Introduction
Bipolar disorder (BD) is associated with impairments in social and nonsocial cognitive functioning during both mood episodes [1, 2] and periods of remission [3–5]. Interestingly, not only patients but also their unaffected relatives exhibit deficits in cognitive abilities when compared to control subjects. These findings of a measurable, state-independent, heritable condition suggest that cognitive impairments may represent a so-called endophenotype for BD [6]. Psychiatric research is concerned with the identification of such markers to improve early diagnosis and consequently, clinical outcomes [7]. Whereas verbal learning has been identified to be the most likely impaired domain of nonsocial cognition [8, 9], a recent meta-analysis by Bora et al. revealed deficits in theory of mind as well as facial emotion recognition in first-degree relatives of patients suffering from BD [10].
The identification of specific endophenotypes could promote the understanding of the etiopathology and of genetic determinants of serious mental illnesses [11, 12]. However, findings are inconsistent and the profile of cognitive performance in relatives of patients with BD is not clear. Since research in the field of social cognition needs to choose a domain-specific approach to investigate its role as a potential trait marker for the illness, this study focused on emotional intelligence (EI) as assessed by the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT).
To date, only a limited number of studies have investigated EI in patients suffering from BD. According to these studies, they have lower levels of EI compared to healthy control subjects [13] but perform better than patients suffering from schizophrenia [14]. Moreover, EI performance seems to be associated with nonsocial cognitive performance, indicating a close relationship between general cognitive functioning and EI [14, 15]. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study using the full version of the MSCEIT in unaffected siblings of patients suffering from BD.
The primary objective of this study was to assess EI in unaffected siblings of patients suffering from bipolar-I-disorder in order to investigate the potential role of deficits in this domain as endophenotype for BD.
In addition, we compared the levels of EI and nonsocial cognition in patients, unaffected siblings, and control subjects from the general community to replicate earlier findings.
Patients and Methods
All procedures contributing to this work complied with the standards of the local Ethics Committees and were conducted according to Good Clinical Practice standards on human experimentation and the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Study procedures were performed by a trained research team consisting of psychiatrists and master-level clinical psychologists.
Participants
Patient recruitment took place at the Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics of the Medical University of Innsbruck. The majority of patients were treated on a regular basis at a specialized unit for outpatients with BD, while the remaining patients had been treated as inpatients in the past. All patients met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV) criteria for bipolar-I-disorder as assessed by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.) [16]. Psychopathology was assessed by using the German version [17] of the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) [18] and the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) [19]. Functional outcome was assessed using the Personal and Social Performance Scale [20].
Patients had to be between 18 and 65 years of age, clinically stable without hospitalization for at least 6 months, and without any change in psychopharmacological treatment within 3 months before study inclusion. Any other axis I disorder as well as axis II disorder as assessed by the Structured Clinical Interview for Axis-II-Disorders according to DSM-IV (SCID-II) [21] led to study exclusion.
Unaffected siblings and control subjects were recruited through advertisements in local newspapers. In both groups, the M.I.N.I. [16] and the SCID-II [21] were used to screen for current psychiatric disorders. A current axis I or II disorder would have led to study exclusion. Siblings were not related to the patients enrolled in the study. Diagnosis of their affected relatives had to be confirmed through written documentation of a licensed psychiatrist. Control subjects had to have a negative personal or family history of any DSM-IV psychotic disorder.
A brief medical screening interview was used in all groups to exclude subjects with any clinical condition that might interfere with cognitive performance. Premorbid intelligence was assessed with the Multiple Choice Vocabulary Test (Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest B, MWT-B) [22]. All participants provided written informed consent.
Emotional intelligence
Each participant completed the German pencil-and-paper version [23] of the MSCEIT [24, 25]. This instrument consists of 141 items and provides eight task scores that measure the four branches of EI: perceiving, using, understanding, and managing emotions. “Perceiving emotions” measures emotion perception abilities in faces and pictures, while the “using emotions” branch is about using emotions to enhance and facilitate cognitive processes and assesses the associations between emotions and sensations. The “understanding emotions” branch tests the knowledge how emotions interact with each other and change over time, and the “managing emotions” part measures the ability to deal with and regulate emotions in oneself but also in relationships with others. These branches cover all aspects of EI and can be assigned to the areas of emotional experiencing (perceiving + using emotions) and emotional reasoning (=“strategic” EI; understanding + managing emotions). Response types included multiple-choice format and 5-point Likert ratings. Similar to other intelligence tests, the average score is 100 with a standard deviation (SD) of 15.
The MSCEIT is both content and structurally valid (overall reliability r = 0.93) showing discriminate validity from measures of analytic intelligence and many personality constructs [26].
Nonsocial cognition
The German version [27] of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) [28] was used to measure nonsocial cognition. This battery covers several nonsocial cognitive functions (verbal memory, working memory, motor speed, attention and processing speed, executive functioning, and verbal fluency) and has been suggested to serve as a means of neuropsychological assessment for patients with various psychotic disorders [29]. The BACS composite score is calculated by standardizing the average of those six measures by dividing that average by the SD of the average in the normative sample. It requires less than 35 min to complete, yields a high completion rate in patients, and has high reliability.
Statistical methods and data analysis
Prior to the analysis, all metric variables were checked for deviations from normality based on the skewness of the distribution, where skewness values above 0.5 or below −0.5 were considered as an indication of non-normality. Variables violating the normality assumption were transformed to approximate normality by an appropriate transformation, for example, square root transformation, to allow parametric testing for all metric variables.
The three groups (patients, unaffected siblings, and control subjects) were compared with respect to sociodemographic characteristics by one-way analysis of variance or chi-square test, depending on the variable type (metric or categorical, respectively). One-way analysis of variance was also used to compare the three groups with regard to EI and nonsocial cognition. As the three groups differed significantly in their level of education, additional analyses of covariance with adjustment for education were performed. Post-hoc pairwise group comparisons were done by means of the least significant difference method, provided that the overall comparison had yielded statistical significance (p < 0.05). In the case of three groups, this sequential procedure yields valid p-values without further correction [30].
Results
Study sample
The study sample consisted of 54 patients suffering from bipolar-I-disorder, 54 unaffected siblings, and 80 control subjects. The groups were comparable in terms of age; however, the proportion of males was higher in the patient group compared to the sibling group. Moreover, control subjects had significantly higher education levels than patients.
Patients were chronically ill but clinically stable with only mild symptoms as shown by very low YMRS and MADRS scores. About 44% had experienced psychotic symptoms in the course of the illness. Background sample characteristics are presented in Table 1.
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Variable | Group | Comparison | |||||
(1) Patients (N = 54) | (2) Siblings (N = 54) | (3) Control subjects (N = 80) | Statistic | p-value | Pairwise comparison | ||
Age, mean ± SD | Years | 45.9 ± 11.4 | 44.7 ± 13.2 | 44.6 ± 10.3 | F = 0.22 | 0.806 | – |
Sex, N (%) | Male | 27 (50.0) | 17 (31.5) | 33 (41.2) | χ2 = 3.83 | 0.147 | (1) > (2)a |
Female | 27 (50.0) | 37 (68.5) | 47 (58.8) | ||||
Education, mean ± SD | Years | 13.0 ± 2.9 | 13.9 ± 4.1 | 14.5 ± 3.2 | F = 3.04 | 0.052 | (1) < (3)b |
Marital status, N (%) | Single | 19 (35.2) | 20 (37.0) | 31 (38.8 | χ2 = 6.76, df = 6 | 0.343 | – |
Married | 22 (40.7) | 29 (53.7) | 38 (47.5) | ||||
Divorced | 12 (22.2) | 4 (7.4) | 10 (12.5) | ||||
Widowed | 1 (1.9) | 1 (1.9) | 1 (1.3) | ||||
Premorbid intelligence (MWT-B, percentile rank, mean ± SD) | 67.4 ± 24.5 | 75.5 ± 23.5 | 79.2 ± 19.2 | F = 4.56 | 0.012 | (1) < (3)c | |
Duration of illness, mean ± SD | Years | 14.1 ± 10.5 | – | – | |||
History of psychotic episodes, N (%) | 24 (44.4) | – | – | ||||
Psychotropic medication, N (%)d | |||||||
Mood stabilizer | 35 (64.8) | – | – | ||||
Antipsychotic | 39 (72.2) | – | – | ||||
Antidepressant | 24 (44.4) | – | – | ||||
Benzodiazepine | 5 (9.3) | – | – | ||||
MADRS, mean ± SD | 6.7 ± 6.4 | – | – | ||||
YMRS, mean ± SD | 3.3 ± 4.2 | – | – | ||||
PSP, mean ± SD | 69.3 ± 12.7 | – | – |
Abbreviations: MADRS, Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale; MWT-B, the Multiple Choice Vocabulary Test (Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest B); N, sample size; PSP, Personal and Social Performance Scale; SD, standard deviation; YMRS, Young Mania Rating Scale.
a Almost significantly higher percentage of males in the bipolar patient group as compared to the sibling group (p = 0.0502).
b Significantly lower level of education in bipolar patients than in control subjects (p = 0.008).
c Significantly lower premorbid intelligence (MWT-B percentile rank) in bipolar patients than in control subjects (p = 0.003).
d Category “mood stabilizer” includes lithium, valproate, and lamotrigine.
Emotional intelligence
A comparison of the three groups with regard to EI is given in Table 2. Patients achieved significantly lower MSCEIT total scores and performed significantly worse in almost all MSCEIT branches compared to unaffected siblings and control subjects. MSCEIT (sub)scores were comparable between patients with a history of psychosis and those without. Performance of the three groups was comparable in the “perceiving emotions” branch (lower scores in patients with a tendency to statistical significance). Patients’ performance in the “using emotions” branch was comparable with that of siblings but significantly worse compared to control subjects.
Table 2. Comparison of bipolar patients, unaffected siblings of bipolar patients, and control subjects with respect to emotional intelligence.
MSCEIT subscalea | Group | Statistics | |||||||||||
(1) Patients (N = 54) | (2) Siblings (N = 54) | (3) Control subjects (N = 80) | One-way analysis of variance | Analysis of covariance with adjustment for education | |||||||||
Mean ± SD | Mean ± SD | Mean | F | poverall | 1 vs. 2 | 1 vs. 3 | 2 vs. 3 | F | poverall | 1 vs. 2 | 1 vs. 3 | 2 vs. 3 | |
Perceiving emotions | 98.0 ± 17.7 | 104.9 ± 11.7 | 103.5 ± 15.7 | 2.48 | 0.087 | (0.051) | (0.052) | n.s. | 2.29 | 0.104 | (0.051) | (0.075) | n.s. |
Using emotions | 100.9 ± 18.5b | 105.1 ± 16.3 | 107.8 ± 13.2 | 2.89 | 0.058 | n.s. | 0.017 | n.s. | 1.78 | 0.171 | n.s. | (0.054) | n.s. |
Understanding emotions | 89.8 ± 18.5c | 100.1 ± 17.2 | 100.4 ± 14.7 | 7.13 | 0.001 | 0.002 | 0.001 | n.s. | 4.86 | 0.009 | 0.004 | 0.013 | n.s. |
Managing emotions | 93.1 ± 18.7c | 100.2 ± 17.8b | 106.4 ± 13.3 | 10.38 | <0.001 | 0.029 | <0.001 | 0.035 | 7.70 | 0.001 | 0.048 | <0.001 | (0.079) |
Experiential EI | 99.4 ± 17.7c | 105.9 ± 14.5 | 106.2 ± 15.2 | 3.09 | 0.048 | 0.043 | 0.021 | n.s. | 2.52 | 0.084 | 0.049 | 0.049 | n.s. |
Strategic EI | 88.8 ± 20.5c | 99.9 ± 20.2 | 104.5 ± 14.3 | 12.09 | <0.001 | 0.001 | <0.001 | n.s. | 8.66 | <0.001 | 0.002 | <0.001 | n.s. |
MSCEIT total score | 94.2 ± 19.4c | 104.2 ± 17.7 | 106.7 ± 15.1 | 8.87 | <0.001 | 0.002 | <0.001 | n.s. | 6.50 | 0.002 | 0.004 | <0.001 | n.s. |
Abbreviations: EI, emotional intelligence; MSCEIT, Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test; N, sample size; n.s., nonsignificant (p > 0.10); SD, standard deviation.
a All MSCEIT (sub-)scales were subjected to a square-root transformation prior to statistical testing to obtain approximate normality.
b Significantly lower than in the two other groups, p < 0.05.
c Significantly lower than in the control group, p < 0.05 (after adjustment for education, only trend-level significance is retained, p < 0.10).
In contrast, unaffected siblings and control subjects achieved comparable MSCEIT total scores and comparable levels in three out of four MSCEIT branches (“perceiving,” “using,” and “understanding emotions”). However, siblings reached significantly lower levels in the “managing emotions” branch. After adjustment for education, this significance was lost.
Nonsocial cognition
Compared to unaffected siblings and control subjects, patients were significantly impaired in overall nonsocial cognitive functioning (BACS composite score) and in most subtests of the BACS. The three groups were comparable with regard to verbal memory, and patients and unaffected siblings achieved comparable levels in verbal fluency.
Siblings’ and control subjects’ performance in the token motor task differed significantly with siblings achieving lower scores. On the other hand, siblings exhibited higher scores in digit sequencing with a tendency to statistical significance. After adjustment for education, this difference reached the level of significance (Table 3). Patients with a history of psychosis scored significantly lower than those without in the digit sequencing task (mean ± SD, 38.7 ± 10.7 vs. 45.5 ± 9.1, Z = –2.01, p = 0.045), the Tower of London task (38.5 ± 12.0 vs. 47.9 ± 6.3, Z = –2.22, p = 0.028), and in overall nonsocial cognitive functioning (38.5 ± 8.4 vs. 46.8 ± 10.7, Z = –2.10, p = 0.035).
Table 3. Comparison of bipolar patients, unaffected siblings of bipolar patients, and control subjects with respect to nonsocial cognition.
BACS subscale (t-scores) | Group | Statistics | |||||||||||
(1) Patients (N = 29) | (2) Siblings (N = 53) | (3) Control subjects (N = 79) | One-way analysis of variance | Analysis of covariance with adjustment for education | |||||||||
Mean ± SD | Mean ± SD | Mean ± SD | F | poverall | 1 vs. 2 | 1 vs. 3 | 2 vs. 3 | F | poverall | 1 vs. 2 | 1 vs. 3 | 2 vs. 3 | |
Verbal memorya | 53.6 ± 11.0 | 54.2 ± 12.1 | 56.0 ± 11.2 | 0.68 | 0.505 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | 0.28 | 0.755 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. |
Digit sequence | 42.9 ± 10.0b | 58.3 ± 7.6c | 55.5 ± 8.8 | 31.75 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | (0.064) | 31.57 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 0.028 |
Token motor task | 44.3 ± 8.3b | 51.8 ± 10.2d | 56.6 ± 8.2 | 20.50 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 0.003 | 20.15 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 0.004 |
Verbal fluency | 50.4 ± 10.3e | 52.5 ± 10.6 | 55.8 ± 11.3 | 3.02 | 0.051 | n.s. | 0.025 | n.s. | 2.24 | 0.110 | n.s. | (0.051) | n.s. |
Symbol coding | 40.6 ± 10.3b | 50.1 ± 11.1 | 49.5 ± 8.5 | 10.58 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | n.s. | 10.18 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | n.s. |
Tower of Londona | 44.6 ± 9.9b | 52.2 ± 7.8 | 52.8 ± 6.6 | 11.14 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | n.s. | 10.35 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | n.s. |
Composite score | 43.7 ± 10.3b | 55.4 ± 10.9 | 57.2 ± 9.1 | 20.26 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | n.s. | 20.00 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | n.s. |
Abbreviations: BACS, Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia; N, sample size; n.s., nonsignificant (p > 0.10); SD, standard deviation.
a A square-root transformation (√[maximum score – observed score]) was performed prior to statistical testing to obtain approximate normality.
b Significantly lower than in the two other groups, p < 0.05.
d Significantly lower than in the control group, p < 0.05.
(e)Significantly lower than in the healthy control group, p < 0.05; after adjustment for education, only trend-level significance is retained, p < 0.10.
c Significantly higher than in the control group after adjustment for education.
Discussion
This study investigated a sample of patients suffering from bipolar-I-disorder, unaffected siblings of patients with bipolar-I-disorder, and control subjects to assess whether EI as measured by the MSCEIT might serve as a marker of genetic risk for BD. Our finding of comparable EI in siblings and control subjects does not support this hypothesis. In addition, and based on previous findings on social-cognitive impairments in patients suffering from BD [14], we examined nonsocial cognition.
Patients’ mean EI scores lay within general population norms, which is in accordance with the findings of Varo et al., who assessed EI in euthymic subjects suffering from BD and found adequate EI performance in about 88% of study participants [15]. In line with a recent study by Aparicio et al. [13], patients included in this study were significantly impaired in overall EI (MSCEIT total score) and achieved significantly lower scores in most MSCEIT branches compared to control subjects. However, the two studies obtained divergent results in the areas of emotional experiencing (“perceiving emotions” and “using emotions”). In this study, patients showed significant impairments in the “using emotions” branch of the MSCEIT and did not differ from control subjects with regard to “perceiving emotions,” whereas Aparicio and coworkers reported the opposite. Further studies are needed to investigate these contradictory findings. It has to be mentioned, however, that emotional experiencing is seen as a “lower level” social cognitive ability [31] and that both studies concordantly point to impairments of higher level social cognitive functioning in patients with BD.
Siblings of patients with BD and control subjects had comparable levels of overall EI and performed comparably in most MSCEIT branches, except in the “managing emotions” branch. Task performance of siblings was significantly lower in this area, although significance was lost after adjustment for education. Similarly, Green et al. have shown that unaffected relatives of patients with BD tend to use maladaptive strategies of emotion regulation (catastrophizing, self-blame) [32], and a functional magnetic resonance study reported on deficits in emotion regulation through reappraisal [33]. Moreover, in contrast to our findings, a recent study using the Matrics Consensus Cognitive Battery, which includes the “emotion regulation” branch of the MSCEIT, did not find emotion regulation impairments in relatives [9]. However, the small sample investigated in that study comprised both individuals with a first-degree sibling or a parent with BD I or II and is therefore not entirely comparable with ours.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study investigating EI as covered by the four branches of the MSCEIT in unaffected siblings of patients suffering from BD. Our results suggest that deficits in EI do not represent an endophenotype for the illness. In contrast, previous studies on social cognitive abilities in relatives of patients suffering from BD identified impairments in theory of mind as strongest candidates for an independent trait marker of BD [10, 34, 35].
Similar to the results regarding EI and in line with previous findings [36, 37], patients included in this study achieved a significantly lower BACS composite score as well as lower scores in most BACS subtests compared to control subjects, indicating impairments in working memory, motor speed, attention and processing speed, executive functioning, and verbal fluency. Interestingly, patients, siblings, and controls showed comparable verbal memory. This is in line with a study by Sumiyoshi et al. and may be due to the kind of word learning task used in the BACS battery [38]. Importantly, previous studies have suggested that nonsocial cognition is responsible for the differences in social cognitive functioning between bipolar patients and healthy control subjects [39, 40] but also between patients suffering from BD or schizophrenia [14]. This highlights the importance of a combined training of social and nonsocial cognition in serious mental illnesses [41].
Unaffected siblings of patients suffering from BD showed a significant impairment in motor speed and, surprisingly, a significant better performance in the digit sequencing task, indicating better working memory compared to control subjects. Considering a previous review by Balanzá-Martinez et al. [8], who described verbal learning/memory and verbal working memory as the most suitable “endophenocognitypes” for BD and in view of the fact that this was the only BACS subscale (of six) in which siblings of BD patients achieved significantly higher values than healthy control subjects, this might be a chance finding.
Previous studies evaluating motor skills in relatives of patients with BD obtained conflicting results: whereas Kremen et al. did not find impaired motor functions in first-degree relatives [42], McDonough-Ryan et al. as well as Volkert et al. detected a lower motor speed in first-degree relatives of bipolar patients compared to healthy controls [43, 44]. These discrepancies show that the profile of nonsocial cognitive impairments in relatives of patients suffering from BD is still not clear.
This study has some limitations. First, there are general considerations about the MSCEIT that have to be taken into account when interpreting our findings and that have been discussed in detail elsewhere [45]. Briefly, the MSCEIT has been shown to have discriminate validity from measures of analytic intelligence and many personality constructs [26]; however, it is still a matter of discussion whether the test is sufficiently valid and independent from personality traits or general intelligence [46] and whether it is possible to reliably detect differences among individuals who score average and above average [47]. This last point may be of special relevance for the current investigation as both siblings’ and control subjects’ EI scores lay within general population norms.
The cross-sectional design of our study, which provides only a snapshot of cognitive abilities, and the missing of other measures of EI including self-report tools are further limitations. Moreover, although siblings and control subjects were screened for axis I or II disorders, subthreshold mood symptoms and their possible influence on EI performance were not assessed in these two groups. Patients, in turn, were clinically stable but not all of them fulfilled the criteria for euthymia. In addition, we have not assessed the number of mood episodes during the course of the illness. Accordingly, a potential influence of subthreshold symptomatology or the number of mood episodes on social and nonsocial cognition has to be taken into account. Furthermore, we have not assessed the potential influence of medication on the outcomes studied. However, as all patients were clinically stable, we can at least disregard efficacy differences between the different drugs. From a clinical perspective, it has to be mentioned that the MSCEIT items are relatively complex with a considerable amount of text to read, which might be very challenging for patients with nonsocial cognitive impairments. Despite a low degree of symptoms, many patients included in this study needed much more time than estimated in the handbook (30–45 min) to complete the test, which reduces the practicality for clinical use.
In conclusion, the identification of endophenotypes of severe mental illnesses has important clinical implications, for example, for early identification and primary prevention in at-risk individuals. Taken the findings of this study together, EI as measured with the MSCEIT does not seem to represent a genetic marker of risk for BD. In a next step, neuroimaging studies may address the question whether our finding of unremarkable performance in the MSCEIT in unaffected siblings of patients suffering from BD may be a consequence of compensatory mechanisms. More longitudinal studies are needed to investigate the stability and course of social and nonsocial cognitive impairments in first-degree relatives. Ideally, such studies should comprise genetic analyses to better differentiate at-risk groups.
Financial Support
This work was supported by a grant (KLI 366) of the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) awarded to Alex Hofer.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.
Data Availability Statement
Due to ethical concerns, supporting data cannot be made openly available.
References[1] Lee RS, Hermens DF, Scott J, Redoblado-Hodge MA, Naismith SL, Lagopoulos J, et al. A meta-analysis of neuropsychological functioning in first-episode bipolar disorders. J Psychiatr Res. 2014; 57: 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.06.019.
[2] Samame C. Social cognition throughout the three phases of bipolar disorder: a state-of-the-art overview. Psychiatry Res. 2013; 210 (3): 1275–1286. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.012.
[3] Robinson LJ, Thompson JM, Gallagher P, Goswami U, Young AH, Ferrier IN, et al. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2006; 93 (1–3): 105–115. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.02.016.
[4] Samame C, Martino DJ, Strejilevich SA. An individual task meta-analysis of social cognition in euthymic bipolar disorders. J Affect Disord. 2015; 173: 146–153. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.055.
[5] Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. J Affect Disord. 2009; 113 (1–2): 1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.009.
[6] Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003; 160 (4): 636–645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636.
[7] Sagar R, Pattanayak RD. Potential biomarkers for bipolar disorder: where do we stand? Indian J Med Res. 2017; 145 (1): 7–16. doi: 10.4103/ijmr.IJMR_1386_16.
[8] Balanzá-Martinez V, Rubio C, Selva-Vera G, Martinez-Aran A, Sanchez-Moreno J, Salazar-Fraile J, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes (endophenocognitypes) from studies of relatives of bipolar disorder subjects: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008; 32 (8): 1426 –38. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.019.
[9] Calafiore D, Rossell SL, Van Rheenen TE. Cognitive abilities in first-degree relatives of individuals with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2018; 225: 147–152. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.029.
[10] Bora E, Ozerdem A. Social cognition in first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017; 27 (4): 293–300. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.02.009.
[11] Allen AJ, Griss ME, Folley BS, Hawkins KA, Pearlson GD. Endophenotypes in schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophr Res. 2009; 109 (1–3): 24–37. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.01.016.
[12] Braff DL. The importance of endophenotypes in schizophrenia research. Schizophr Res. 2015; 163 (1–3): 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.02.007.
[13] Aparicio A, Santos JL, Jimenez-Lopez E, Bagney A, Rodriguez-Jimenez R, Sanchez-Morla EM. Emotion processing and psychosocial functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2017; 135 (4): 339–350. doi: 10.1111/acps.12706.
[14] Frajo-Apor B, Kemmler G, Pardeller S, Plass T, Muhlbacher M, Welte AS, et al. Emotional intelligence and non-social cognition in schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder. Psychol Med. 2017; 47 (1): 35–42. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716002324.
[15] Varo C, Jimenez E, Sole B, Bonnin CM, Torrent C, Valls E, et al. Social cognition in bipolar disorder: focus on emotional intelligence. J Affect Disord. 2017; 217: 210–217. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.012.
[16] Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998; 59 (suppl 20): 22–33.
[17] Muhlbacher M, Egger C, Kaplan P, Simhandl C, Grunze H, Geretsegger C, et al. Reliability and concordance validity of a German version of the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS-D). Neuropsychiatr. 2011; 25 (1): 16–25.
[18] Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry. 1978; 133: 429–435. doi: 10.1192/bjp.133.5.429.
[19] Montgomery SA, Asberg M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry. 1979; 134: 382–389. doi: 10.1192/bjp.134.4.382.
[20] Morosini PL, Magliano L, Brambilla L, Ugolini S, Pioli R. Development, reliability and acceptability of a new version of the DSM-IV Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale (SOFAS) to assess routine social functioning. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2000; 101 (4): 323–329.
[21] Wittchen H-U, Wunderlich U, Gruschwitz S, Zaudig M. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV (SKID). Göttingen, Germany: Beltz-Test, 1996.
[22] Lehrl S, Triebig G, Fischer B. Multiple Choice Vocabulary Test MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence. Acta Neurol Scand. 1995; 91 (5): 335–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1995.tb07018.x.
[23] Steinmayr R, Schütz A, Hertel J, Schröder-Abé M. Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Test zur Emotionalen Intelligenz (MSCEIT™). Deutschsprachige Adaptation des Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT™) von John D. Mayer, Peter Salovey und David R. Caruso. Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber, 2011.
[24] Mayer JD, Salovey P, Caruso DR. Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelliegnce Test (MSCEIT) item booklet. Toronto, Canada: MHS Publishers, 2002.
[25] Mayer JD, Salovey P, Caruso DR. Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) user’s manual. Toronto, Canada: MHS Publishers, 2002.
[26] Brackett MA, Salovey P. Measuring emotional intelligence with the Mayer-Salovery-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). Psicothema. 2006; 18: 34–41.
[27] Sachs G, Winklbaur B, Jagsch R, Keefe RS. Validation of the German version of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS)—preliminary results. Eur Psychiatry. 2011; 26 (2): 74–77. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2009.10.006.
[28] Keefe RS, Goldberg TE, Harvey PD, Gold JM, Poe MP, Coughenour L. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophr Res. 2004; 68 (2–3): 283–297. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011.
[29] Hochberger WC, Hill SK, Nelson CL, Reilly JL, Keefe RS, Pearlson GD, et al. Unitary construct of generalized cognitive ability underlying BACS performance across psychotic disorders and in their first-degree relatives. Schizophr Res. 2016; 170 (1): 156–161. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.11.022.
[30] Levin JR, Serlin RC, Seaman MA. A controlled, powerful multiple-comparison strategy for several situations. Psychol Bull. 1994; 115 (1): 153–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.153.
[31] Mancuso F, Horan WP, Kern RS, Green MF. Social cognition in psychosis: multidimensional structure, clinical correlates, and relationship with functional outcome. Schizophr Res. 2011; 125: 143–151.
[32] Green MJ, Lino BJ, Hwang EJ, Sparks A, James C, Mitchell PB. Cognitive regulation of emotion in bipolar I disorder and unaffected biological relatives. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2011; 124 (4): 307–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01718.x.
[33] Kanske P, Schonfelder S, Forneck J, Wessa M. Impaired regulation of emotion: neural correlates of reappraisal and distraction in bipolar disorder and unaffected relatives. Transl psychiatry. 2015; 5: e497. doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.137.
[34] Reynolds MT, Van Rheenen TE, Rossell SL. Theory of mind in first degree relatives of individuals with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2014; 219 (2): 400–402. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.05.041.
[35] Santos JM, Pousa E, Soto E, Comes A, Roura P, Arrufat FX, et al. Theory of mind in euthymic bipolar patients and first-degree relatives. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2017; 205 (3): 207–212. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000595.
[36] Bortolato B, Miskowiak KW, Kohler CA, Vieta E, Carvalho AF. Cognitive dysfunction in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: a systematic review of meta-analyses. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015; 11: 3111–3125. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S76700.
[37] Bourne C, Aydemir O, Balanza-Martinez V, Bora E, Brissos S, Cavanagh JT, et al. Neuropsychological testing of cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar disorder: an individual patient data meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013; 128 (3): 149–162. doi: 10.1111/acps.12133.
[38] Sumiyoshi T, Toyomaki A, Kawano N, Kitajima T, Kusumi I, Ozaki N, et al. Verbal memory impairments in bipolar disorder: effect of type of word learning tasks. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2017; 71 (8): 570–571. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12536.
[39] Bora E, Vahip S, Gonul AS, Akdeniz F, Alkan M, Ogut M, et al. Evidence for theory of mind deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005; 112 (2): 110–116. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00570.x.
[40] Martino DJ, Strejilevich SA, Fassi G, Marengo E, Igoa A. Theory of mind and facial emotion recognition in euthymic bipolar I and bipolar II disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2011; 189 (3): 379–384. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.033.
[41] Lindenmayer JP, McGurk SR, Khan A, Kaushik S, Thanju A, Hoffman L, et al. Improving social cognition in schizophrenia: a pilot intervention combining computerized social cognition training with cognitive remediation. Schizophr Bull. 2013; 39 (3): 507–517. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs120.
[42] Kremen WS, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ, Pepple JR, Tsuang MT. Neuropsychological risk indicators for schizophrenia: a preliminary study of female relatives of schizophrenic and bipolar probands. Psychiatry Res. 1998; 79 (3): 227–240. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(98)00042-0.
[43] McDonough-Ryan P, DelBello M, Shear PK, Ris DM, Soutullo C, Strakowski SM. Academic and cognitive abilities in children of parents with bipolar disorder: a test of the nonverbal learning disability model. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002; 24 (3): 280–285. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.3.280.980.
[44] Volkert J, Haubner J, Kazmaier J, Glaser F, Kopf J, Kittel-Schneider S, et al. Cognitive deficits in first-degree relatives of bipolar patients: the use of homogeneous subgroups in the search of cognitive endophenotypes. J Neural Transm. 2016; 123 (8): 1001–1011. doi: 10.1007/s00702-016-1581-y.
[45] Frajo-Apor B, Kemmler G, Pardeller S, Huber M, Macina C, Welte AS, et al. Is emotional intelligence impaired in unaffected siblings of patients with schizophrenia? J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2017; 23 (7): 577–583. doi: 10.1017/S135561771700042X.
[46] Brody N. COMMENTARIES on "Seven Myths About Emotional Intelligence" and "Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Findings, and Implications". Psychol Inq. 2004; 15 (3): 216–238. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1503_03.
[47] Fiori M, Antonietti JP, Mikolajczak M, Luminet O, Hansenne M, Rossier J. What is the Ability Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) good for? An evaluation using item response theory. PLoS One. 2014; 9 (6):e98827. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098827.
Beatrice Frajo-Apor, E-mail: [email protected]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Psychiatric Association. This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (
Abstract
Background. Impairments in social and nonsocial cognition have been demonstrated in both patients suffering from bipolar disorder (BD) and their unaffected relatives and might therefore represent a heritable marker of risk. This study investigated the relevance of emotional intelligence (EI) as part of the emotion processing domain of social cognition in this regard. Methods. A total of 54 outpatients suffering from BD, 54 unaffected siblings, and 80 control subjects were investigated using the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) and the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS). Analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were performed with adjustment for the BACS composite score. The three groups were compared by one-way analysis of variance or chi-square test, depending on the variable type. As the three groups differed significantly in their level of education, additional ANCOVAs with adjustment for education were performed. Results. Patients achieved significantly lower levels of overall EI and overall nonsocial cognitive functioning compared to unaffected siblings and controls, whereas performance of the latter two groups was comparable in both domains. Conclusions. Due to comparable levels of EI in unaffected siblings of patients suffering from BD and control subjects, EI assessed by means of the MSCEIT does not represent an endophenotype for BD.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics: Division of Psychiatry I, Medical University Innsbruck, 6020 Innsbruck, Austria
2 Department of Psychiatry, General Hospital Brunico, 39031 Brunico, Italy