Introduction
Diabetes mellitus increases the risk of falls in older adults due to reduced sensorimotor function [1] that reduces the accuracy of foot adjustments in response to external hazards such as obstacles [2]. It is reported that older adults with type 2 diabetes (diabetes mellitus) demonstrated worse balance, gait, and functionality, contributing to an increased fall risk compared to those without diabetes [3]. Individuals with diabetic peripheral neuropathy exhibit reduced sensory input, impaired motor control, and slower functional mobility, further elevating their risk of falling [3]. Additionally, even older adults with type 2 diabetes who do not have peripheral neuropathy may still face heightened fall risks due to subtle declines in sensory functions (somatosensory, visual, vestibular), metabolic muscle function, and cognitive performance [4]. Over 50% of older adults with diabetes report one injurious fall or two non-injurious falls annually [5]. Each year, an estimated 37.3 million falls worldwide result in injuries requiring medical attention [6]. Falls are the leading cause of injury-related hospitalisations in older Australians [7]. The most frequent injuries that result from falls are fractures followed by open wounds [7]. Fall-related injuries in people with diabetes are more serious than in those without diabetes [8]; therefore, fall prevention programs to reduce the incidence of falls in older people with diabetes are of particular importance.
Visual feedback for training more accurate foot landing adjustments to external hazards is a potential intervention to prevent falls. Targeted biofeedback was used to reduce falls in older adults and patients with chronic stroke and Parkinson’s disease by training step length and minimum toe clearance (MTC) adjustments in response to virtual targets during treadmill walking [9]. Therefore, gait adaptability involves adjusting the movement of the landing foot in response to goal-oriented tasks such as stepping on projected targets [10,11]. The training program provided feedback by directly comparing foot displacement with the position of targets on the ground [10], or by comparing visualised foot displacement and targets on a monitor installed in front of the treadmill (non-immersive visual reality) [11].
Reduced accuracy of foot landing adjustments in older people has been investigated, and the varying results may reveal several methodological issues [12,13]. Despite the influence of time-constrained conditions, it is unlikely that all participants were free from age-related conditions that might affect their responses to tasks. Hence, it is important to investigate foot-landing adjustments in cohorts whose characteristics are homogenous. Aging without or with diabetes mellitus may impair vision, cognition, and peripheral nerves [14]. Reduced accuracy of foot landing adjustments may place adults with age-related conditions at increased risk of falling when negotiating unexpected hazards. When tasked to step on a virtual target and increase their step length, older adults increased the number of steps (by one), shortened their step lengths and spent more time in double support, compared with young adults [15]. This adaptation by older adults results in poorer stepping accuracy and failure to adjust steps to meet stepping targets and avoid obstacles. Similar trends have been reported in research studies with the involvement of older people with stroke and diabetes mellitus [2,16,17]. Although the development of peripheral neuropathy can be responsible for a reduced accuracy in foot landing adjustments when responding to goal-oriented tasks in people with diabetes mellitus [11,18,19], no study has been found to investigate the effectiveness of meaningful visual feedback of spatial gait parameters in this population.
The use of visual feedback to train more accurate foot landing adjustments in individuals with diabetes mellitus without neuropathy also remained relatively unexplored; however, some investigators reported that diabetes mellitus in older people impairs the accuracy of foot landing adjustments in the sagittal plane [2]. This study aimed to present the application of a new computer system for training foot landing adjustments with visual feedback. If successful, this application could be used to prevent falls in older people living with diabetes.
The hypotheses tested in this study were (i) diabetes mellitus would impair spatiotemporal gait parameters, (ii) individuals with diabetes mellitus would be able to use visual feedback (meaningful information) and immediately increase the accuracy of their foot adjustments, and (iii) diabetes mellitus would reduce the accuracy of foot landing adjustments in an older group with diabetes compared with matched healthy older and young groups.
Materials and methods
Study design
A cross-sectional study design was used to investigate the effects of diabetes mellitus on foot displacement adjustments in the sagittal plane in response to visual feedback using a more developed visual feedback system [20].
Setting
All procedures were undertaken at the Biomechanics Laboratory at Victoria University, located in metropolitan Melbourne, Australia. The Human Research Ethics Committee reviewed the proposal and approved it (HRE17‐194).
Sample size calculation
A priori power calculation was conducted using the results of a study [12] and software [21]. It was estimated that 48 participants in three groups would be required to detect an effect size of 0.66 with 95% power and a significant level of 0.05.
Recruitment of participants
Recruitment of participants was undertaken in the western suburbs in western metropolitan Melbourne, Australia, near the university campus for ease of access to participants. Eligibility criteria included aged between 18 and 40 years for young adults and aged between 65 and 85 years for older adults; free from any musculoskeletal injury at the time of testing; free from uncorrected vision issues; able to walking without any walking aids including assistive devices; no history of falling within a year before participating in the study; free from any diabetes and ageing-related neuropathy, and able to understand English. Exclusion criteria included vision impairment (less than 20/40 based on eye chart examination); stroke; heart problems; physical injuries; a history of fall within a year before participating in the study; medications that may impact balance (antidepressants, anti-anxiety drugs, blood pressure medications, sleep aids); neuropathy (Michigan Neuropathy Screening Instrument (MNSI) [22] scores of 3 and over), and dementia (Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [23] scores of less than 27).
Experimental setup and equipment
Retroreflective markers were attached to the toe of each shoe. Toe marker trajectory data was collected by a three-dimensional motion analysis (VICON, Oxford, UK). For visualisation of real-time step length and MTC and targets on a monitor, a customised MATLAB program (MathWorks, Natick, USA) used the data by VICON streamed by a Visual3D-Server software (C-Motion, Germantown, USA). This study used a visual feedback system [20] with the addition of visualisation of the MTC in each step in the current study to enhance the accuracy of foot displacement adjustments in the sagittal plane.
Procedure
Participants first completed a 5–10-minute treadmill walking when wearing a safety harness (familiarisation session). A preferred walking speed was determined for each participant [24]. In the baseline condition, participants walked for 10 minutes at their preferred speed without visual feedback (the monitor was off). Baseline walking trials were used to determine spatiotemporal gait parameters. Four targets were defined based on each participant’s baseline step length and MTC. Two step-length targets were 10% shorter or longer than baseline mean step length and two MTC targets were 3.5 cm or 5.5 cm higher than baseline mean MTC (high MTC target and higher MTC target).
During the practice session, the real-time graphs and points of interest for step length and MTC and virtual targets were illustrated on the monitor (Fig 1). Every ten steps (resting time), one in four targets was presented on the monitor and stayed during the next ten steps. Each target was triggered three times during a total of 240 steps. However, targets were removed 12 times during 10 step-blocks of walking. In total, each participant walked without any target for 120 steps and responded to four targets during 120 steps. The ten-step blocks without any targets let participants to go back to their mean baseline step length and MTC.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
The participant used visual feedback on the monitor to match the peaks of step-length graphs with the target (the horizontal line). The Y-axis shows the real-time length of each step and a target during four steps.
Date analysis
Only, 120 steps in response to four targets were analysed. Step adjustments were grouped into two error measures. The reactive error (Step 1) for each target was computed as the mean errors observed during the first step of each 10-step block when a target appeared in the last two steps of a 10-step block without a target. However, the online correction error (Steps 2–10) was calculated as the mean errors observed during steps 2–10, corresponding to the period when the target stayed visible, and participants used visual feedback to match their step length or MTC height with the presented target on the monitor (Fig 1). These two errors (accuracy) were compared across participants and among groups.
All statistical analyses were performed in SPSS (Version 25 for Windows, SPSS Science, Chicago, USA) at a significance level of α = 0.05. Three-way ANOVA tests compared the gait and foot landing adjustments between groups (Group I, Group II, Group III) and between conditions (Step 1 and Steps 2–10). Nonparametric tests (Kruskal-Wallis H and Mann-Whitney U) were used when data were not normally distributed.
Results
Forty-eight volunteers,16 young adults (Group I), 16 healthy older adults (Group II), and 16 older adults with diabetes (Group III) participated. Before foot landing adjustment tests, all older participants with diabetes had a mean glycated haemoglobin A1c (GHbA1c) of 7.6% ± 1.8%. None of the participants had fallen in the year before their participation, and all achieved a score of 27 and over when the MMSE was completed for them. Two older adults with diabetes were excluded because they had peripheral neuropathy. The rest of the older participants achieved MNSI scores between zero and 1.5, so neuropathy was not confirmed. During treadmill familiarisation, two healthy older participants and one older participant with diabetes were excluded because they were uncomfortable walking on the treadmill.
Only collected data from 43 participants, 16 young adults (9 males, 7 females), 14 healthy older adults (8 males, 6 females), and 13 older adults with diabetes (7 males, 6 females) was used for statistical analyses. Table 1 presents participants’ characteristics and baseline gait analysis.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Participants’ characteristics and spatiotemporal gait parameters while walking at their preferred speeds (Table 1) were not different among groups.
In step shortening when a short step length target was presented (Fig 2), all three groups used visual feedback during the online correction (Steps 2–10) and reduced the errors in Step 1 (Z = -3.516, p = 0.0004 in Group I, Z = -3.296, p = 0.001 in Group II, and Z = -2.970, p = 0.003 in Group III). Group errors were different in Step 1 and Steps 2–10 significantly (H (2) = 11.457, p = 0.003 and H (2) = 15.467, p = 0.0004). Errors of foot landing adjustments during step shortening were different between Group I and Group III (U = 37, p = 0.003 and U = 21.5, p = 0.0003) and Group II and Group III (U = 31, p = 0.003 and U = 31, p = 0.004).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
The errors in Step 1 (1) were significantly greater than those in Steps 2-3 (2). Young adults (Group I); healthy older adults (Group II); older adults with diabetes (Group III).
In step lengthening when a long step length target was presented, all groups reduced their error in Step 1 during Steps 2–10 significantly (Group I, Z = -3.517, p = 0.0004; Group II, Z = -2.480, p = 0.013; and Group III, Z = -2.411, p = 0.016) (Fig 3). The mean errors of the group were different in Step 1 and Steps 2–10 (H (2) = 13.201, p = 0.001 and H (2) = 18.749, p = 0.00009). Mean step lengthening errors were different between Group I and Group III (U = 30, p = 0.001 and U = 13, p = 0.0006) and Group II and Group III (U = 36, p = 0.008 and U = 20, p = 0.001).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Errors in Step 1 (1) were significantly reduced in Steps 2-3 (2). Young adults (Group I); healthy older adults (Group II); older adults with diabetes (Group III).
In increasing MTC in response to high MTC targets (Fig 4), groups’ mean errors in Step 1 reduced in Steps 2–10 significantly (Z = -2.870, p = 0.004 in Group I, Z = -3.297, p = 0.001 in Group II and Z = -2.760, p = 0.006 in Group III). Groups’ mean errors were different significantly in Step 1 and Steps 2–10 (H (2) = 15.599, p = 0.0004 and H (2) = 11.081, p = 0.004). Mean errors were different between Group I and Group III (U = 22, p = 0.0003 in Step 1 and U = 37.5, p = 0.004 in Steps 2–10) and Group II and Group III (U = 31, p = 0.004 in Step 1 and U = 42, p = 0.017 in Steps 2–10).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
The errors in Step 1 (1) were significantly reduced in Steps 2-3 (2). Group I (young adults), Group II (healthy older adults), and Group III (older adults with diabetes).
During increasing MTCs in response to MTC targets that were 5.5 cm higher than baseline MTCs (Fig 5), participants in all groups used biofeedback to reduce the errors in Step 1 during Steps 2–10 (Z = -3.517, p = 0.0004 in Group I, Z = -3.234, p = 0.001 in Group II, Z = -2.691, p = 0.007 in Group III). Mean errors were also different between groups in Step 1 and Steps 2–10 (H (2) = 10.846, p = 0.004 and H (2) = 12.563, p = 0.002), with significant differences between Group I and Group III (U = 35, p = 0.002 with biofeedback and U = 33, p = 0.002 without biofeedback) and Group II and Group III (U = 35.5, p = 0.007 with biofeedback and U = 41.5, p = 0.016 without biofeedback).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
The errors in Step 1 (1) were significantly reduced in Steps 2-3 (2). Young adults (Group I); healthy older adults (Group II); older adults with diabetes (Group III).
Discussion
This study found that older participants with diabetes mellitus had the most inaccurate foot landing in response to displayed virtual targets when compared to older and younger participant groups. However, older participants with diabetes were able to use meaningful information about the difference between their actual step length/minimum toe clearance and virtual targets to reduce the differences between groups on their performance (adjusted step length and MTC) and virtual targets during the online correction.
The findings supported the first two hypotheses, highlighting the role of visual feedback in gait adaptation among participants. In line with previous research [24,25], gait characteristics during walking at preferred speeds, when no challenge was involved (baseline gait analysis), did not reveal significant differences between groups. Bard et al. [25] reported that when a target suddenly appeared on the monitor, the response was reactive and did not require further processing of afferent (sensory) feedback. However, when the target stayed visible for a few steps, it provided some feedback about the foot landing in the previous step. Therefore, a feedforward correction model was used to improve foot landing adjustment in the following steps. Consistent with previous research on foot adjustments in older adults and stroke survivors [15,17,26], this study found that receiving detailed and meaningful information about a specific task - e.g., walking with long steps - improved the accuracy of foot landing in older adults with diabetes. Like young and healthy older adults, the older participants with diabetes compared their step lengths and MTC heights with presented targets to reduce errors. Increased errors of foot displacement adjustments in older participants with diabetes might result from the delayed comparison between the expected and the real performance during the online correction (Steps 2–10) [27] or the instantaneous comparison between the goal and expected sensory feedback during inter-sensory and visuomotor integration [28]. The corticospinal system plays a key role in gait adaptation when visual feedback is available, as the visual input excites corticospinal neurons in the motor cortex and along the corticospinal pathway [29,30]. This influences neural activity measured by electroencephalography while walking with short steps [31]. Given this involvement, the reduced errors observed during steps 2–10 in older participants with diabetes in comparison with their errors in Step 1 may reflect corticospinal contributions to gait adjustments. During steps 2–10, the participants made voluntary adjustments to their performance based on feedback. Older participants with diabetes might use a similar forward model [27] in other groups of participants to compare their foot landing response with the presented target, which helped them refine the accuracy of the internal forward model in their next step/response. This period involved decision-making and reprogramming of the performed task in the following step [32]. Hence, the forward model enabled the central nervous system to predict the errors of motor commands by modulating feedback loops. In line with previous research [33], the participants reduced their foot landing errors in response to targets during the online correction (Steps 2–10). However, increased standard deviation of foot landing errors during walking with feedback may indicate an increased risk of falling [34]. The combined effects of two independent variables (diabetes and ageing) may lead to impairment of the central nervous system, which is responsible for processing and integrating information [35,36]. This impairment in the integrated brain and spinal neural network activity during walking increases the latencies of evoked potentials and reduces conduction velocity in peripheral nerves [37]. Consequently, older participants with diabetes may need more time to respond during the online correction phase (Steps 2–10) compared with participants without diabetes. Since the presented tasks had similar available times for all participants, errors of foot adjustments for online correction (Steps 2–10) increased in older participants with diabetes.
The finding also supported the third hypothesis - diabetes mellitus would decrease the accuracy of foot landing adjustments in older participants with diabetes. The older participants with diabetes showed reduced accuracy of foot adjustments when targets in Step 1, compared with the healthy older and young participants. The older participants with diabetes showed higher mean errors compared to healthy older and younger groups. Previous studies [38,39] identified that older adults may encounter more challenges in modulating and adapting foot trajectory with a target that shifts forward, backward, or upward. Additionally, the older participants with diabetes in our study displayed increased errors during online correction (Steps 2–10) compared with the other groups. Notably, this impairment was not observed in older participants without diabetes, suggesting that these deficits may be attributed to diabetes-related factors rather than age-related changes. Dingwell et al. [40] also reported that diabetes-related impairments can increase the physiological noise of the central nervous system. Also, the frontal cortex executes response inhabitation which is important to avoid falling by stopping ongoing commands and modulating them based on sensory information [41,42]. Hence, the increased errors in foot-landing adjustments in older participants with diabetes in the current study may be explained by several factors, including frontal and parietal cortex lesions [43], lateralised right hemisphere [44], and reduced abilities to control the development of force in response to tasks [39]. It is important to highlight that these underlying mechanisms have been studied in individuals with Parkinson’s disease and those who have experienced a stroke. Therefore, further investigation is needed to assess the effect of these mechanisms on individuals with diabetes mellitus.
The higher error during the online correction (Steps 2–10) in the older group with diabetes compared with the control groups (younger and healthy older adult participants) may also suggest the presence of learning deficits. In a study on diabetic rats, learning deficits were associated with changes in the hippocampus that were dependent on the severity of diabetes [45]. However, it is important to note that the older participants with diabetes were able to decrease their errors during the online correction (Steps 2–10) compared to their foot adjustments without feedback (Step 1), indicating that any learning impairment among participants was minimal and negligible. This suggests that more practice and exposure to feedback can improve performance [46].
This study examined the effectiveness of a developed visual feedback system for foot landing adjustments in older adult participants with diabetes in a safe, controlled environment, marking it as the first investigation of its kind. However, the study also encountered some limitations. The system was limited to quantifying the accuracy of foot landing adjustments in the sagittal plane; therefore, foot adjustments were not assessed in the mediolateral direction. In future research, it would be valuable to add MATLAB scripts that enable assessment of the foot landing adjustments in the mediolateral direction. Another limitation of this study was the exclusion of participants with a history of falls and neuropathy. These individuals may be able to increase the accuracy of foot landing adjustments using visual feedback. Future research will also investigate the long-term effects of visual feedback for more accurate foot-landing adjustments.
Conclusions
This study investigated the application of a novel visual feedback system for training foot-landing adjustments in older participants with diabetes mellitus. The findings demonstrated that the visual feedback system used in this study significantly enhanced the accuracy of sagittal foot landing adjustments of the leading foot in older participants with diabetes. Specifically, older participants with diabetes mellitus adjusted their step length and minimum toe clearance more accurately when provided with real-time visual feedback using the role of feedforward biofeedback during step adaptation in the sagittal plane. The findings also highlighted the potential of visual feedback as a training tool to enhance gait control, which may contribute to fall prevention in this high-risk population.
References
1. 1. Crews RT, Yalla SV, Fleischer AE, Wu SC. A growing troubling triad: diabetes, aging, and falls. J Aging Res. 2013;2013:342650. pmid:23476773
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
2. 2. Martin S, Taylor SB, Shideler BL, Ogrin R, Begg R. Overground gait adaptability in older adults with type 2 diabetes in response to virtual targets and physical obstacles. PLoS One. 2023;18(9):e0276999. pmid:37703264
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
3. 3. Tavares NMB, Silva JM, Silva MDM da, Silva LDT, Souza JN de, Ithamar L, et al. Balance, Gait, Functionality and Fall Occurrence in Adults and Older Adults with Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus and Associated Peripheral Neuropathy. Clin Pract. 2024;14(5):2044–55. pmid:39451876
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
4. 4. Hewston P, Deshpande N. Falls and Balance Impairments in Older Adults with Type 2 Diabetes: Thinking Beyond Diabetic Peripheral Neuropathy. Can J Diabetes. 2016;40(1):6–9. pmid:26778679
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
5. 5. Roman de Mettelinge T, Cambier D, Calders P, Van Den Noortgate N, Delbaere K. Understanding the relationship between type 2 diabetes mellitus and falls in older adults: a prospective cohort study. PLoS One. 2013;8(6):e67055. pmid:23825617
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
6. 6. World Health Organization. (n.d.). Falls 2021. [cited February 9, 2025. ]. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/falls
* View Article
* Google Scholar
7. 7. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. Falls in older Australians 2019–20: hospitalisations and deaths among people aged 65 and over. 2022
8. 8. Diabetes: Australian facts 2020. [cited March 9, 2025. ]. https://www.aihw.gov.au/reports/diabetes/diabetes
* View Article
* Google Scholar
9. 9. Geerse DJ, Roerdink M, Marinus J, van Hilten JJ. Walking adaptability for targeted fall-risk assessments. Gait Posture. 2019;70:203–10. pmid:30901621
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
10. 10. Dingwell JB, Davis BL. A rehabilitation treadmill with software for providing real-time gait analysis and visual feedback. 1996.
11. 11. Mirelman A, Rochester L, Maidan I, Del Din S, Alcock L, Nieuwhof F, et al. Addition of a non-immersive virtual reality component to treadmill training to reduce fall risk in older adults (V-TIME): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2016;388(10050):1170–82. pmid:27524393
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
12. 12. Liu M-W, Hsu W-C, Lu T-W, Chen H-L, Liu H-C. Patients with type II diabetes mellitus display reduced toe-obstacle clearance with altered gait patterns during obstacle-crossing. Gait Posture. 2010;31(1):93–9. pmid:19875290
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
13. 13. Hsu W-C, Liu M-W, Lu T-W. Biomechanical risk factors for tripping during obstacle--Crossing with the trailing limb in patients with type II diabetes mellitus. Gait Posture. 2016;45:103–9. pmid:26979890
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
14. 14. Bouche P. Neuropathy of the elderly. Rev Neurol (Paris). 2020;176(9):733–8. pmid:32980155
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
15. 15. Caetano MJD, Lord SR, Schoene D, Pelicioni PHS, Sturnieks DL, Menant JC. Age-related changes in gait adaptability in response to unpredictable obstacles and stepping targets. Gait Posture. 2016;46:35–41. pmid:27131174
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
16. 16. Tirosh O, Cambell A, Begg RK, Sparrow WA. Biofeedback training effects on minimum toe clearance variability during treadmill walking. Ann Biomed Eng. 2013;41(8):1661–9. pmid:23064822
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
17. 17. Begg RK, Tirosh O, Said CM, Sparrow WA, Steinberg N, Levinger P, et al. Gait training with real-time augmented toe-ground clearance information decreases tripping risk in older adults and a person with chronic stroke. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:243. pmid:24847234
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
18. 18. Roerdink M, Lamoth CJC, Kwakkel G, van Wieringen PCW, Beek PJ. Gait coordination after stroke: benefits of acoustically paced treadmill walking. Phys Ther. 2007;87(8):1009–22. pmid:17553922
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
19. 19. van Swigchem R, van Duijnhoven HJR, den Boer J, Geurts AC, Weerdesteyn V. Deficits in motor response to avoid sudden obstacles during gait in functional walkers poststroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27(3):230–9. pmid:23077147
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
20. 20. Mehdikhani M, Taylor S, Shideler BL, Ogrin R, Begg R. Age effects on step adaptation during treadmill walking with continuous step length biofeedback. Gait Posture. 2020;80:174–7. pmid:32521471
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
21. 21. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods 2009;41:1149–60. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
* View Article
* Google Scholar
22. 22. Lunetta M, Le Moli R, Grasso G, Sangiorgio L. A simplified diagnostic test for ambulatory screening of peripheral diabetic neuropathy. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 1998;39(3):165–72. pmid:9649948
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
23. 23. Cockrell JR, Folstein MF. Mini-Mental State Examination. Principles and Practice of Geriatric Psychiatry, 2002, p. 140–1.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
24. 24. Nagano H, Begg RK, Sparrow WA, Taylor S. A comparison of treadmill and overground walking effects on step cycle asymmetry in young and older individuals. J Appl Biomech. 2013;29(2):188–93. pmid:22814355
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
25. 25. Bard C, Turrell Y, Fleury M, Teasdale N, Lamarre Y, Martin O. Deafferentation and pointing with visual double-step perturbations. Exp Brain Res. 1999;125(4):410–6. pmid:10323286
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
26. 26. Spedden ME, Choi JT, Nielsen JB, Geertsen SS. Corticospinal control of normal and visually guided gait in healthy older and younger adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;78:29–41. pmid:30852367
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
27. 27. Desmurget M, Grafton S. Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4(11):423–31. pmid:11058820
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
28. 28. Magescas F, Urquizar C, Prablanc C. Two modes of error processing in reaching. Exp Brain Res. 2009;193(3):337–50. pmid:19011846
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
29. 29. Schubert M, Curt A, Colombo G, Berger W, Dietz V. Voluntary control of human gait: conditioning of magnetically evoked motor responses in a precision stepping task. Exp Brain Res. 1999;126(4):583–8. pmid:10422722
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
30. 30. Drew T, Jiang W, Kably B, Lavoie S. Role of the motor cortex in the control of visually triggered gait modifications. Can J Physiol Pharmacol. 1996;74(4):426–42. pmid:8828889
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
31. 31. Wagner J, Makeig S, Gola M, Neuper C, Müller-Putz G. Distinct β Band Oscillatory Networks Subserving Motor and Cognitive Control during Gait Adaptation. J Neurosci. 2016;36(7):2212–26. pmid:26888931
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
32. 32. Gaveau V, Pisella L, Priot A-E, Fukui T, Rossetti Y, Pélisson D, et al. Automatic online control of motor adjustments in reaching and grasping. Neuropsychologia. 2014;55:25–40. pmid:24334110
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
33. 33. Tseng S-C, Stanhope SJ, Morton SM. Impaired reactive stepping adjustments in older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(7):807–15. pmid:19351694
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
34. 34. Begg R, Best R, Dell’Oro L, Taylor S. Minimum foot clearance during walking: strategies for the minimisation of trip-related falls. Gait Posture. 2007;25(2):191–8. pmid:16678418
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
35. 35. Seidler RD. Aging affects motor learning but not savings at transfer of learning. Learn Mem. 2007;14(1–2):17–21. pmid:17202428
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
36. 36. Rosso AL, Studenski SA, Chen WG, Aizenstein HJ, Alexander NB, Bennett DA, et al. Aging, the central nervous system, and mobility. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68(11):1379–86. pmid:23843270
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
37. 37. Di Mario U, Morano S, Valle E, Pozzessere G. Electrophysiological alterations of the central nervous system in diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Metab Rev. 1995;11(3):259–77. pmid:8536543
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
38. 38. Mazaheri M, Roerdink M, Bood RJ, Duysens J, Beek PJ, Peper CLE. Attentional costs of visually guided walking: effects of age, executive function and stepping-task demands. Gait Posture. 2014;40(1):182–6. pmid:24767613
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
39. 39. Kim H-D, Brunt D. Effect of a change in step direction from a forward to a lateral target in response to a sensory perturbation. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2013;23(4):851–7. pmid:23528810
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
40. 40. Dingwell JB, Salinas MM, Cusumano JP. Increased gait variability may not imply impaired stride-to-stride control of walking in healthy older adults: Winner: 2013 Gait and Clinical Movement Analysis Society Best Paper Award. Gait Posture. 2017;55:131–7. pmid:28454071
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
41. 41. Aron AR. From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(12):e55-68. pmid:20932513
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
42. 42. Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;108:44–79. pmid:23856628
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
43. 43. Mutha PK, Stapp LH, Sainburg RL, Haaland KY. Frontal and parietal cortex contributions to action modification. Cortex. 2014;57:38–50. pmid:24763127
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
44. 44. Mars RB, Piekema C, Coles MGH, Hulstijn W, Toni I. On the programming and reprogramming of actions. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(12):2972–9. pmid:17389629
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
45. 45. Gispen WH, Biessels G-J. Cognition and synaptic plasticity in diabetes mellitus. Trends Neurosci 2000;23:542–9.
* View Article
* Google Scholar
46. 46. Potocanac Z, Duysens J. Online adjustments of leg movements in healthy young and old. Exp Brain Res. 2017;235(8):2329–48. pmid:28478576
* View Article
* PubMed/NCBI
* Google Scholar
Citation: Martin S, Taylor SB, Pejhan S, Shideler BL, Ogrin R, Begg R (2025) Immediate effects of visual feedback on the accuracy of foot landing adjustments in older people with diabetes mellitus: A cross-sectional study. PLoS One 20(5): e0323569. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0323569
About the Authors:
Suzanne Martin
Roles: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Institute of Health and Sport, Victoria University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8363-097X
Simon B. Taylor
Roles: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Institute of Health and Sport, Victoria University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Shabnam Pejhan
Roles: Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Automotive and Mechatronics Engineering, Ontario Tech University, Oshawa, Ontario, Canada
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5447-3856
Blynne L. Shideler
Roles: Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: School of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States of America
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4520-9700
Rajna Ogrin
Roles: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Bolton Clarke Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia
Rezaul Begg
Roles: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Institute of Health and Sport, Victoria University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
1. Crews RT, Yalla SV, Fleischer AE, Wu SC. A growing troubling triad: diabetes, aging, and falls. J Aging Res. 2013;2013:342650. pmid:23476773
2. Martin S, Taylor SB, Shideler BL, Ogrin R, Begg R. Overground gait adaptability in older adults with type 2 diabetes in response to virtual targets and physical obstacles. PLoS One. 2023;18(9):e0276999. pmid:37703264
3. Tavares NMB, Silva JM, Silva MDM da, Silva LDT, Souza JN de, Ithamar L, et al. Balance, Gait, Functionality and Fall Occurrence in Adults and Older Adults with Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus and Associated Peripheral Neuropathy. Clin Pract. 2024;14(5):2044–55. pmid:39451876
4. Hewston P, Deshpande N. Falls and Balance Impairments in Older Adults with Type 2 Diabetes: Thinking Beyond Diabetic Peripheral Neuropathy. Can J Diabetes. 2016;40(1):6–9. pmid:26778679
5. Roman de Mettelinge T, Cambier D, Calders P, Van Den Noortgate N, Delbaere K. Understanding the relationship between type 2 diabetes mellitus and falls in older adults: a prospective cohort study. PLoS One. 2013;8(6):e67055. pmid:23825617
6. World Health Organization. (n.d.). Falls 2021. [cited February 9, 2025. ]. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/falls
7. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. Falls in older Australians 2019–20: hospitalisations and deaths among people aged 65 and over. 2022
8. Diabetes: Australian facts 2020. [cited March 9, 2025. ]. https://www.aihw.gov.au/reports/diabetes/diabetes
9. Geerse DJ, Roerdink M, Marinus J, van Hilten JJ. Walking adaptability for targeted fall-risk assessments. Gait Posture. 2019;70:203–10. pmid:30901621
10. Dingwell JB, Davis BL. A rehabilitation treadmill with software for providing real-time gait analysis and visual feedback. 1996.
11. Mirelman A, Rochester L, Maidan I, Del Din S, Alcock L, Nieuwhof F, et al. Addition of a non-immersive virtual reality component to treadmill training to reduce fall risk in older adults (V-TIME): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2016;388(10050):1170–82. pmid:27524393
12. Liu M-W, Hsu W-C, Lu T-W, Chen H-L, Liu H-C. Patients with type II diabetes mellitus display reduced toe-obstacle clearance with altered gait patterns during obstacle-crossing. Gait Posture. 2010;31(1):93–9. pmid:19875290
13. Hsu W-C, Liu M-W, Lu T-W. Biomechanical risk factors for tripping during obstacle--Crossing with the trailing limb in patients with type II diabetes mellitus. Gait Posture. 2016;45:103–9. pmid:26979890
14. Bouche P. Neuropathy of the elderly. Rev Neurol (Paris). 2020;176(9):733–8. pmid:32980155
15. Caetano MJD, Lord SR, Schoene D, Pelicioni PHS, Sturnieks DL, Menant JC. Age-related changes in gait adaptability in response to unpredictable obstacles and stepping targets. Gait Posture. 2016;46:35–41. pmid:27131174
16. Tirosh O, Cambell A, Begg RK, Sparrow WA. Biofeedback training effects on minimum toe clearance variability during treadmill walking. Ann Biomed Eng. 2013;41(8):1661–9. pmid:23064822
17. Begg RK, Tirosh O, Said CM, Sparrow WA, Steinberg N, Levinger P, et al. Gait training with real-time augmented toe-ground clearance information decreases tripping risk in older adults and a person with chronic stroke. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:243. pmid:24847234
18. Roerdink M, Lamoth CJC, Kwakkel G, van Wieringen PCW, Beek PJ. Gait coordination after stroke: benefits of acoustically paced treadmill walking. Phys Ther. 2007;87(8):1009–22. pmid:17553922
19. van Swigchem R, van Duijnhoven HJR, den Boer J, Geurts AC, Weerdesteyn V. Deficits in motor response to avoid sudden obstacles during gait in functional walkers poststroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27(3):230–9. pmid:23077147
20. Mehdikhani M, Taylor S, Shideler BL, Ogrin R, Begg R. Age effects on step adaptation during treadmill walking with continuous step length biofeedback. Gait Posture. 2020;80:174–7. pmid:32521471
21. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods 2009;41:1149–60. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
22. Lunetta M, Le Moli R, Grasso G, Sangiorgio L. A simplified diagnostic test for ambulatory screening of peripheral diabetic neuropathy. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 1998;39(3):165–72. pmid:9649948
23. Cockrell JR, Folstein MF. Mini-Mental State Examination. Principles and Practice of Geriatric Psychiatry, 2002, p. 140–1.
24. Nagano H, Begg RK, Sparrow WA, Taylor S. A comparison of treadmill and overground walking effects on step cycle asymmetry in young and older individuals. J Appl Biomech. 2013;29(2):188–93. pmid:22814355
25. Bard C, Turrell Y, Fleury M, Teasdale N, Lamarre Y, Martin O. Deafferentation and pointing with visual double-step perturbations. Exp Brain Res. 1999;125(4):410–6. pmid:10323286
26. Spedden ME, Choi JT, Nielsen JB, Geertsen SS. Corticospinal control of normal and visually guided gait in healthy older and younger adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;78:29–41. pmid:30852367
27. Desmurget M, Grafton S. Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4(11):423–31. pmid:11058820
28. Magescas F, Urquizar C, Prablanc C. Two modes of error processing in reaching. Exp Brain Res. 2009;193(3):337–50. pmid:19011846
29. Schubert M, Curt A, Colombo G, Berger W, Dietz V. Voluntary control of human gait: conditioning of magnetically evoked motor responses in a precision stepping task. Exp Brain Res. 1999;126(4):583–8. pmid:10422722
30. Drew T, Jiang W, Kably B, Lavoie S. Role of the motor cortex in the control of visually triggered gait modifications. Can J Physiol Pharmacol. 1996;74(4):426–42. pmid:8828889
31. Wagner J, Makeig S, Gola M, Neuper C, Müller-Putz G. Distinct β Band Oscillatory Networks Subserving Motor and Cognitive Control during Gait Adaptation. J Neurosci. 2016;36(7):2212–26. pmid:26888931
32. Gaveau V, Pisella L, Priot A-E, Fukui T, Rossetti Y, Pélisson D, et al. Automatic online control of motor adjustments in reaching and grasping. Neuropsychologia. 2014;55:25–40. pmid:24334110
33. Tseng S-C, Stanhope SJ, Morton SM. Impaired reactive stepping adjustments in older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(7):807–15. pmid:19351694
34. Begg R, Best R, Dell’Oro L, Taylor S. Minimum foot clearance during walking: strategies for the minimisation of trip-related falls. Gait Posture. 2007;25(2):191–8. pmid:16678418
35. Seidler RD. Aging affects motor learning but not savings at transfer of learning. Learn Mem. 2007;14(1–2):17–21. pmid:17202428
36. Rosso AL, Studenski SA, Chen WG, Aizenstein HJ, Alexander NB, Bennett DA, et al. Aging, the central nervous system, and mobility. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68(11):1379–86. pmid:23843270
37. Di Mario U, Morano S, Valle E, Pozzessere G. Electrophysiological alterations of the central nervous system in diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Metab Rev. 1995;11(3):259–77. pmid:8536543
38. Mazaheri M, Roerdink M, Bood RJ, Duysens J, Beek PJ, Peper CLE. Attentional costs of visually guided walking: effects of age, executive function and stepping-task demands. Gait Posture. 2014;40(1):182–6. pmid:24767613
39. Kim H-D, Brunt D. Effect of a change in step direction from a forward to a lateral target in response to a sensory perturbation. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2013;23(4):851–7. pmid:23528810
40. Dingwell JB, Salinas MM, Cusumano JP. Increased gait variability may not imply impaired stride-to-stride control of walking in healthy older adults: Winner: 2013 Gait and Clinical Movement Analysis Society Best Paper Award. Gait Posture. 2017;55:131–7. pmid:28454071
41. Aron AR. From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(12):e55-68. pmid:20932513
42. Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;108:44–79. pmid:23856628
43. Mutha PK, Stapp LH, Sainburg RL, Haaland KY. Frontal and parietal cortex contributions to action modification. Cortex. 2014;57:38–50. pmid:24763127
44. Mars RB, Piekema C, Coles MGH, Hulstijn W, Toni I. On the programming and reprogramming of actions. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(12):2972–9. pmid:17389629
45. Gispen WH, Biessels G-J. Cognition and synaptic plasticity in diabetes mellitus. Trends Neurosci 2000;23:542–9.
46. Potocanac Z, Duysens J. Online adjustments of leg movements in healthy young and old. Exp Brain Res. 2017;235(8):2329–48. pmid:28478576
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2025 Martin et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Diabetes Mellitus in older adults reduces the accuracy of foot landing adjustments and increases the risk of falling. This study investigated whether targeted visual feedback enhances the accuracy of the foot landing in older participants with diabetes. Forty-eight volunteers in three groups of young, healthy older and older adults with diabetes participated. During treadmill walking, Nexus Vicon software streamed real-time orientation data of two markers on the first toes into MATLAB scripts through a Visual3D server. The system visualised real-meaningful step length or minimum toe clearance (MTC) in each step on a monitor in front of a treadmill at eye level. Participants responded to four subject-specific step-length (±10% mean baseline step length) or MTC targets (3.5 and 5.5 cm higher than the mean baseline MTC). One target was displayed every 10 steps and removed after 10 steps when participants walked without seeing any target, then a new or the same target appeared and stayed on for another 10 steps. Tasks were increasing or decreasing baseline step length or increasing the baseline MTC in response to virtual targets. The accuracy (absolute error) of each step adjustment in each 10-step block with a target was calculated. The mean accuracy of Step 1 and the mean accuracy of Steps 2–10 were measured and compared within and between groups using three-way ANOVA tests. Errors significantly differed between conditions (Step 1 and Steps 2–10) in all groups. All groups showed reduced errors in Step 1 during Steps 2–10. Among groups, the group with diabetes presented the greatest errors in Step 1 and Steps 2–10. These findings suggest that meaningful visual feedback about spatial gait parameters (step length, MTC) can improve foot-landing accuracy in older participants with diabetes, highlighting its potential as a training tool to prevent falls in this high-risk population.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer