ABSTRACT. In a broader context, the pursuit of happiness has become one of the most important goals of a modern human. Despite substantial research on this topic, few studies have examined the links between happiness and cultural dimensions and none have looked at the Member States of the European Union in this context. This study was an attempt to fill this gap by examining the relationship between the rankings of the 26 EU Member States on the World Happiness Report and Hofstedes six cultural dimensions. Pearson correlation coefficients and linear regression coefficients were calculated to examine the relationships vía the use of the SPSS software. The results show a relatively strong positive relationship between happiness and Indulgence and Individualism, while the relationship between happiness and the cointegration of the cultural dimensions of Uncertainty Avoidance and Power Distance is relatively strong but negative. No correlation is found between happiness and the Masculinity and Long-term Orientation dimensions. Thus, a permissive and individualistic social environment is the main contributor to happiness in each nation, while excessive social uncertainty and power distance are the main detractors. Overall, these cultural dimensions must be given more attention if the European Union is to have happier Member States.
Keywords: culture, happiness, Hofstede, European Union
JEL Classification: A13, F50, 131
Introduction
The importance of life satisfaction and happiness 1s becoming increasingly apparent today. Nothing proves this more than the fact that many international organisations are conducting research in this area. The Better Life Index (2025), published by the OECD, assesses the life satisfaction of the inhabitants of the Member States. Eurostat (2025) has published its Personal Well-Being indicator three times so far in 2013, 2018 and 2022 and the European Social Survey (2025) has also asked several times in recent years - although not annually - how happy the citizens of the European countries consider themselves. In this context, it is surprising that there has not yet been a study on the happiness of EU Member States in relation to their cultural dimensions. This may be due to the fact that not all EU Member States are members of the OECD (see Cyprus, Croatia, Malta, and Romania) and that neither Eurostat nor the European Social Survey carry out their surveys every year. In the last ten years, the World Happiness Report (2025), published in cooperation with Gallup, has covered all EU Member States. However, even the World Happiness Report data have not been compared to Hofstede's data on cultural dimensions (Individualism, Indulgence, Long-term Orientation, Masculinity, Power Distance and Uncertainty Avoidance). As there are only a few studies in the literature individually and they are either from different perspectives (Achim et al., 2016; Hysa & Mansi, 2020; Kasman & Kasman, 2020; Androniceanu, 2022; Akgun et al., 2023;) or their research angle does not reflect the concerns of the Member States (Veenhoven, 2012; Steel et al., 2018; Mushtaq & Siddiqui, 2020; Rajkumar, 2023; Li et al., 2024), the present effort aims to fill the respective gap.
The purpose of this paper is to discuss whether happiness can be observed across nations and whether cultural dimensions can be used as an indicator of people's wellbeing. The research is aimed at identifying a possible link between cultural dimensions and happiness in the countries of the European Union. The paper presents a comparative case study in relation to Hofstede's six cultural dimensions and to the latest data provided by the World Happiness Report.
The research questions therefore focus on cultural differences as well as those related to happiness. They have been formulated in such a way as to provide an opportunity to explore the cultural dimensions that generate the strongest impact on happiness:
Ol: Do Hofstede 's cultural dimensions have any influence and, if so, what is the role of each of them on the happiness of EU Member States as indicated on the World Happiness Report?
02: Which of Hofstede 's cultural dimensions have a positive and which have a negative impact on the happiness of EU citizens?
The research seeks to answer the respective questions through the examples of 26 EU Member States. It tries to show which cultural dimensions have the greatest impact on people's happiness in the European Union. For the sake of evaluating the strength of the respective correlations the relationship between happiness and culture was analysed by using the Pearson correlation coefficient with a dual probe followed by the interpretation of the specific data. Furthermore, linear regression equations have been set up to explore the relationship between cultural dimensions and the happiness index. Followed by a theoretical overview focusing on happiness and cultural dimensions in the European Union, the author will introduce the respective research methodology, present and analyse the given results, and summarize the findings.
1. Literature review
1.1. Happiness in the European Union
Richard Easterlin has examined the relationship between happiness and GDP both among and within individual countries through time in several papers (1974, 1995, 2005). While Easterlin shows little significant evidence of a link between aggregate income and average happiness, Stevenson and Wolfers (2008) found that people living in better material circumstances also enjoy higher subjective well-being, and that the steady rise in living standards has led to higher subjective well-being. Nevertheless, other research results have supported and confirmed the existence of a paradox (Diener & Seligman, 2004). Responding to his critics, Easterlin (2017) admitted that the paradox has been confirmed leading him to conclude that if a society had to choose between GDP and happiness, it would choose the latter (Easterlin in Rojas, 2019). Kahneman and Krueger (2006) identified only one of 11 factors considered strongly correlated with life satisfaction and happiness, namely material well-being (Kulcsár, 2020). Ng and Diener established in 2014 that a nation's GDP per capita decreases the correlation between such factors as autonomy and social support, financial satisfaction, and various subjective well-being outcomes (positive feelings, negative feelings, life evaluation). Inglehart and colleagues (Inglehart et al., 2008) found that economic growth and GDP per capita explain 50% of the differences in SWB (subjective well-being) across countries.
Regarding Gross National Happiness, in 1972, Bhutan's Fourth King declared Gross National Happiness (GNH) more important than Gross Domestic Product. (Givel, 2023, p.1). According to Veenhoven (2007 in Nath, 2018) Gross National Happiness (GNH) measures the quality of living in a country in a more holistic way (than GNP), and considers that human society undergoes beneficial development when material and spiritual growth go hand in hand, complementing and reinforcing each other. GNH is a multidimensional approach to development that seeks to achieve a harmonious balance between material well-being and the spiritual, emotional and cultural needs of society (Gross National Happiness Commission, 2015 in Givel, 2015). The Gross National Happiness Index includes nine factors: Time use, Cultural diversity and resilience, Psychological wellbeing, Community vitality, Ecological diversity and resilience, Living standards, Health, Education, Good governance (Gross National Happiness Index webpage, 2024).
According to Buttrick and Oishi (2023) the income level of a country shows correlation with its happiness. Although rich Americans still work long hours, they probably have more control over how they spend their time than poor Americans. Better quality social relationships, as well as greater opportunities to spend money on others and greater autonomy in how they use their time, may provide an overall explanation why higher incomes are associated with greater happiness. People tend not to judge their happiness in terms of absolute income, but rather compare it vertically and horizontally with others (Hu, 2023). High income can only contribute to life satisfaction, but not happiness. Income itself is important, it should not be discounted, but it is not the only criterion when making decisions (Stober, 2023). Regarding the size of the government, going below a certain threshold leads to a reduction in people's happiness (Mahmouei & Razmi, 2023). People are not happier in welfare states than in equally wealthy nations where the "caretaker fatherly state" is less accessible. Nor does happiness inequality appear to be smaller in welfare states (Veenhoven, 2000 in Veenhoven, 2020).
As for the connection between religion and happiness, religion plays an important role in Happiness as demonstrated by the rate of happiness among followers of Buddhism compared to that of other religions. Givel, (2022) and Givel, (2015) focused on especially Mahayana Buddhism, which holds that happiness and compassion come from an awareness of suffering in oneself and in others. Accordingly, education can help in eliminating the immediate distractions related to suffering. Demenech and colleagues analysed the connection between stress and happiness (Demenech et al., 2022). They showed that the happiness levels of both rich and poor respondents" decreased as stress levels increased. In their conclusion, stress plays an important role in the relationship between income and happiness. While there is some evidence that money can influence happiness, achieving and experiencing the latter appears to be highly dependent on the given individual stress level. In multivariate studies happiness was significantly independently associated with younger age, satisfaction with health, satisfaction with household income, trust in the community, satisfaction with democracy and religious belief (DiCosimo & Kelly, 2022). Bergsma, Buijt and Veenhoven (2020) concluded that a form of happiness training is advisable for individuals seeking a more satisfying life. Since happier workers tend to be more productive, organizations would be wise to provide such training programs and techniques for their employees.
The publications cited above show that happiness has been explored by many people and in many different ways. Below, the results of the most recent "happiness" research for the EU Member States are presented. Helliwell and co-authors (Helliwell et al., 2023a) identified the following six components of the World Happiness Report: 1. Gross domestic product 2. Social support 3. Healthy Life Expectancy 4. Freedom to make Life Choices 5. Generosity 6. Perception of Corruption. The World Happiness Report 2023 looked at 155 countries and ranked them according to their happiness levels. The average scores of the World Happiness Report pertaining to the European Union between 2020 and 2022 are shown in Figure 1.
In recent years, numerous studies have been conducted on the well-being of European Union member states. One of these studies (Achim et al., 2016) concludes that well-being is an important determinant of the shadow economy in EU countries. The impact of well-being on the shadow economy is larger in former EU member states than in new EU member states. Another study (Hysa & Mansi, 2020) shows that the relationship between education and satisfaction is stronger and more positive in the Western Balkans, but lower for the more educated in EU countries. Androniceanu, Georgescu and Sabie (2022) find that in EU countries the link between digitalization and prosperity 1s clear. Pelka (2019) examined whether selected European countries show similar patterns of happiness. The first and happiest group under consideration contains the core members of the European Union (e.g. Germany, United Kingdom, etc.). The second cluster is made up of the post-communist countries of Central and Eastern Europe (e.g. Hungary, Poland, etc.). The third cluster includes eight countries (e.g. Portugal, Slovenia, etc.).
Contemporary cultural diversity contributes to individual life satisfaction, according to research across several European countries (Li et al., 2024). The study of Artan, Demirel, and Hayaloglu (2022) focusing on 12 EU countries confirm the positive impact of increased trust in government concerning economic growth and well-being. Easterlin and O'Connor (2022) examined10 countries in Northern, Western, and Southern Europe and concluded that generous welfare programs are the obvious key to well-being; a research team (Akgun et al., 2023) focusing on 27 EU member states arrived at the conclusion that due to both taxes and inflation rates the current level of economic well-being is higher. They found that the level of economic outcomes is positively related to the happiness index rate (HIR), while the overall employment rate has a significant negative impact on the HIR. Another group of researchers (Degutis et al., 2010) found that in case of EU member states the level of GDP is positively related to the level of life satisfaction. This relationship is particularly strong in Eastern European countries, but also remains positive in many rich EU countries. A study (Kasman & Kasman, 2020), also based on data from EU member states, shows that income inequality and unemployment have a significantly negative impact on well-being. A study of a representative sample of 28 European countries (Zagorski et al., 2013) shows that an increase in GDP per capita has a significant impact on subjective well-being, economic status, quality of life and health. The World Happiness Index has a significant relationship with the Charitable Aid Foundation (CAF) or Global Philanthropy Index (Mushtaq & Siddiqui, 2020, p.50). Another study emphasizes that denying or down-playing the importance of authentic positive relationships, appreciation, empathy, compassion, and acceptance, undermines important elements of wellbeing and is a critical issue for the human experience. A general cultural and social shift may be needed in many parts of the world to place a higher value on these elements (Spowart, 2022). One analysis investigated the reasons why Scandinavian countries ranked high in the ranking of the world's happiest countries. The results showed that the Scandinavian model is characterized by extremely well-developed social policies that provide high benefits (income redistribution), active participation of citizens in the creation of a common identity, job security, and a developed, dynamic, and vibrant civil society (Pori, 2021).
1.2. Hofstede's cultural dimensions in the European Union
Hofstede, who is one of the most frequently quoted social scientists and who achieved the greatest results in his academic work by comparing the cultures of different nations, has created six dimensions that are culturally distinct (Hofstede, 1980, 2011, 2024; Hofstede & Minkov, 2013):
1. Power Distance: which refers to different solutions to the basic problem of human inequality.
2. Individualism versus Collectivism: this dimension refers to the integration of individuals into major groups.
3. Masculinity versus Femininity: this dimension implies a division of emotional roles between the sexes.
4. Uncertainty Avoidance: relates to the degree of stress in a society faced with an unknown future.
5. Long-term versus Short-term Orientation: relates to people's choice of focus of effort (future versus present/past).
6. Indulgence versus Restraint: 1t refers to the fulfillment of basic human needs for the enjoyment of life versus strict social control.
Some studies have been published that focus on Hofstede's cultural dimension related to Happiness (not only in the European Union). One of these researchers, Finuras (2020) found a significant and strong negative correlation between the Global Happiness Report country scores and the Power Distance index and a significant and strong but positive correlation between the Global Happiness Report country scores and Hofstede's Individualism index. Taras and colleagues (2010) argue that Hofstede's cultural values were most strongly linked to emotions (such as happiness), followed in order by attitudes, behaviour and work performance. According to a recent study (Elkoutour & Abboubi, 2024), although not covering EU countries, happiness at work correlates positively with Masculinity and negatively with Uncertainty Avoidance. Alparslan, Yastioglus and Tag (2021), in a study of data from 98 countries, also confirmed a negative significant relationship (-0,551) between Power Distance and World Happiness Report data, while a strong positive significant relationship (0,526) was identified between Individualism and the World Happiness Report data. Steele and colleagues (2018) reached similar conclusions. Overall, happier countries have lower values for Power Distance and Uncertainty Avoidance, but higher scores for Femininity and Individualism. Furthermore, they find that these effects are associated with political and economic institutions, but partially independent of them (Steel et al., 2018). In partial agreement with his own study, Dulababu (2017) reached the same conclusion in 2017. His study of 66 countries found that countries with individualistic cultures had higher levels of well-being than those with team-oriented cultures. He also confirmed that countries with low Power Distance cultures had higher levels of happiness than those with high Power Distance cultures. Furthermore, countries with high Uncertainty Avoidance cultures have higher levels of happiness than countries with low Uncertainty Avoidance cultures.
A study (Rajkumar, 2023) that examined the impact of Covid-19 in relation to changes in the World Happiness Report (78 countries) found that among cultural dimensions, Longterm Orientation was positively correlated with changes in subjective well-being, while Indulgence was negatively correlated with this variable. A study (1997) found the following relationships between subjective well-being (SWB) and Hofstede's dimensions: 1. lower Uncertainty Avoidance was a good predictor of higher SWB levels in a country, 2. Masculinity was positively associated with SWB in poorer countries and 3. higher SWB levels were found in countries with a higher proportion of female population. This is somewhat inconsistent with the following statements: Masculinity was consistently negatively associated with SWB indicators, but not all reached statistical significance (Steel et al., 2018). It should be noted, however, that average self-reported happiness varies considerably from country to country. These differences cannot be attributed to mere measurement bias, nor can they be explained by cultural differences in life evaluation. Rather, the observed differences in well-being indicate that not all societies are equally responsive to universal human needs (Veenhoven, 2022).
2. Methodological approach
Based on the results of the research presented in the literature review and the fact that no such research has been carried out for the Member States of the European Union, the following research questions have been formulated (as already stated in the introduction):
Ol: Do Hofstede's cultural dimensions have any impact and, if so, what is the role of each of them on the happiness of EU Member States according to the World Happiness Report?
O2: Which of Hofstede 's cultural dimensions have a positive and which a negative impact on the happiness of EU citizens?
On the basis of the above research questions, the following hypotheses have been formulated regarding the 26 Member States of the European Union:
H1: There is a negative significant relationship between the World Happiness Report, the cultural dimensions of Masculinity, Power Distance and Uncertainty Avoidance.
H2: There is a positive significant relationship between the World Happiness Report and the cultural dimensions of Individualism, Indulgence, and Long-term Orientation.
The present research relies primarily on secondary data sources to verify the above hypotheses, the primary reason being that such a large amount of data collection on such a large sample size of individual Member States would be beyond the scope of this study. The secondary data sources, the most recent data available for both databases, are as follows: Hofstede's Cultural Dimensions Database (Hofstede & Hofstede, 2015) and the World Happiness Report Database (Helliwell, 2023a, p.34-35). Table 1. shows the values of Hofstede's cultural dimension and the World Happiness Report's values for the 26 EU member states. In the case of Cyprus, such data are not available and therefore the given values are not representative of all 27 member states.
The collected data was analysed and evaluated by Excel and SPSS programs in order to establish the average and the deviation of the sample. Furthermore, I relied on the Pearson correlation and linear regression analysis as well.
3. Results
This chapter presents the main findings of the research. Figure 2 presents the results of the linear regression between the cultural dimensions and the Happiness Index.
The R? values of the linear regression analyses show that the strongest relationship with the World Happiness Report data is formed between Uncertainty Avoidance (R?=0,4331) and Indulgence (R?=0,4199), while the weakest relationship is displayed in connection with Longterm Orientation (R?=0,003).
The strongest movement or progression of the World Happiness Report with at least a one per cent significance level can be discerned along the following dimensions: Indulgence (0,645) and Individualism (0,501). In a negative direction with at least a one per cent significance level it shows close correlation with Uncertainty Avoidance (-0,659) and Power Distance (-0,568). No significant relationship was found between happiness and Masculinity (0,265) and Long-term Orientation (-0,054). There are some closer correlations just between the dimensions of Hofstede with at least one per cent significance level: Power Distance - Uncertainty Avoidance (0,540), Power Distance - Individualism (-0,543) and Individualism - Uncertainty Avoidance (-0,528).
The values of the Pearson correlation coefficients are shown in Table 2.
4. Discussion
In order to fully understand the impact of culture on happiness, this paper examined the Member States of the European Union in that context. One hypothesis of the study is that there is a negative significant relationship between the World Happiness Report and three cultural dimensions such as Masculinity, Power Distance and Uncertainty Avoidance. As the results of the statistical analyses confirmed the respective hypothesis for two dimensions and refuted it for one dimension, it was only partially accepted as no such relationship was found for the Masculinity dimension. The results of this study reveal a significant and relatively strong negative correlation between the value of Uncertainty Avoidance and the score of the World Happiness Report and between Power Distance and the score of the World Happiness Report. This means that nations that are less accepting of the unequal distribution of power and may fight against it can expect to be happier than those that are more tolerant of power imbalances. This is confirmed in the case of Power Distance by a previously mentioned study (Finuras, 2020) which also found a negative relationship for the Power Distance dimension in the World Happiness Report based on the results of a study of more than 90 countries. The same conclusion was reached by Alparslan and colleagues in 2021. Steel and colleagues (2018) also find a negative correlation for the Uncertainty Avoidance dimension beyond Power Distance, which is in line with the results of the present study. According to Dulababu (2017), countries with lower Power Distance are predisposed to higher happiness, while countries with higher Uncertainty Avoidance are much happier than their counterparts with lower scores, which somewhat contradicts the results of this research.
The other hypothesis of the study is that there is a positive significant relationship between the World Happiness Report and the other three cultural dimensions. As the results of the statistical analyses confirmed the hypothesis for only two dimensions and refuted it for one dimension, the hypothesis related to Individualism, Indulgence and Long-term Orientation was also only partially accepted, as no such relationship was found for the Long-term Orientation dimension. Data show a positive, and also relatively strong and significant correlation between Indulgence and the score of the World Happiness Report along with a positive relation between Individualism and the score of the World Happiness Report. This means that people can expect greater happiness in countries that focus on individual and family well-being and allow the free satisfaction of desires and feelings. For the Individualism dimension, they are also in line with the results of Finuras (2020) and confirm a positive relationship with the World Happiness Report data, as did the aforementioned Alparslan and colleagues' results (2021) in their study of 98 countries. Here, however, Steel and colleagues found a positive correlation not only for Individualism, but also for Feminism, which partially confirms the results of the present study. Dulababu's examination of 66 countries (2017) also revealed that nations with a culture of Individualism had a higher level of happiness.
However, since in this study no significant relationship was found between happiness and Masculinity and Long-term Orientation, the results suggest that there is a correlation between the happiness of each nation and the given cultural differences. Consequently, countries are happier that are less accepting of Uncertainty Avoidance and Power Distance, and also are more individualistic and more permissive of Indulgence. Cultural differences are, of course, not a direct cause of happiness in each country (as they depend on the factors described above), but they certainly have an impact on the well-being of their citizens. Four of Hofstede"s cultural dimensions therefore show a relatively strong relationship with happiness.
5. Conclusion
This research focuses on an area that is a popular topic in many ways, namely happiness. However, from the perspective that it has just been examined, no study has yet been carried out. Although happiness is one of the most commonly used terms in relation to people's well-being, the impact of the cultural dimensions of happiness on the achievement of this goal has not yet been examined for the Member States of the European Union. The results of the present research, based on Hofstede's six cultural dimensions, provide a novel picture of the current state of the relationship between happiness and national cultures.
In order for a country's population to become a happier nation, according to Hofstede's model the following cultural conditions need to be created. First of all, there must be more room, more opportunity for individualistic aspirations to unfold and develop, which implies a higher value of Individualism. Secondly, there must be more permissiveness in the well-being of individuals, more importance given to and space allowed for individual freedom, friendship, the expression of instincts, which is the advanced level of the value of Indulgence. Thirdly, such values as tolerance of ambiguity, trust in the unknown and openness should be propagated and consolidated, which imply the reduction of Uncertainty Avoidance. Last but not least, fourthly, there is a need to become less accepting of unequal power sharing and more critical of excessive power seeking, which means narrowing the Power Distance. The ideas highlighted in italics above summarise the main messages of the paper.
Limitations of the study
The main limitation of the research is the sample size. It is not possible to draw farreaching conclusions for only 26 EU Member States.
The study focuses on a happiness index, the World Happiness Report's Happiness Index. The inclusion of other well-being indicators (e.g. Human Development Index) or happiness indexes (e.g. Eurostat or European Social Survey happiness indicators) would reduce the limitations of the article in this respect.
A further limitation of the study is that it only examines Hofstede's six dimensions of happiness, although there are other researchers who have explored other dimensions of happiness related to cultural differences.
Further possibilities for research
It would be worth extending the sample to all countries in the world for which Hofstede's cultural dimensions and World Happiness Report data are available. The planned analyses using these data should provide a much more accurate picture of the relationship between cultural dimensions and happiness.
It would also be worthwhile to include other cultural dimensions and not just Hofstede's dimensions. They could help to extend the scope of the research, increasing its current reliability and breadth. These broadened fields of research could be based, for example, on the models of Inglehart and Welzel or Trompenaars.
Quantitative research could be complemented by qualitative research methods (e.g. interviews with intercultural professionals from the nation in question) that could confirm or refute the results of quantitative research.
Acknowledgement
I would like to thank Brigitta Pécsek PhD. and Andras Tarnóc PhD. for their enthusiastic and dedicated support.
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Received: July, 2024
1st Revision: March, 2025
Accepted: June, 2025
References
Achim, M. V., Borlea, S. N., Gäban, L. V., € Cuceu, I. С. (2016). Rethinking the shadow economy in terms of happiness. Evidence for the European Union Member States. Technological and Economic Development of Economy, 24(1), 199-228. doi:https://doi.org/10.3846/20294913.2016.1209250.
Akgun, A. I, Türkoǧlu, $. P., & Erikli, $. (2023). Investigating the determinants of happiness index in EU-27 countries: a quantile regression approach. International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy, 43(1/2), 156-177. doi:https://doi.org/10.1108/IISSP-012022-0005.
Alparslan, A. M., Yastioglu, S., € Tas, M. A. (2021). Role of national culture in happiness: Cultural dimensions-happiness score relationship models. Maku Sobed, 34, 53-67. doi: https://doi.org/10.20875/makusobed. 907187.
Androniceanu, A., Georgescu, I, & Sabie, О. (2022). The Impact of Digitalization on Public Administration, Economic Development, and Well-Being in the EU Countries. Central European Public Administration Review, 20(2), 9-31. doi: https://doi.org/10.17573/cepar.2022.2.01.
Artan, S., Hayaloglu, P., & Demirel, $. К. (2022). How Does Trust in Government Affect Economic Growth and Happiness: Evidence from EU-12 Countries. Current Debates on Sustainable Development, Pinar Hayaloglu, Seyfettin Artan, Editor, 115-134. London: ПОРЕС Publication.
Bergsma, A., Buijt, L, & Veenhoven, К. (2020). Will happiness-trainings make us happier? A research synthesis using an online findings-archive. Contemporary Theories, Approaches and Applications, Special issue of Frontier in Psychology, 11, 177-208. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01953.
Buttrick, N., € Oishi, $. (2023). Money and happiness: A consideration of history and psychological mechanisms. Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, 120(13), 1-3. doi:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas. 2301893120.
Demenech, L. M., Almeida, К. B., Neiva-Silva, L., & Dumith, $. С. (2022). Does Money Buy Happiness? Disentangling the Association between Income, Happiness and Stress. Annals of the Brazilian Academy of Sciences, 942) 1-5. dai: https://doi.org/10.1590/0001-3765202220210364.
DiCosimo, A., & Kelly, B. D. (2022). Happiness in pre-pandemic Europe: correlates of individual happiness prior to Covid. Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, 40(3), 460463. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/ipm.2022.47.
Diener, E., & Seligman, М. Е. P. (2004). Beyond money: Toward an economy of well-being. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5(1), 1-31. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00501001 x.
Porié, Z. (2021). Economy and Happiness - Questioning Relationships. Megatrend Review, 19(3), 141-165. doi: https://doi.org/10.5937/megrev2203141d.
Dulababu, T (2017). Global Happiness: Continental and Cross-Cultural Models Perspective. Journal of Global Economics, 5(4), 1-7. doi:10.4172/2375-4389.1000268. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from https://www.hilarispublisher.com/open-access/global-happinesscontinental-and-crosscultural-models-perspective-2375-4389-1000268.pdf
Easterlin, К. A., & O'Connor, К. J. (2022). Explaining happiness trends in Europe. PNAS, 119(37), 1-4. doi: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2210639119.
Easterlin, R. A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence. In Nations and Households in Economic Growth: Essays in Honor of Moses Abramowitz, by Paul A. David and Melvin W. Reder. New York: Academic Press.
Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will Raising the Incomes of All Increase the Happiness of All? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 27(1), 35-48.
Easterlin, R. A. (2005). Diminishing Marginal Utility of Income? Caveat Emptor. Social Indicators Research, 70(3), 243-255.
Easterlin, К. A. (2017). Paradox lost? Review of Behavioural Economics, 4(4), 311-339. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1561/105.00000068.
Elkoutour, M., & El Abboubi, M. (2024). A systematic review of happiness at work and cultural dimensions. International Journal of Accounting, Finance, Auditing, Management and Economics, 5(9), 92-120. doi: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13622993.
European Social Survey (2025). European Social Survey. Retrieved February 27, 2025, from https://ess.sikt.no/en/series/321b06ad-1b98-4b7d-93ad-ca8a24e8788a.
Eurostat (2025). Persons being happy in the last 4 weeks by sex, age, educational attainment and frequency. European Union. Retrieved February 27, 2025, from https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/databrowser/view/ilc_pw08$dv_426/default/table? lang=en &category-gol.gol lif.gol life aff https://doi.org/10.2908/ILC_PWO08.
Finuras, P. (2020). National Happiness and National Culture: What's The Link? Journal of Intercultural Management and Ethics, 3(4), 7-15. doi: https://doi.org/10.35478/jime.2020.4.02.
Givel, M. (2015). Mahayana Buddhism and Gross National Happiness in Bhutan. International Journal of Wellbeing, 5(2), 14-27. doi:10.5502/;jw.v5i2.2. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from https://internationaljournalofwellbeing.org/index. php/ijow/article/view/434/475
Givel, M. (2022). Buddhist happiness compared with other religious views of happiness. Dharma World, 49.
Givel, M. (2023). Gross National Happiness Policy Outputs in Bhutan from 1972 to 2014. Journal of Bhutan Studies, 47.
Gross National Happiness Index webpage (2024). Gross National Happiness Index. Retrieved Мау 9, 2024, from https://www.grossnationalhappiness.com/
Helliwell, J. F., Layard, Huang, H., Norton, M., Goff, L., & Wang, $. (2023a). World Happiness, Trust and Social Connections in Times of Crisis. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from https://worldhappiness.report/ed/2023/world-happiness-trust-and-social-connections-intimes-of-crisis/
Helliwell, J. F., Layard, R., Sachs, J. D., De Neve, J., Aknin, L. B., Wang, S., & Paculor, $. (2023b). World Happiness Report 2023. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from https://happinessreport.s3.amazonaws.com/2023/WHR-23.pdf
Hofstede, G., & Minkov, M. (2013). Values Survey Module 2013 Manual. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from https://geerthofstede.com/research-and-vsm/vsm-2013/
Hofstede, С. (1980). Culture 's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hofstede, G. (2011). Dimensionalizing Cultures: The Hofstede Model in Context. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2(1).
Hofstede, G. Webpage (2024). Retrieved May 9, 2024, from<https://geerthofstede.com/culturegeert-hofstede-gert-jan-hofstede/6d-model-of-national -culture/
Hofstede, G., & Hofstede, G.J. (2015). Dimension Data Matrix. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from http://www.geerthofstede.com/dimension-data-matrix
Hu, J. (2023). Research on the Relationship between Income and Happiness: From the Perspective of Economics. Frontiers in Business, Economics and Management, 7(1), 193195.
Hysa, E., & Mansi, E. (2020, March). Happiness and Economic Growth: Western Balkans and European Union. Happiness and Contemporary Society: Conference Proceedings Volume, Lviv, March, 20-21, 2020, 105-113. doi :https://do1.org/10.31108/7.2020.25.
Inglehart, R., Foa, R., Peterson, C., & Welzel, C. (2008). Development, freedom and rising happiness: A global perspective (1981-2007). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(4), 264-285.
Kahneman, D., & Krueger, A. B. (2006). Developments in the measurement of subjective wellbeing. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(1), 3-24. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/089533006776526030.
Kasman, S., & Kasman, A. (2020). The Impact of Obesity and Income on Happiness: Evidence from EU Countries. Panoeconomicus, 70(2), 303-319. doi: https://doi.org/10.2298/PAN200311001K.
Kulcsár, L. (2020). Theoretical and methodological considerations for research on quality of life and well-being. Statisztika Szemle, 98(11), 1239-1287. doi:https://doi.org/10.20311/stat2020.11.hu1239.
Li, L. M. W., Lun, У. M. C., Bond, М.Н. et al. (2024). The Role of Cultural Heterogeneity in Strengthening the Link Between Family Relationships and Life Satisfaction in 50 Societies. Journal of Happiness Studies 25(76). doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-02400768-1.
Mahmouei, М. F., & Razmi, М. J. (2023). The Relationship between Government Size and Happiness. Social Welfare Quarterly, 22(87). doi:10.32598/refahj.22.87.3593.2. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from https://refahj.uswr.ac.ir/browse.php?a 1d=3898&sid=1&slc lang=en&html=1
Mushtaq, M., € Siddiqui, D. A. (2020). Exploring the Nexus between Culture, Values, Institutions, Happiness and Philanthropy: A Global Evidence. International Journal of Social Work, 7(1), 16-55. doi: https://doi.org/10.5296/jsw.v711.16366.
Nath, L. (2018). An old monarchy, a new democracy and gross national happiness in Bhutan: A holistic approach for sustainable development. The Clarion-International Multidisciplinary Journal, 7(2), 38-49. doi:10.5958/2277-937X.2018.00024.2. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from https://www.indianjournals.com/ijor.aspx?target=ijor:cla&volume=7&issue=2&article= 006
Ng, W., & Diener, E. (2014). What matters to the rich and the poor? Subjective well-being, financial satisfaction, and post-materialistic needs across the world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(2), 326-338.
OECD (2025). Better Life Index. OECD. Retrieved February 27, 2025, from https://www.oecdbetterlifeindex.org/topics/life-satisfaction/.
Petka, М. (2019). Analysis of Happiness in EU Countries Using the Multi-Model Classification based on Models of Symbolic Data. Econometrics. Advances in Applied Data Analysis, 23(3), 15-25. doi:10.15611/eada.2019.3.02. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from https://sciendo.com/article/10.15611/eada.2019.3.02
Rajkumar, R. P. (2023). Cultural values and changes in happiness in 78 countries during the COVID-19 pandemic: An analysis of data from the World Happiness Reports. Positive Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology, 2. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1090340.
Spowart, S. (2022). A Path to Happiness. Happiness and Wellness-Biopsychosocial and Anthropological Perspectives. London: IntechOpen Limited. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen. 107413.
Steel, P., Taras, V., Uggerslev, K., & Bosco, Е. (2018). The Happy Culture: A Theoretical, Meta-Analytic, and Empirical Review of the Relationship between Culture and Wealth and Subjective Well-Being. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(2), 128-169. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868317721372.
Stevenson, B., € Wolfers, J. (2008). Economic Growth and Subjective Well-Being: Reassessing the Easterlin Paradox. National Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge.
Stober, E. O. (2023). The Role of Money in the Pursuit of Happiness. International Journal of Humanity Studies, 6(2), 169-177. doi:https://doi.org/10.24071/1jhs.v6i2.4714.
Taras, V., Kirkman, В. L., € Steel, P. (2010). Examining the impact of Culture's consequences: A three-decade, multilevel, meta-analytic review of Hofstede's cultural value dimensions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(3), 405-439. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018938.
Veenhoven, R. (2000). Wellbeing in the welfare state: Level not higher, distribution not more equitable. Journal of Comparative Policy Analysis, 2, 91-125. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from https://personal.eur.nl/veenhoven/Pub2000s/2000b-full.pdf
Veenhoven, R. (2007). Measures of Gross National Happiness. Statistics, Knowledge and Policy. Measuring and fostering the progress of societies, OECD, 231-253.
Veenhoven, R. (2012). Cross-national differences in happiness: Cultural measurement bias or effect of culture? International Journal of Wellbeing, 2(4), 333-353. doi:10.5502/jw.v2.14.4. Retrieved May 9, 2024, from https://www .internationaljournalofwellbeing.org/index.php/ijow/article/view/98/267
Veenhoven, К. (2020). World Database of Happiness - A findings archive'. In: Welsch, H., Maddison, D., & Rehdanz, К. (Eds.): Handbook of Wellbeing, Happiness and the Environment. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing. Chapter 2, 25-45. doi: https://doi.org/10.4337/9781788119344.00009.
Veenhoven, К. Buijt, L, & Burger, М. J. (2022). Online 'Findings-Archive': a new Tool for Research Synthesis. International Journal of Innovation, Scientific Research and Review, 4(5), 2774-2784
World Happiness Report (2025). World Happiness Report. Gallup, the Oxford Wellbeing Research Centre, the UN Sustainable Development Solutions Network, & the WHR's Editorial Board. Retrieved February 27, 2025, from https://worldhappiness.report/data/.
Zagorski, К. Evans, M. D. R., Kelley, J., & Piotrowska, P. (2013). Does National Income Inequality Affect Individuals" Quality of Life in Europe? Inequality, Happiness, Finances, and Health. Soc Indic Res 117, 1089-1110. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205013-0390-z.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2025. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the "License"). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
In a broader context, the pursuit of happiness has become one of the most important goals of a modern human. Despite substantial research on this topic, few studies have examined the links between happiness and cultural dimensions and none have looked at the Member States of the European Union in this context. This study was an attempt to fill this gap by examining the relationship between the rankings of the 26 EU Member States on the World Happiness Report and Hofstedes six cultural dimensions. Pearson correlation coefficients and linear regression coefficients were calculated to examine the relationships vía the use of the SPSS software. The results show a relatively strong positive relationship between happiness and Indulgence and Individualism, while the relationship between happiness and the cointegration of the cultural dimensions of Uncertainty Avoidance and Power Distance is relatively strong but negative. No correlation is found between happiness and the Masculinity and Long-term Orientation dimensions. Thus, a permissive and individualistic social environment is the main contributor to happiness in each nation, while excessive social uncertainty and power distance are the main detractors. Overall, these cultural dimensions must be given more attention if the European Union is to have happier Member States.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 Department of Economy, Eszterházy Karoly Catholic University, Eger, Hungary