019-11358-8
40 Clewett D, Sakaki M, Nielsen S, et al. Noradrenergic mechanisms of arousal’s bidirectional effects on episodic memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2017; 137: 1–14. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2016.10.017
41 Zerbi V, Floriou-Servou A, Markicevic M, et al. Rapid Reconfiguration of the Functional Connectome after Chemogenetic Locus Coeruleus Activation. Neuron 2019; 103: 702–18. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.034
42 Oyarzabal EA, Hsu L-M, Das M, et al. Chemogenetic stimulation of tonic locus coeruleus activity strengthens the default mode network. Sci Adv 2022; 8: eabm9898. doi:10.1126/sciadv.abm9898
43 Poe GR, Foote S, Eschenko O, et al. Locus coeruleus: a new look at the blue spot. Nat Rev Neurosci 2020; 21: 644–59. doi:10.1038/s41583-020-0360-9
44 Kao SC, Cadenas-Sanchez C, Shigeta TT, et al. A systematic review of physical activity and cardiorespiratory fitness on P3b. Psychophysiology 2020; 57: e13425. doi:10.1111/psyp.13425
45 Hyodo K, Dan I, Suwabe K, et al. Acute moderate exercise enhances compensatory brain activation in older adults. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33: 2621–32. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.022
46 Weng TB, Pierce GL, Darling WG, et al. The Acute Effects of Aerobic Exercise on the Functional Connectivity of Human Brain Networks. Brain Plast 2017; 2: 171–90. doi:10.3233/BPL-160039
47 Ai J-Y, Chen F-T, Hsieh S-S, et al. The Effect of Acute High-Intensity Interval Training on Executive Function: A Systematic Review. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2021; 18: 3593. doi:10.3390/ijerph18073593
48 Suwabe K, Hyodo K, Byun K, et al. Acute moderate exercise improves mnemonic discrimination in young adults. Hippocampus 2017; 27: 229–34. doi:10.1002/hipo.22695
49 Bari A, Xu S, Pignatelli M, et al. Differential attentional control mechanisms by two distinct noradrenergic coeruleo-frontal cortical pathways. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2020; 117: 29080–9. doi:10.1073/pnas.2015635117
50 Counts SE, Mufson EJ. Chapter 12 - Locus coeruleus. In: Mai JK, Paxinos G, eds. The human nervous system. 3rd edn. San Diego: Academic Press, 2012: 425–38.
51 Berridge CW, Waterhouse BD. The locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system: modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2003; 42: 33–84. doi:10.1016/s0165-0173(03)00143-7
52 Marcyniuk B, Mann DMA, Yates PO. The topography of nerve cell loss from the locus caeruleus in elderly persons. Neurobiol Aging 1989; 10: 5–9. doi:10.1016/s0197-4580(89)80004-1
53 Betts MJ, Cardenas-Blanco A, Kanowski M, et al. Locus coeruleus MRI contrast is reduced in Alzheimer’s disease dementia and correlates with CSF Aβ levels. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2019; 11: 281–5. doi:10.1016/j.dadm.2019.02.001
54 Wadley AJ, Chen Y-W, Lip GYH, et al. Low volume-high intensity interval exercise elicits antioxidant and anti-inflammatory effects in humans. J Sports Sci 2016; 34: 1–9. doi:10.1080/02640414.2015.1035666
55 Kilian Y, Engel F, Wahl P, et al. Markers of biological stress in response to a single session of high-intensity interval training and high-volume training in young athletes. Eur J Appl Physiol 2016; 116: 2177–86. doi:10.1007/s00421-016-3467-y
56 McMorris T, Turner A, Hale BJ, et al. Beyond the catecholamines hypothesis for an acute exercise–cognition interaction: a neurochemical perspective. In: Exercise-cognition interaction: neuroscience perspectives. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press, 2016: 65–103.
57 McMorris T. The acute exercise-cognition interaction: From the catecholamines hypothesis to an interoception model. Int J Psychophysiol 2021; 170: 75–88. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.10.005
58 Kao S-C, Westfall DR, Soneson J, et al. Comparison of the acute effects of high-intensity interval training and continuous aerobic walking on inhibitory control. Psychophysiology 2017; 54: 1335–45. doi:10.1111/psyp.12889
59 Kujach S, Byun K, Hyodo K, et al. A transferable high-intensity intermittent exercise improves executive performance in association with dorsolateral prefrontal activation in young adults. Neuroimage 2018; 169: 117–25. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.003
60 Diaz KM, Howard VJ, Hutto B, et al. Patterns of Sedentary Behavior in US Middle-Age and Older Adults: The REGARDS Study. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2016; 48: 430–8. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000000792
61 Charlett OP, Morari V, Bailey DP. Impaired postprandial glucose and no improvement in other cardiometabolic responses or cognitive function by breaking up sitting with bodyweight resistance exercises: a randomised crossover trial. J Sports Sci 2021; 39: 792–800. doi:10.1080/02640414.2020.1847478
62 Duvivier BMFM, Schaper NC, Koster A, et al. Benefits of Substituting Sitting with Standing and Walking in Free-Living Conditions for Cardiometabolic Risk Markers, Cognition and Mood in Overweight Adults. Front Physiol 2017; 8: 353. doi:10.3389/fphys.2017.00353
63 Wennberg P, Boraxbekk C-J, Wheeler M, et al. Acute effects of breaking up prolonged sitting on fatigue and cognition: a pilot study. BMJ Open 2016; 6: e009630. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009630
64 Kjaer M. Epinephrine and some other hormonal responses to exercise in man: with special reference to physical training. Int J Sports Med 1989; 10: 2–15. doi:10.1055/s-2007-1024866
65 Zouhal H, Jacob C, Delamarche P, et al. Catecholamines and the effects of exercise, training and gender. Sports Med 2008; 38: 401–23. doi:10.2165/00007256-200838050-00004
66 Kjaer M, Christensen NJ, Sonne B, et al. Effect of exercise on epinephrine turnover in trained and untrained male subjects. J Appl Physiol (1985) 1985; 59: 1061–7. doi:10.1152/jappl.1985.59.4.1061
67 Voss MW, Weng TB, Narayana-Kumanan K, et al. Acute Exercise Effects Predict Training Change in Cognition and Connectivity. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2020; 52: 131–40. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000002115
68 Keating CJ, Párraga Montilla JÁ, Latorre Román PÁ, et al. Comparison of High-Intensity Interval Training to Moderate-Intensity Continuous Training in Older Adults: A Systematic Review. J Aging Phys Act 2020; 28: 798–807. doi:10.1123/japa.2019-0111
69 Xu F, Cohen SA, Greaney ML, et al. Longitudinal Sex-Specific Physical Function Trends by Age, Race/Ethnicity, and Weight Status. J Am Geriatr Soc 2020; 68: 2270–8. doi:10.1111/jgs.16638
70 Ahmed FS, McMillan TM, Guenther BA, et al. Cognitive Performance following Single- or Multi-Session Exercise Intervention in Middle Age: A Systematic Review. Exp Aging Res 2024; 50: 28–64. doi:10.1080/0361073X.2022.2137360
71 Hötting K, Reich B, Holzschneider K, et al. Differential cognitive effects of cycling versus stretching/coordination training in middle-aged adults. Health Psychol 2012; 31: 145–55. doi:10.1037/a0025371
72 Iso-Markku P, Aaltonen S, Kujala UM, et al. Physical Activity and Cognitive Decline Among Older Adults: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. JAMA Netw Open 2024; 7: e2354285. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2023.54285
73 SAS Institute Inc. SAS/stat® 15.2 user’s guide. Cary, NC: SAS Institute INC, 2020. Available: https://documentation.sas.com/doc/en/statug/15.2/titlepage.htm
74 Knopman DS, Roberts RO, Geda YE, et al. Validation of the telephone interview for cognitive status-modified in subjects with normal cognition, mild cognitive impairment, or dementia. Neuroepidemiology 2010; 34: 34–42. doi:10.1159/000255464
75 Liguori G, Feito Y, Fountaine C, et al. ACSM’s guidelines for exercise testing and prescription. 11th edn. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer, 2022.
76 Crawford JR, Henry JD, Crombie C, et al. Normative data for the HADS from a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol 2001; 40: 429–34. doi:10.1348/014466501163904
77 Zablotsky B, Weeks JD, Terlizzi EP, et al. Assessing Anxiety and Depression: A Comparison of National Health Interview Survey Measures. Natl Health Stat Report 2022; 172: 1–17. doi:10.15620/cdc:117491
78 Lee B, Wang Y, Carlson SA, et al. National, State-Level, and County-Level Prevalence Estimates of Adults Aged ≥18 Years Self-Reporting a Lifetime Diagnosis of Depression - United States, 2020. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 2023; 72: 644–50. doi:10.15585/mmwr.mm7224a1
79 Blakesley RE, Mazumdar S, Dew MA, et al. Comparisons of methods for multiple hypothesis testing in neuropsychological research. Neuropsychology 2009; 23: 255–64. doi:10.1037/a0012850
80 Warburton DER, Jamnik VK, Bredin SSD, et al. The physical activity readiness questionnaire for everyone (PAR-Q+) and electronic physical activity readiness medical examination (ePARmed-X+). Health Fit J Canada 2011; 4: 3–17. doi:10.14288/hfjc.v4i2.103
81 Piché M-E, Poirier P, Lemieux I, et al. Overview of Epidemiology and Contribution of Obesity and Body Fat Distribution to Cardiovascular Disease: An Update. Prog Cardiovasc Dis 2018; 61: 103–13. doi:10.1016/j.pcad.2018.06.004
82 Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53: 695–9. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x
83 Åstrand PO. Work tests with the Bicycle Ergometer Varberg. Sweden: AB Cykelfabriken Monark, 1965.
84 Bayles MP. Cardiorespiratory fitness assessment. In: Riebe D, Liguori G, Ehrman JK, et al, eds. ACSM’s exercise testing and prescription. 10th edn. Philadelphia, PA: Wolters Kluwer, 2018: 58–83.
85 Borg G. Borg’s perceived exertion and pain scales. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1998: 104, Viii. Available: https://uiuclib.on.worldcat.org/oclc/38002592
86 Talukdar T, Román FJ, Operskalski JT, et al. Individual differences in decision making competence revealed by multivariate fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 2018; 39: 2664–72. doi:10.1002/hbm.24032
87 Fletcher GF, Ades PA, Kligfield P, et al. Exercise standards for testing and training: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation 2013; 128: 873–934. doi:10.1161/CIR.0b013e31829b5b44
88 Shetler K, Marcus R, Froelicher VF, et al. Heart rate recovery: validation and methodologic issues. J Am Coll Cardiol 2001; 38: 1980–7. doi:10.1016/s0735-1097(01)01652-7
89 White N, Flannery L, McClintock A, et al. Repeated computerized cognitive testing: Performance shifts and test-retest reliability in healthy older adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2019; 41: 179–91. doi:10.1080/13803395.2018.1526888
90 White N, Forsyth B, Lee A, et al. Repeated computerized cognitive testing: Performance shifts and test-retest reliability in healthy young adults. Psychol Assess 2018; 30: 539–49. doi:10.1037/pas0000503
91 Kirkpatrick SI, Subar AF, Douglass D, et al. Performance of the Automated Self-Administered 24-hour Recall relative to a measure of true intakes and to an interviewer-administered 24-h recall. Am J Clin Nutr 2014; 100: 233–40. doi:10.3945/ajcn.114.083238
92 Islam H, Townsend LK, McKie GL, et al. Potential involvement of lactate and interleukin-6 in the appetite-regulatory hormonal response to an acute exercise bout. J Appl Physiol (1985) 2017; 123: 614–23. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00218.2017
93 Vanderheyden LW, McKie GL, Howe GJ, et al. Greater lactate accumulation following an acute bout of high-intensity exercise in males suppresses acylated ghrelin and appetite postexercise. J Appl Physiol (1985) 2020; 128: 1321–8. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00081.2020
94 Matthews CE, Kozey Keadle S, Moore SC, et al. Measurement of Active and Sedentary Behavior in Context of Large Epidemiologic Studies. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2018; 50: 266–76. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000001428
95 Akerstedt T, Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci 1990; 52: 29–37. doi:10.3109/00207459008994241
96 Crawford ER, LePine JA, Buckman BR. Job engagement scale short form [unpublished manuscript]. Iowa City: University of Iowa, 2013.
97 Newton DW, LePine JA, Kim JK, et al. Taking engagement to task: The nature and functioning of task engagement across transitions. J Appl Psychol 2020; 105: 1–18. doi:10.1037/apl0000428
98 Rich BL, Lepine JA, Crawford ER. Job engagement: antecedents and effects on job performance. AMJ 2010; 53: 617–35. doi:10.5465/amj.2010.51468988
99 Paas F. Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: a cognitive-load approach. J Educ Psychol 1992; 84: 429–34. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.84.4.429
100 Lee KA, Hicks G, Nino-Murcia G. Validity and reliability of a scale to assess fatigue. Psychiatry Res 1991; 36: 291–8. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(91)90027-m
101 Kendzierski D, DeCarlo KJ. Physical activity enjoyment scale: two validation studies. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1991; 13: 50–64. doi:10.1123/jsep.13.1.50
102 Hillman CH, Motl RW, Pontifex MB, et al. Physical activity and cognitive function in a cross-section of younger and older community-dwelling individuals. Health Psychol 2006; 25: 678–87. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.678
103 Kao S-C, Chen F-T, Moreau D, et al. Acute effects of exercise engagement on neurocognitive function: a systematic review and meta-analysis on P3 amplitude and latency. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol 2022; 1–43. doi:10.1080/1750984X.2022.2155488
104 Draheim C, Tsukahara JS, Martin JD, et al. A toolbox approach to improving the measurement of attention control. J Exp Psychol Gen 2021; 150: 242–75. doi:10.1037/xge0000783
105 Stark SM, Kirwan CB, Stark CEL. Mnemonic Similarity Task: A Tool for Assessing Hippocampal Integrity. Trends Cogn Sci 2019; 23: 938–51. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2019.08.003
106 Yassa MA, Mattfeld AT, Stark SM, et al. Age-related memory deficits linked to circuit-specific disruptions in the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2011; 108: 8873–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.1101567108
107 Callow DD, Pena GS, Stark CEL, et al. Effects of acute aerobic exercise on mnemonic discrimination performance in older adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2023; 29: 519–28. doi:10.1017/S1355617722000492
108 Stark SM, Yassa MA, Lacy JW, et al. A task to assess behavioral pattern separation (BPS) in humans: Data from healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 2013; 51: 2442–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.12.014
109 Folstein JR, Van Petten C. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology 2008; 45: 152–70. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x
110 Braver TS, Barch DM. A theory of cognitive control, aging cognition, and neuromodulation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2002; 26: 809–17. doi:10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00067-2
111 Miyake A, Friedman NP. The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2012; 21: 8–14. doi:10.1177/0963721411429458
112 Falkenstein M, Hoormann J, Hohnsbein J. ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychol (Amst) 1999; 101: 267–91. doi:10.1016/s0001-6918(99)00008-6
113 Drollette ES, Scudder MR, Raine LB, et al. Acute exercise facilitates brain function and cognition in children who need it most: an ERP study of individual differences in inhibitory control capacity. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2014; 7: 53–64. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2013.11.001
114 Yeung N, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The Neural Basis of Error Detection: Conflict Monitoring and the Error-Related Negativity. Psychol Rev 2004; 111: 931–59. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.931
115 Pontifex MB, Parks AC, Delli Paoli AG, et al. The effect of acute exercise for reducing cognitive alterations associated with individuals high in anxiety. Int J Psychophysiol 2021; 167: 47–56. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.06.008
116 Paller KA, Voss JL, Boehm SG. Validating neural correlates of familiarity. Trends Cogn Sci 2007; 11: 243–50. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.002
117 Curran T, Hancock J. The FN400 indexes familiarity-based recognition of faces. Neuroimage 2007; 36: 464–71. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.016
118 Addante RJ, Ranganath C, Yonelinas AP. Examining ERP correlates of recognition memory: evidence of accurate source recognition without recollection. Neuroimage 2012; 62: 439–50. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.031
119 Paller KA, Kutas M. Brain Potentials during Memory Retrieval Provide Neurophysiological Support for the Distinction between Conscious Recollection and Priming. J Cogn Neurosci 1992; 4: 375–92. doi:10.1162/jocn.1992.4.4.375
120 Wilding EL. In what way does the parietal ERP old/new effect index recollection? Int J Psychophysiol 2000; 35: 81–7. doi:10.1016/s0167-8760(99)00095-1
121 Anderson ML, James JR, Kirwan CB. An event-related potential investigation of pattern separation and pattern completion processes. Cogn Neurosci 2017; 8: 9–23. doi:10.1080/17588928.2016.1195804
122 Spadone S, Wyczesany M, Della Penna S, et al. Directed Flow of Beta Band Communication During Reorienting of Attention Within the Dorsal Attention Network. Brain Connect 2021; 11: 717–24. doi:10.1089/brain.2020.0885
123 Mantini D, Della Penna S, Marzetti L, et al. A signal-processing pipeline for magnetoencephalography resting-state networks. Brain Connect 2011; 1: 49–59. doi:10.1089/brain.2011.0001
124 Hauk O. Keep it simple: a case for using classical minimum norm estimation in the analysis of EEG and MEG data. Neuroimage 2004; 21: 1612–21. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.018
125 Hämäläinen MS, Ilmoniemi RJ. Interpreting magnetic fields of the brain: minimum norm estimates. Med Biol Eng Comput 1994; 32: 35–42. doi:10.1007/BF02512476
126 Geerligs L, Renken RJ, Saliasi E, et al. A Brain-Wide Study of Age-Related Changes in Functional Connectivity. Cereb Cortex 2015; 25: 1987–99. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu012
127 Malagurski B, Liem F, Oschwald J, et al. Functional dedifferentiation of associative resting state networks in older adults - A longitudinal study. Neuroimage 2020; 214: 116680. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116680
128 Chao S-H, Liao Y-T, Chen VC-H, et al. Correlation between brain circuit segregation and obesity. Behav Brain Res 2018; 337: 218–27. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.017
129 Investigational new drug application. Title 21, Code of federal regulations, part 312. 2010. 2010. Available: https://www.ecfr.gov/current/title-21/chapter-I/subchapter-D/part-312
130 Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, et al. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 2007; 39: 175–91. doi:10.3758/bf03193146
131 Tsai C-L, Pan C-Y, Tseng Y-T, et al. Acute effects of high-intensity interval training and moderate-intensity continuous exercise on BDNF and irisin levels and neurocognitive performance in late middle-aged and older adults. Behav Brain Res 2021; 413: 113472. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113472
132 Kamijo K, Hayashi Y, Sakai T, et al. Acute effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive function in older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2009; 64: 356–63. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbp030
133 Galbo H, Holst JJ, Christensen NJ. Glucagon and plasma catecholamine responses to graded and prolonged exercise in man. J Appl Physiol 1975; 38: 70–6. doi:10.1152/jappl.1975.38.1.70
134 Eldridge SM, Chan CL, Campbell MJ, et al. CONSORT 2010 statement: extension to randomised pilot and feasibility trials. BMJ 2016; 355: i5239. doi:10.1136/bmj.i5239
135 Jones B, Kenward MG. Chapter 3. Higher-order designs for two treatments. In: Design and analysis of cross-over trials. 3rd edn. New York, NY, US: Chapman and Hall/CRC, 2014: 105–34.
136 Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, et al. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Res 2012; 1453: 87–101. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068
137 Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd edn. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis Group, 1988.
138 Nieuwenhuis S, Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. Psychol Bull 2005; 131: 510–32. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.510
139 Keadle SK, Patel S, Berrigan D, et al. Validation of ACT24 Version 2.0 for Estimating Behavioral Domains, Active and Sedentary Time. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2023; 55: 1054–62. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000003135
140 Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev 1988; 8: 77–100. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5
141 Jurca R, Jackson AS, LaMonte MJ, et al. Assessing cardiorespiratory fitness without performing exercise testing. Am J Prev Med 2005; 29: 185–93. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2005.06.004
142 Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 1991; 14: 540–5. doi:10.1093/sleep/14.6.540
143 Szabo AN, Mullen SP, White SM, et al. Longitudinal invariance and construct validity of the abbreviated late-life function and disability instrument in healthy older adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2011; 92: 785–91. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2010.12.033
144 Jette AM, Haley SM, Kooyoomjian JT. Late-life FDI manual. Boston, MA: Boston University, 2002: 73. Available: https://www.bu.edu/sph/files/2011/06/LLFDI_Manual_2006_rev.pdf
145 McAuley E, Konopack JF, Motl RW, et al. Measuring disability and function in older women: psychometric properties of the late-life function and disability instrument. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2005; 60: 901–9. doi:10.1093/gerona/60.7.901
146 McAuley E, Wójcicki TR, White SM, et al. Physical activity, function, and quality of life: design and methods of the FlexToBa trial. Contemp Clin Trials 2012; 33: 228–36. doi:10.1016/j.cct.2011.10.002
147 Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin Gerontol J Aging Ment Health 1986; 5: 165–73. doi:10.1300/J018v05n01_09
148 Godin G. The Godin-Shephard leisure-time physical activity questionnaire. Health Fit J Canada 1969; 4: 18–22. doi:10.14288/hfjc.v4i1.82
149 Amireault S, Godin G. The Godin-Shephard leisure-time physical activity questionnaire: validity evidence supporting its use for classifying healthy adults into active and insufficiently active categories. Percept Mot Skills 2015; 120: 604–22. doi:10.2466/03.27.PMS.120v19x7
150 Kenny RA, Coen RF, Frewen J, et al. Normative values of cognitive and physical function in older adults: findings from the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing. J Am Geriatr Soc 2013; 61 Suppl 2: S279–90. doi:10.1111/jgs.12195
151 Ekkekakis P, Hall EE, Petruzzello SJ. Some like it vigorous: measuring individual differences in the preference for and tolerance of exercise intensity. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005; 27: 350–74. doi:10.1123/jsep.27.3.350
152 Rosenberg DE, Norman GJ, Wagner N, et al. Reliability and validity of the Sedentary Behavior Questionnaire (SBQ) for adults. J Phys Act Health 2010; 7: 697–705. doi:10.1123/jpah.7.6.697
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2025 Author(s) (or their employer(s)) 2025. Re-use permitted under CC BY-NC. No commercial re-use. See rights and permissions. Published by BMJ Group. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited, appropriate credit is given, any changes made indicated, and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ . Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Introduction
Excessive sedentary time (ST) is linked to dementia risk, poorer attentional control and episodic memory. These cognitive decrements have been associated with decreased functional connectivity (FC) in the frontoparietal network (FPN) and default mode networks (DMN) with ageing. Physical activity (PA) interventions can enhance FC in these networks, but these interventions are not designed to decrease ST among older adults. Prolonged sitting (ie, sitting continuously for ≥20 min) can acutely reduce frontoparietal brain function and attentional control, while a single PA bout lasting at least 20 min can enhance them. It has been theorised that stimulation of the cerebral norepinephrine release through peripheral increase in catecholamines may explain this effect. In contrast, the effects of shorter (<10 min) PA bouts used to interrupt prolonged sitting on neurocognitive functions remain poorly understood. This pilot randomised crossover feasibility trial capitalises on PA intensity as the major limiting factor in peripheral catecholamine increase and tests the effects of interrupting prolonged sitting every 30 min with 6 min high-intensity interval training (HIIT) compared with low-intensity interval training (LIIT) bouts. The study will address three aims: (1) to assess feasibility, acceptability, fidelity and safety of HIIT breaks to improve neurocognitive function in middle-aged and older adults; (2) to quantify the differences between conditions in the change in the amplitude and latency of the P3b component of event-related potentials (a marker for frontoparietal function) and (3) to explore the differences between conditions in attentional control, episodic memory and FC of the FPN and DMN in middle-aged and older adults.
Methods and analysis
54 healthy adults, aged 40–75 years, will be recruited from the local community and randomly assigned to a condition sequence (HIIT, LIIT vs LIIT and HIIT). Each HIIT bout comprises a 1 min warm-up, 2 min at 90% of the maximum heart rate (HRmax), 1 min passive rest and 2 min at 90% HRmax. During 2 min intervals in LIIT, participants exercise at 57%–60% of HRmax. The primary outcomes include the feasibility (recruitment and retention rates, percentage of valid electroencephalogram data), acceptability of time commitment, HIIT bouts and neurocognitive assessments, fidelity (the intensity of HIIT breaks, percentage of time spent sitting) and the amplitude and the latency of the P3b component of event-related brain potentials measured during the modified Eriksen flanker task at pretests, after the first and the third PA bout and at post-test. General linear mixed-effects models will be used to test the effects of the intervention on the P3b component.
Ethics and dissemination
The Institutional Review Board at the University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign provided the ethical approval for the study. Findings will be disseminated in peer-reviewed journals and at scientific conferences.
Trial registration number
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details


1 Department of Health and Kinesiology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign College of Applied Health Sciences, Urbana, Illinois, USA; Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA; Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA
2 Department of Health and Kinesiology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign College of Applied Health Sciences, Urbana, Illinois, USA; University of Vermont Larner College of Medicine, Burlington, Vermont, USA; Evergreen Sports Medicine, Williston, Vermont, USA
3 Department of Health and Kinesiology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign College of Applied Health Sciences, Urbana, Illinois, USA; Department and Urgent Care VA, Hospital Medicine, Danville, Illinois, UK
4 Insitute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Krakow, Małopolskie, Poland
5 Interdisciplinary Health Sciences Institute, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA
6 Department of Health and Kinesiology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign College of Applied Health Sciences, Urbana, Illinois, USA
7 Department of Health and Kinesiology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign College of Applied Health Sciences, Urbana, Illinois, USA; Department of Psychology, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA
8 Department of Health and Kinesiology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign College of Applied Health Sciences, Urbana, Illinois, USA; The School of Cellular and Molecular Biology, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA
9 Center for Cognitive & Brain Health, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
10 Department of Health and Kinesiology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign College of Applied Health Sciences, Urbana, Illinois, USA; Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA; Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA; Division of Nutritional Sciences, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA
11 Center for Cognitive & Brain Health, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA; Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA; Department of Physical Therapy, Movement, & Rehabilitation Sciences, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
12 Center for Cognitive & Brain Health, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA; Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA