New research on consumer behavior throws light on what creates a mindset that leads customers to keep on shopping. What underlies shopping momentum is a shift from a deliberation mindset to one of implementation. In the deliberation mindset, the potential customer evaluates the pros and cons of a purchase, rather in the manner suggested by the theory of utility maximization. However, once they have shifted to the implementation mindset, they are focused on purchasing - and this is just what they do. When they make the shift to an implementation mindset, they will carry on shopping without engaging their evaluative capacity until something intervenes to break the momentum.
Retailers have long believed that getting potential customers through the door is one of their main challenges. Marketers seek to lure customers with attractive offers and loss leaders, and once through the door, in-store marketing sets out to ensure that the first purchase will be followed by subsequent ones. This is the same strategy employed by catalogues which make use of extensive mailing campaigns spiced with attractive offers; experience shows that once a customer has the catalogue in their hands they will continue to buy from it.
Yet as every store or catalogue category manager will tell you, this does not always prove to be the case. Not every customer footfall translates into a stream of purchases, and not every catalogue subscription produces sales. All too often the customer footfall remains just that - though the individual has been attracted into the store and might indeed purchase a single item, they often leave without buying anything further. Even worse, on many occasions they make no purchase at all.
One possible explanation for this is supplied by the theory of purchasing behavior, which views each of the customer's purchases as a "utility-maximizing choice". In this explanation, the customer evaluates each purchase rationally and separately, according to the value the product or service might offer them. In support of this theory, there is a good deal of evidence that consumers do indeed make rational choices. The plethora of consumer magazines and comparison websites are there to supply the hunger for more information about consumer products. It is also true that many people sometimes research the alternatives on offer before making a significant purchase.
But is this the whole story? In the utility-maximizing view of shopping behavior, the impact of budgetary constraints suggests that once any single purchase is made, this would make each subsequent purchase less likely - the very opposite of what the marketers are seeking to encourage. And where does impulse buying fit in to this view of shoppers' behavior? Many an intrepid shopper has found upon returning from an expedition that they are loaded with goods they had not originally set out to buy. The utility-maximizing view of purchasing behavior does not provide a full explanation of what is happening here. So what is really going on?
It's all a matter of mindsets
Recent research by Ravi Dhar, Joel Uber and Uzma Khan throws new light on why some people just carry on shopping until they drop - a phenomenon which the researchers call shopping momentum. They believe that what underlies this momentum is a change in mindsets - a shift from a deliberation mindset to an implementation mindset. It has already been established that such a shift will change both the customer's cognition and their behavior. In the deliberation mindset, the potential customer evaluates the pros and cons of a purchase, rather in the manner suggested by the theory of utility maximization. However, once they have shifted to the implementation mindset, they are focused on purchasing - and this is just what they do. When they make the shift to an implementation mindset they will carry on shopping without engaging their evaluative capacity until something intervenes to break the momentum.
To gain a full understanding of this phenomenon, the researchers conducted a number of experiments, each of which is helpful in illustrating various important aspects of shopping momentum.
In the first, they looked at how the purchase of a primary item can influence the purchase of a second, unrelated one, which they described as the target item. The basic idea here is that the first purchase creates shopping momentum, therefore increasing the likelihood of subsequent purchases. However this likelihood depends on the buyer's current mindset, which can be understood as the commitment to the purchasing process, itself based on a continuum between a purely deliberative and a purely implementation-based buying orientation.
After being paid for completing a questionnaire, the participants (all of them students) were told that they could either keep all the money or use some of it to make purchases. The researchers divided them into three groups. A "high driver" group was offered a highly attractive primary item, namely an educational CD, that was likely to be purchased by members of this group. For the "low driver" group, the researchers selected a relatively unattractive primary item, namely a lightbulb, which had a lower likelihood of being purchased. A third control group was not given any purchase opportunity. The chosen target item was a key chain, which was unrelated to either of the primary items. A separate test ensured that while students were more likely to purchase the CD than the lightbulb, they believed that both of these items were of comparable monetary value. The prices for the driver item and target item were held constant across all experimental conditions.
As expected, the attractiveness of the primary item had a significant influence on whether it was chosen. Seventy- two percent of the high-driver group purchased the educational CD, while only 15 percent of the low-driver group chose to buy the lightbulb. This result is not surprising in itself, as people tend to buy products they regard as important and personally relevant.
More significantly, and as predicted, people tended to keep on buying after making initial purchases they considered attractive. Those who made the initial purchase of the CD were much more likely to keep on shopping and purchase an unrelated item. Figure 1 shows that 65 percent of those who had bought the CD (the high driver group) went on to buy the key chain, but only 37 percent of those who had bought the lightbulb (the lowdriver group) did so. Moreover, the probability of purchasing the key chain did not differ significantly between those who were initially offered a lightbulb and the control group, who were not offered any initial purchase opportunity (Refer to figure 1).
Although these results show that shopping momentum is a real phenomenon, they do not reveal what drives it. Is it the attractiveness of the item, or the act of making a purchase?
If it is truly the act of purchasing and not merely a positive effect, the likelihood of buying the target item should be lower when the primary item is given to participants as a free gift than when they have to pay for it. This is exactly what the second experiment showed.
For this purpose, the researchers divided participants into three groups. As before, they were paid to fill out a questionnaire. This time, those in the high driver group were offered a pen for purchase before being offered the key chain at half the price of the pen. The low driver group was offered the pen as a free gift before being offered the key chain at the same price as the first group. In the control condition, only the target key chain was offered for purchase.
The results of this study clearly confirm the prediction that shopping momentum is triggered by the act of purchasing itself, as the participants who did not have to pay for the pen were less likely to buy the target item. As figure 2 shows, 62.5 percent of those who were offered the pen purchased it, and 77.5 percent of this group then went on to buy the key chain. However, just 52.5 percent of those who received the pen as a free gift went on to purchase the key chain. There was no significant difference between those who had received the pen as a free gift and the control group in terms of whether they purchased the key chain (Refer to figure 2).
How thinking about purchasing changes behavior
It could be argued, of course, that although these experiments show that an initial purchase stimulates further purchases, it does not prove that what underlies this momentum is a shift in mindsets. The researchers therefore devised a further experiment to gain additional insights into the psychological process underlying the postulated shift. They predicted that the mere act of purchasing should induce an implementation mindset (thoughts about when, how and where to act) rather than a deliberation mindset (thinking about the pros and cons of a decision). Consequently, the number of implementation- and deliberation-related thoughts remembered in a recall task was expected to shed more light on the activation of a particular mindset.
Prior to the recall task, the students were paid $1 to participate in the study and were randomly assigned to two groups: one was given the option of making a purchase and the second, a control group, was not. The first group were told that they could either keep their dollar or spend 25 cents of it on an apple or bag of chips. Both these items had been selected because they had previously been shown to be attractive to students at that price. In consequence, and consistently with the previous results, 97 percent of those offered the option to purchase a snack, chose to do so.
Both groups were then asked to read through twelve thoughts that a person might have when deciding whether or not to purchase a car, and what they would need to do in order to carry out the purchase after that decision had been made. The thoughts included implementation as well as deliberation thoughts that had been previously revealed in a pretest. After an appropriate delay, the participants were asked to recall as many of the twelve thoughts as they could. The number of each type of thought remembered in this task was expected to shed more light on the activation of a particular mindset.
The results show that those participants who had already made a purchase recalled significantly more implementation thoughts and fewer deliberation thoughts than those who had not. It is therefore clear that the initial purchase changed the mindset of this randomly selected group - those that had already made a purchase developed an implementation mindset, as shown by the thoughts they later recalled.
This experiment confirms that making a purchase leads to a shift in mindset. But for the theory of shopping momentum to be true, this shift should cause further purchases. To test this, the researchers decided to manipulate the type of mindset by asking the participants to engage in an "implementation versus deliberation" thinking style about an unrelated purchase decision before being offered the target item. The researchers expected this to impact the type of mindset without participants actually purchasing a primary item before the target item.
To induce the particular mindset, the participants were randomly assigned to two groups. The first (the deliberation group) was asked to write down four pros and four cons of buying a car. The second (the implementation group) was asked to write down eight steps that they would need to take in order to purchase a car. Both groups were paid for completing this exercise, and were subsequently given the opportunity to purchase a key chain.
The results clearly show the impact of the type of mindset on the propensity to purchase. Whereas 65.8 percent of the implementation group bought the key chain, only 41 percent of the deliberation group did so. It was not an initial purchase that produced this difference, but merely the development of an implementation mindset as a result of thinking about the steps required to make a purchase.
How shopping momentum can be brought to a halt
To return to the question we posed at the beginning of this article, why do some people keep on shopping while others quickly stop, apparently satiated? The answer is that shopping momentum appears to be a fairly fragile and short-term phenomenon. It should therefore continue only for as long as there is little to disrupt it, and should be brought to a halt as soon as the customer has any cause for deliberation.
The authors conducted two experiments to identify two boundary conditions of shopping momentum. The results show firstly that if the shopper needs to draw upon a separate source of funds (for example earning money from two different tasks to make a subsequent payment), this will break the momentum. Secondly, if their attention is drawn to an unfavorable price contrast, this can also interrupt the momentum. This latter point is particularly important for marketers.
To illustrate this phenomenon, the researchers conducted an experiment to investigate whether price discounts shift attention back to a deliberation mindset, which should then disrupt shopping momentum; specifically, they compared the effect of a deep discount on shopping momentum to that of a lesser discount. They expected the deep discount to have a counterproductive and contrasting effect on subsequent offers, because these would appear less attractive than the initial one. In other words, prior deep discounts could serve as reference points for consumers when making subsequent purchases, therefore reducing the attractiveness of items offered without such deep discounts.
There is no doubt that discounts can increase the likelihood of a primary purchase, but how do such pricing strategies influence subsequent purchases in the light of the shopping momentum effect?
In the corresponding experiment, two groups were offered a pen at the same price of 18 rupees, the only difference being the supposed initial price. Whereas one group was told that the pen's market price was 40 rupees (in other words, they were receiving a deep discount), members of the other were informed that the market price was 25 rupees, so they were being granted a regular discount. After being offered the primary item, each group was given the opportunity to purchase the key chain. As expected, where participants were offered the pen at a deep discount, fewer went on to purchase the target item than in the case where the discount was lower (see figure 3). The participants apparently discounted the target item at the same rate as the pen, so it appeared to be a less attractive offer at its regular price.
Implications for marketing
Marketers will need to think about how they can use shopping momentum to get the most out of their potential customers. The marketing implications of the research can be summarized as follows:
* It is only once a customer makes the shift to an implementation mindset that they will carry on shopping relatively unconstrained by concerns about value.
* Free gifts and other incentives do not in themselves induce shopping momentum; however, they can help to create it, if they are closely linked to a purchase and make it more attractive.
* Shopping momentum is likely to be a fairly shortterm phenomenon that will be brought to a halt as soon as the customer's attention is shifted to deliberation. For instance, steep discounts can affect the attractiveness of subsequent offers because focusing on financial resources leads to a deliberation mindset and interrupts the momentum.
The work of Ravi Dhar, Joel Uber and Uzma Khan therefore has important implications. Many a marketing strategy is likely to be reshaped as a result of their insights into what causes some shoppers to just keep on shopping.
The article is adapted with permission from the Journal of Marketing Research published by the American Marketing Association: Dhar, R., Huber, J. and Khan U., "The Shopping Momentum Effect", Vol. XLIV (August 2007), 370-378.
» There is no doubt that discounts can increase the likelihood of a primary purchase, but how do such pricing strategies influence subsequent purchases in the light of the shopping momentum effect? «
{Deutsche Zusammenfassung }
Neue psych ologische Erkenntnisse zum
Phänomen des Kaufrauschs
Ravi Dhar, Joel Huber und Uzma Khan
Der Handel setzt gezielt Marketinginstrumente wie bspw. Sonderpreisaktionen ein, um Konsumenten zur Kaufentscheidung zu bewegen. Mindestens ebenso wichtig wie das Zustandekommen eines Erstkaufs ist es jedoch für Händler, den Kunden auch nach dem Erstkauf zu weiteren Kaufentscheidungen zu bewegen. Tatsächlich zeigen Studien zu Impulskäufen, dass Konsumenten häufig wesentlich mehr Produkte kaufen, als sie vor dem Einkauf geplant haben. Jedoch verlassen viele Kunden die Einkaufsstätte auch, ohne weitere Produkte neben dem ursprünglich intendierten Produkt zu erwerben. Die traditionelle Annahme des nutzenmaximierenden Konsumenten vermag allerdings nicht zu erklären, unter welchen Umständen Konsumenten zur einen oder zur anderen Verhaltensweise neigen.
Wie Dhar/Huber/Khan in ihrem Beitrag zeigen, ist das Kaufverhalten von Konsumenten davon abhängig, ob sie entweder eher bewertungs- oder eher handlungsorientiert eingestellt sind. Eine eher bewertende Haltung geht mit dem Abwägen zwischen Vor- und Nachteilen eines Kaufs einher, während eine eher handlungsorientierte Haltung auf den Kaufakt an sich fokussiert. Ein Kaufrausch entsteht genau dann, wenn die Einstellung des Konsumenten vom einen in den anderen Zustand übergeht. Dieses Phänomen wurde in einer Reihe von Experimenten untersucht und empirisch bestätigt.
Die Autoren zeigen, dass die Attraktivität des Erstkaufs einen Einfluss auf den Kauf eines nachfolgenden Produktangebots hat. Sie stellten eine Kaufsituation unter Laborbedingungen dar, bei der die Teilnehmer einen gewissen Geldbetrag für Einkäufe zur Verfügung hatten. Untersuchungsteilnehmer, denen beim Erstkauf ein für sie besonders attraktives Produkt offeriert wurde, kauften mit höherer Wahrscheinlichkeit ein weiteres Produkt, das in keinerlei Zusammenhang zum ersten Produkt stand. Offensichtlich löste der Erstkauf bei diesen Konsumenten eine Art Kaufrausch aus, der sie zu einem weiteren Kaufakt antrieb.
Um weiter zu ergründen, ob die Wahrscheinlichkeit eines Folgekaufs wirklich auf eine veränderte Einstellung zurückzuführen ist, führten die Autoren ein weiteres Experiment durch. Hierbei erhielten alle Probanden beim Erstkauf dasselbe Produkt angeboten, wobei die erste Gruppe für das Produkt bezahlte, während die zweite Gruppe das Produkt als Geschenk erhielt. Eine dritte Gruppe erhielt weder die Kaufgelegenheit noch das Geschenk. Danach wurde allen Versuchsteilnehmern ein weiteres Produkt zum Kauf angeboten. Dabei zeigte sich, dass Untersuchungsteilnehmer, die das Geschenk erhalten hatten, mit einer geringeren Wahrscheinlichkeit die nachfolgende Kaufgelegenheit wahrnahmen. Insbesondere unterschieden sich diese Untersuchungsteilnehmer nicht von der Gruppe, die weder die anfängliche Kaufgelegenheit hatten noch das Geschenk erhielten. Offensichtlich führt nicht die Attraktivität eines Erstkaufs zur Entwicklung des affektiven Zustands der Kauflust, sondern die Kaufentscheidung an sich zu einer veränderten Einstellung hin zur Handlungsorientierung.
Darüber hinaus zeigen die Autoren in weiteren Untersuchungen, dass der Kaufrausch von Konsumenten unterbrochen werden kann. Dies ist insbesondere dann der Fall, wenn die Aufmerksamkeit des Konsumenten auf finanzielle Ressourcen gelenkt wird. Beispielsweise erzeugt ein Rabatt beim zuerst gekauften Produkt einen deutlichen Unterschied zum nachfolgend gekauften Produkt, wenn dieses nicht ebenfalls rabattiert ist. Dann nehmen Konsumenten eine bewertungsorientierte, abwägende Haltung ein und zeigen ein Verhalten, das nicht mit den beschriebenen Phänomenen korrespondiert.
Die Herausforderung für das Marketing besteht in der Erzeugung einer dauerhaften Kauflaune. Geschenke sind als Kaufanreize offensichtlich nur bedingt geeignet, auch Rabatte scheinen nur begrenzt wirksam. Kaufanreize sollten so beschaffen sein, dass die wahrgenommene Attraktivität von Produkten erhöht wird, diese tatsächlich gekauft werden und dabei keine Fokussierung auf finanzielle Ressourcen stattfindet. Vor diesem Hintergrund sind Bonussysteme für Unternehmen besonders attraktiv, da sie alle aufgeführten Bedingungen zur Erzeugung einer Handlungsorientierung bei Kaufentscheidungen zu erfüllen scheinen.
Den ausführlichen Artikel in englischer Sprache finden Sie in diesem Magazin auf Seite ...
... 8.
Dhar Ravi, Huber Joel and Khan Uzmar (2007): "The Shopping Momentum Effect", Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. XLIV (August 2007), 370-378.
Dhar Ravi and Itamar Simonson (1999), "Making Complementary Choices in Consumption Episodes: Highlighting Versus Balancing," Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 36 (February), 29-44.
Peter M. Gollwitzer (1990), "From Weighing to Willing: Approaching a Change Decision Through Pre- or Postdecisional Implementation," Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol. 45 (February), 41-46.
Peter M. Gollwitzer, Heinz Heckhausen and Brigit Steller (1990), "Deliberative and Implemental Mindsets: Cognitive Tuning Toward Congruous Thoughts and Information," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 59 (December), 1119-27.
Angela Lee and Brian Sternthal (1999), "The Effects of Positive Mood on Memory," Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 26 (2), 115-32.
The Authors
Ravi Dhar, School of Management, Yale University [email protected]
Joel Huber, Fuqua School of Business, Duke University, [email protected]
Uzma Khan, Graduate School of Business, Stanford University, [email protected]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright GfK Association, Marketing Intelligence Review May 2009
Abstract
New research on consumer behavior throws light on what creates a mindset that leads customers to keep on shopping. What underlies shopping momentum is a shift from a deliberation mindset to one of implementation. In the deliberation mindset, the potential customer evaluates the pros and cons of a purchase, rather in the manner suggested by the theory of utility maximization. However, once they have shifted to the implementation mindset, they are focused on purchasing - and this is just what they do. When they make the shift to an implementation mindset, they will carry on shopping without engaging their evaluative capacity until something intervenes to break the momentum. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer