This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
1. Introduction
Mammalian respiration is a fundamentally rhythmic motor program, linking cycles of inhalation-exhalation to conform an oscillation of flow in the airways. Homeostatic and behavioral demands flexibly modulate the rate of this oscillation. In rats, respiratory rate varies from ~1 Hz during sleep, through ~3 Hz during quiet waking, up to 11 Hz during active behavior [1–4]. Discrete events like emitting a vocalization or uttering a word [3–6] or apneic reflexes to noxious smells [7] can also dramatically affect the duration of a respiratory cycle. Actively behaving rats perform other motor programs with marked rhythmicity. While staying in place rats can engage in a stereotyped multisensory exploration of the environment known as sniffing behavior [1, 8]. During sniffing, respiration, whisking, and head position all oscillate with rates in the 5–11 Hz. Moreover, all of them synchronize into a common cycle, with inhalation, vibrissae protraction, and head approach being followed by exhalation, vibrissae retraction, and head withdrawal [1].
Locomotion is rhythmic too, from the chaining of step cycles or strides. As quadrupeds increase their locomotion speed, they transition through three main gaits: walking, trotting, and galloping [9]. Rats switch from walking to trotting at ~0.5 m/s and from trotting to galloping at ~0.7–0.8 m/s [10, 11]. Each of these gaits is associated with specific rhythmic patterns. Walking is the less regular of the gaits, with two or three limbs contacting the ground at each time and frequent stops. Trotting involves the rhythmic alternation of diagonal pairs of limbs (i.e., fore right/hind left followed by fore left/hind right) while in galloping both forelimbs contact the ground close in time, followed by both hindlimbs [9, 10]. The duration of each step cycle, defined as the time between successive floor contacts of a given foot, decreases with speed [11, 12]. Thus, the rates of the related forces acting on the body increase accordingly. Because of the left/right alternations in walking and trotting, the body experiences two cycles of rising and falling for each stride. Thus, the rate of stride forces acting on the body is twice that of stride itself [13, 14]. During the symmetrical motions of galloping these forces match the stride one to one, so their rate halves as the animal transitions to this gait from trotting [15].
The rhythms of locomotion and respiration can interact—a phenomenon known as locomotion-respiratory coupling [16]. In various species of mammals, including dogs, horses, and humans, respiration was found to synchronize with stride [15–19]. For nonhumans moving at stable moderate-to-high speeds the preferred locking is of one respiration cycle per stride (1 : 1 ratio) for symmetric and 2 : 1 for asymmetric gaits, although other stable ratios can be observed too. Both passive and active mechanisms have been proposed to explain this coupling. Rhythmic fluctuations in the loading of the lungs by forces associated with limb movements or displacement of inner organs may passively entrain respiration during locomotion [15–17]. A possible active mechanism has been described whereby activation of sensory input pathways from the limbs could entrain respiratory motor activity [20].
There is growing interest in the flexible coupling of sensorimotor rhythms characteristic of active rodent behavior [4, 8, 21–23]. To our knowledge, the interactions of these with the characteristic rhythmicity of locomotion have not yet been studied. In this work, we analyze recordings of head accelerometry, intranasal pressure, ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs), and video tracking to understand possible couplings between rhythmic behaviors during different modes of locomotion in freely behaving rats in a social context. We show evidence for phase coupling between the respiratory cycle and head/body movements associated with stride. The degree of coupling was, however, lower than what is usually reported for other quadrupeds. We show evidence suggesting that the emission of ultrasonic vocalizations disrupts the synchrony and propose that locomotion-respiration coupling is flexibly controlled during natural rat behavior.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Animal Subjects and Recording Sessions
This work combines recordings from two datasets. Most quantitative analysis comes from dataset 1, combining instantaneous speed, respiration, vocalizations, and head acceleration from 6 rats. To better understand the relationship between the acceleration signals and stride, we collected dataset 2 combining all of the above with high-speed video in 1 rat.
2.1.1. Dataset 1
This dataset includes recordings from 6 Long Evans rats (Charles River): 2 females (ages 4.5–6.5 months) and 4 males (ages 2.5–5 months) interacting in male-female and male-male pairs. Total recording time for each rat was 46 and 28 minutes (females) and 74, 25, 12, and 10 minutes (males). Rats were implanted with intranasal cannulae and carried wireless headstage recording pressure from the cannula and head acceleration from a 3-axis accelerometer. The estrous cycle of females was controlled through ovariectomy and hormonal treatment and all recordings were made during estrus. Rats were held on an inverted light cycle and all recordings were carried out during the dark phase under infrared illumination. All procedures were approved by The Rockefeller University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol #09035).
2.1.2. Dataset 2
This dataset includes recordings from 1 Long Evans male rat behaving alone or in the presence of a surgically devocalized female in hormonally induced estrus (both aged 5.0–6.1 months, Brain Institute animal facility). Respiration and head acceleration were recorded in the same way as for dataset 1 and detailed movements were captured with high-speed video under infrared illumination. All procedures were approved by the Animal Use Ethics Committee (Protocol #044/2014, CEUA/UFRN).
2.2. Surgical Procedures
Rats who underwent surgery were anesthetized with a combination of ketamine, xylazine, and atropine (i.m.: 100, 6, and 0.04 mg/kg, resp.). After surgery, buprenorphine (i.p.: 0.1 mg/kg) was administered as analgesic and enrofloxacin (i.p.: 20 mg/kg) as antibiotic. Animals recovered for at least one week before recordings.
As described in [4], the end of a thin 2 cm long stainless steel cannula (gauge 22) was implanted through the nasal bone. The cannula was bent to an S-shape for it to end above the temporal bone and secured with bone screws and dental acrylic. A ring magnet (R422; OD 6.35 mm, ID 3.18 mm; K & J Magnetics) was attached to the exposed end of the cannula to match an equivalent one secured to the pressure sensor in the wireless headstage. This allowed us to easily and safely secure the headstages on the rats’ heads by using magnetic force only.
For bilateral ovariectomy, we made incisions through the skin and muscle posterior to the rib cage, through which we pulled the ovaries out with forceps. After clamping the uterine horns with hemostats and absorbable suture we proceeded to cut off the ovaries, suture the muscle with absorbable thread, and close the skin with suture clamps. For induction of estrus we injected females with estradiol benzoate (s.c.: 0.05 mg/kg) followed, 48 h later, by progesterone (s.c.: 2.5 mg/kg). Recordings were made 5–10 h after the administration of progesterone.
The female placed in the arena adjacent to the male for high-speed video recordings was devocalized so that all recorded vocalizations were from the male. We made a vertical incision on the skin of the neck (~3 cm) and divulsed the sternohyoid muscle. We then sectioned approximately 1 cm of the left laryngeal recurrent nerve [24].
2.3. Data Acquisition
We recorded animals in dataset 1 in an arena built with vertical gratings and split into two parallel linear tracks, 0.2 × 2.67 × 0.74 m (
2.3.1. Video
We recorded video at 30 fps with two overhanging Logitech c920 webcams with infrared filters removed. We synchronized video with the other recorded signals with <1 frame precision through an infrared LED blinking in the visual field of the cameras controlled by the main data acquisition board. For dataset 2 we recorded video at 200 fps with a Point Grey FL3-U3-13Y3 M-C camera using FlyCapture2 software. We triggered frame acquisition with TTL pulses generated by the data acquisition board.
2.3.2. Ultrasound
We recorded ultrasound with condenser microphones with nearly flat (±5 dB) response from 10 to 150 kHz (CM16/CMPA-5V, Avisoft Bioacoustics) digitized by a data acquisition board at 250 kHz sampling frequency (PCIe-6259 DAQ with BNC-2110 connector, National Instruments). For dataset 1 we recorded from 3 overhanging microphones distributed along the long axis of the arena. For dataset 2 we placed 1 microphone 10 cm from the floor pointing at the area within the visual field of the high-speed camera.
2.3.3. Respiration and Accelerometry
During recordings, we magnetically attached to the intranasal cannula a pressure sensor (24PCAFA6G, Honeywell) integrated into a custom-made wireless headstage based on the DIGI XBee module powered with 3 V from a Li-ion battery (weight 20–23 g, see Figure 1, schematics available on request). The headstage included a 3-axis accelerometer (ADXL335, Analog Devices) with signal filtered to 50 Hz 3 dB bandwidth with 0.1 μF capacitors. Voltage outputs from this component are linearly proportional to acceleration (300 mV/g with 3 V supply voltage). The pressure and acceleration signals were transmitted with a sampling rate of 200 Hz each and digitized in synchrony with the ultrasound. The transmission imposed a 2-sample (10 ms) delay which was not corrected.
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]2.4. Data Preprocessing
We carried out all data preprocessing with custom-made routines in MATLAB (The Mathworks).
2.4.1. Locomotion
We obtained the position of the rat in each video frame through a custom-made implementation of object tracking based on adaptive background subtraction. We smoothed the position by independently convolving the
2.4.2. Ultrasonic Vocalizations
We thoroughly describe elsewhere the methodologies for automatically detecting ultrasonic vocalizations and assigning them to the emitting rat in a pair ([4] and to be published elsewhere). Briefly, detection involves finding times with low spectral entropy in the ultrasonic range and low noise in the sonic range at any of the three overhanging microphones. At times when USVs are detected, we compare the intranasal pressure of both rats and assign the USV to the one with characteristic constant pressure close to atmospheric values. We did not resolve cases of simultaneous vocalization from the two rats, so only one rat could be considered to be vocalizing at any given time. Because of this, when segregating respiration or acceleration cycles from one rat based on the emission of USVs, we discarded those cycles when the other was vocalizing, as those USVs could be obscuring calls from the analyzed rat. When needed, ultrasound was segmented by the sniffing cycle, such that all of the ultrasound emitted during a single exhalation is grouped as a USV.
We visually classified 100 randomly selected “50 kHz” USVs from each rat into four classes. “Flats” were identified as calls with little or no frequency modulation, with or without frequency jumps, “trills” as those of high frequency and frequency modulation, “flat-trills” as those combining the previous two elements, and “splits” as those with fundamental frequency momentarily jumping down to the 30–35 kHz range, typically with a visible second harmonic.
2.4.3. Respiration and Accelerometry
Respiration is the intranasal pressure measurement and was left unfiltered, with atmospheric pressure baseline removed. We subtracted the baseline values from the acceleration signals with
2.5. Data Analysis
2.5.1. Frequency and Synchrony
Instantaneous respiratory rate was obtained as the inverse of the sniff cycle duration. Acceleration rates were obtained as the peak frequency in a Fourier spectrum of a 1-second window centered at each time point, previously convolved with a 0th-order Slepian taper. For synchrony analysis we bandpass filtered the data at 3–11 Hz with eegfilt [27] and obtained instantaneous phases from its Hilbert transform. For the acceleration signals, peak timestamps were defined as the times when the instantaneous phase crosses zero in the positive direction. For Phase Locking Value (PLV) calculation, we collected the instantaneous phases of a given signal (such as intranasal pressure) at a selected set of timestamps from another one (such as the peaks of
2.5.2. Granger Causality
We analyzed Granger causality between intranasal pressure, emission of ultrasonic vocalizations, and
3. Results
We recorded locomotion, vocalizations, respiration, and head acceleration from 4 male and 2 female adult rats during free behavior (Figure 1). The position and instantaneous speed of the rats were obtained from video tracking (top view) under infrared light. Ultrasound was recorded from overhanging microphones and ultrasonic vocalizations were automatically detected from it. Each rat carried on its head a wireless headstage provided with a pressure sensor and a 3-axis accelerometer. The pressure sensor connected to an intranasal cannula to monitor the respiratory cycle. The accelerometer was positioned with one axis horizontal and directed to the front of the head “
We sought to understand how respiration and emission of vocalizations couple with head movements and stride across different modes of behavior. As detailed above, each of these motor components can be rhythmic so we analyzed them as oscillatory signals and studied possible synchronizations between them. Based on previous knowledge and preliminary analysis, we looked for coupling in the 3–11 Hz, the typical range of rates for these variables during active behavior. We begin by analyzing how the rhythmicity of these signals varies with the instantaneous speed of the animal (Figure 2). We performed frequency analysis of head acceleration in a sliding 1 s window and plotted the mean spectrum for each instantaneous speed range (Figure 2(a)). Both
The respiration rate of awake rats has a large dynamic range from about 2 to 11 Hz, with a transition from passive breathing to active sniffing typically reported at 4-5 Hz [1–3]. If the respiratory and stride cycles were to consistently couple one to one, their rates should match across speeds. This was not the case in our recordings. We calculated instant respiratory rate as the inverse of the duration of each cycle and found its mean to be above 7 Hz for all speeds (Figure 2(c)), reflecting that the rats maintained high activity levels during the sessions. In detail, its mean rate moderately dropped with speed from 8 to 7 Hz. We reasoned that ultrasonic vocalizations of the “50 kHz” family could be causing the observed drop in respiratory rate, as we know that their emission increases during locomotion (details to be published elsewhere) and prolongs the respiratory cycle [4]. Indeed, the percentage of cycles with vocalizations increased with speed from 20 to 80% (Figure 2(d)). Cycles with USVs were about 2 Hz slower than those without them and the mean instantaneous rate of the silent respiratory cycles was above 8 Hz for all speeds where it could be measured (Figure 2(e)).
These exploratory analyses show that the behavior of the rats in the arena was not homogeneous. Instead, it interleaved moments of staying in place, with low vocal production and head acceleration, with locomotor behavior of varied stride rate and higher vocal output. We decided to segregate the behavior into 4 locomotion modes based on the recorded signals: staying in place, walking, trotting, and galloping. First, we segmented locomotion into episodes of “progressing,” moving between places, and “lingering,” staying in one location with only local movement [26, 31]. We noticed that during a single progression rats could use more than one gait. We thus kept from each progression only one second of data, centered at its peak speed. We found this to result in more homogenous locomotion events. We labeled as walking those progressions with maximum speed between 0.2 and 0.5 m/s. Those with speed above 0.5 m/s and peak
3.1. Sniffing Behavior
Rats engage in various behaviors while staying in place such as grooming, rearing, and sniffing. We sought to study periods of sniffing behavior, when the rats are known to coordinate fast respiration with head movements and whisking [1]. Figure 3(a) shows an example of this, where respiration and acceleration are apparently synchronized and match approach and withdrawal movements of the head (Supplementary Video 1 in Supplementary Material available online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/4065073). To detect from the large dataset periods of sniffing behavior we obtained a smoothed measure of respiratory rate and collected nonoverlapping 1-second episodes around peaks of at least 3 Hz (total 2675, discarding episodes with very low acceleration power). For each episode, we calculated mean rates for respiration,
3.2. Walking
We now consider those progressions labeled as walking (maximum speed between 0.2 and 0.5 m/s). Observation of high-speed videos evidenced that rats would sometimes engage in sniffing behavior while walking, such that the head acceleration signals would be dominated by head approach/withdrawal and not by the stride movements (see below for the large dataset). We will now focus on walking progressions with both acceleration rates below 6 Hz (37 episodes) and leave the analysis of sniffing while walking for Supplementary Figure 1. Figure 4(a) shows an example of walking gait under high-speed video (Supplementary Video 2). We quantified the relationship between stride and head acceleration during walking by aligning the latter to the times when the front or hind feet touched the ground (the beginning of the stance part of each step cycle) (Figure 4(b)). The onset of both front and hind feet stance coincided with the peak of
3.3. Trotting
Figure 5(a) shows an example of trotting under high-speed video recording (Supplementary Video 3). During trotting, a given diagonal pair of feet (i.e., front left and hind right) contacts the floor close in time, alternating with the other diagonal pair. We characterized the synchrony between stride and head acceleration while trotting as we did before for walking (Figure 5(b), 91 episodes). Forces acting on the head aligned with front and hind feet at similar phases, with
3.4. Galloping
We detected only a few cases of galloping in our recordings: 5 in the dataset with high-speed video and 15 in the larger one. Figure 6(a) shows an example of this gait (Supplementary Video 4). During galloping, both front feet land closely in time, followed by both hind feet. Aligning of head acceleration to the feet timestamps suggests that peaks in the vertical forces acting on the head occur about 50 ms prior to the landing of the hind feet and 50 ms past the landing of the front ones (Figure 6(b),
3.5. Disruption of Coupling between Respiration and Head/Body Movements during Vocalizations
Rats in our social arena were emitting high rates of ultrasonic vocalizations (mean 2.0, range 1.3–2.5 calls/sec). All calls were of the “50 kHz” family, including “flats” (
[figures omitted; refer to PDF]
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]3.6. Causality
As we have seen, all recorded behavioral variables show some degree of coupling to all others. This makes it difficult to understand if there exist hierarchies between them, such as locomotion forces entraining the respiratory cycle. We looked for possible directionalities in the coupling by measuring Granger causality between four time series: intranasal pressure, ultrasonic vocalization emission, and the two axes of head acceleration. Given a set of variables (
4. Discussion
We simultaneously monitored respiration, vocalizations, head acceleration, and locomotion in rats freely behaving in a social context. We focused on behaviors where these motor components oscillate and analyzed their detailed synchrony—or lack of it. The biggest challenge in our study came from the heterogeneity of relatively unconstrained natural behavior. From our observations, most behavioral patterns in the rat do not last longer than one or a few seconds, such that consistent but short-lived relationships between variables could be easily missed. Building on solid existing literature describing the different components of rat behavior, we devised ways of automatically parsing active behavior into four modes based on instantaneous speed and rhythmicity of respiration and head/body movements: sniffing (in place or while walking), walking, trotting, and galloping.
Sniffing is a characteristic sensorimotor behavior displayed by rats during exploration. It combines fast (5–11 Hz) respiration with head and whisker movements, all of these coordinated into a common cycle [1, 8]. We detected putative sniffing bouts as periods of fast (>6 Hz) respiration and head movements without fast locomotion. These episodes adhered in detail to previous descriptions of sniffing, with consistent phase relationships: head withdrawal during exhalations and head approach during inspirations. We have no measure of whisking in our large dataset but the rat under high-speed video recording was actively whisking during sniffing bouts.
Horizontal and vertical forces measured by the head-mounted accelerometer were consistently in antiphase during rhythmic behaviors, with some small but consistent phase differences during different behavioral modes. Although these two variables are not independent, it is possible that they are dominated by forces from different sets of muscles. It is interesting that respiration aligned better to
Our results point to a flexible coupling between respiration and locomotion forces during spontaneous rat behavior. It is clear that head acceleration and respiration are synchronized to some degree during walking and trotting, such that there is a larger probability of exhalations beginning around the peak of the vertical component. This phase relationship is consistent with that observed in dogs and rabbits [17, 19, 32]. The degree of synchrony we found was, however, lower than those reported for other mammals. Note that synchrony was worse when aligning respiration phase directly to the footsteps of the rat recorded under high-speed video. It could thus be that respiration couples more to head movements during stride than to stride itself. During walking, we cannot rule out that some of the synchrony reflects brief contributions of sniffing behavior. In trotting cases when synchrony was apparent, locking was 1 : 1 between acceleration and respiration cycles (equivalent to 2 respiration cycles per stride, as found for dogs and large opossums [17, 19, 33]). Despite respiratory rate being approximately twice the head movement rate at low speeds, we found no evidence for sustained 2 : 1 locking mode, as observed in other small mammals [33], such that 2 breaths coincide with each acceleration cycle. We cannot rule out that this happens for some scattered cycles. Rats were vocalizing at high rates during locomotion, in up to 80% of the sniff cycles at their fastest speeds. Our analysis suggests that emission of a vocalization results in an immediate perturbation in the coupling between respiration and stride, consistent with the known instantaneous prolonging of the exhalation phase [3, 4]. Contraction of trunk musculature during vocal exhalation could decouple respiration from stride forces acting on the lungs. Complementarily, central mechanisms could be blocking proprioceptive influences on respiratory pattern generators. It has been proposed that rodent ultrasound emission is a byproduct of explosive exhalations caused by loading of the thoracic cavity upon forelimb contact during locomotion [34]. Our results disprove this strict causal relationship, as modulation of vocal emission by stride is incomplete and rats can maintain vocal rhythms independent of ongoing locomotion even at high speeds. We do not rule out, however, that stride mechanics could be modulating the detailed timing and properties of vocalizations through their partial coupling with respiration—although such effects were not supported by our causality estimation. We could not analyze broader effects of vocal rates on coupling simply because our rats were rarely walking fast, trotting, or galloping without vocalizing. In the few cases of silent trotting, obtained from one male rat behaving in isolation, we observed reasonable synchrony between respiration and stride. We would need to find natural nonsocial conditions where rats are sufficiently motivated to trot and gallop to extend our study to spontaneous silent locomotion.
We acknowledge some limitations in our work. To be able to track body movements during free behavior we mounted on the head a ~20 g wireless headstage (about 5% of body weight). This is common practice in freely moving electrophysiology studies, and rats seem to quickly get habituated to carrying weight on the head. Our case is potentially more problematic since we are directly measuring acceleration at the head, which depends on force and mass. Rats carrying our headstage engaged in many behaviors with apparent normality. For those behaviors which we analyzed quantitatively, their properties matched previous descriptions: sniffing occurred at the expected rates and phase relationships of respiration and head approach/withdrawal; head acceleration rates matched twice the published stride rates for walking and trotting and once that rate for galloping; peak vertical forces align with the onset of the exhalation. Despite this, it is possible that detailed magnitudes and phases are affected by the headstage weight. We could not extract a clear transition from fast walking to trotting from the data without high-speed video, so we set a threshold at max. progression speed of 0.5 m/s to classify them. It remains unclear to us whether a clear-cut difference between both gaits exists in the rat or rather one smoothly turns into the other as speed increases. There were few instances of galloping in both our datasets, but enough to observe that no obvious locking between respiration and locomotion appears as the rats switch to this gait. Younger estrus females and males should be studied to better sample this behavior. Because not all rats reached fast trotting and galloping, we pooled together data from all of them for most analysis. The fundamental mechanical properties studied in this work likely generalize to the rat population, but we cannot make inferences about their variability across individuals or their correlation with variables such as sex and age.
5. Conclusions
Rats moving freely in space exhibited only partial coupling between the respiratory and stride cycles. Our results clearly differed from the tight cycle-to-cycle synchrony frequently found for other quadruped mammals [35]. We do not know of other works measuring this synchronization in the rat, so we wonder whether this contrast is due to species differences, scaling of coupling with body mass, or differences in the behavioral settings. Most measurements of locomotor-respiratory coupling make efforts to homogenize the locomotor behavior by using treadmills or guiding the animals into running straight lines at stable speeds. These conditions deliberately minimize perturbations that could interfere with the coupling. The rats in our arena were rapidly changing speed, direction, and gait of locomotion and perturbing the respiratory rhythm through emission of vocalizations. We propose that locomotion-respiratory coupling is not a requirement of the system but rather a flexible entrainment that can be overridden by other physiological or behavioral needs such as vocal communication. Two directions should be followed to disambiguate between species and behavioral setting: measure coupling in rats running silently at stable speeds and remeasure it in other mammals during rich natural behavior.
Restricting animal behavior has proven an invaluable tool in understanding its building blocks and mechanisms. New insights can be gained by complementing this with studies where behavior is less bounded and thus closer to the conditions under which it evolved. This approach however poses big challenges on the recording and analysis side. This is particularly true for small animals like rats and mice, for whom behavioral variables evolve fast. A way to compensate for the loss of homogeneity is to develop reliable ways to parse free behavior into stereotyped modes. Additional challenges stem from the intercorrelated nature of these variables. For example, one can believe that a given physiological measure (like neuronal activity at a given brain structure) is partially correlated with the phase of the respiratory cycle when, in fact, this was actually secondary to a tight association between that measure and stride. Understanding the magnitude of these correlations at each behavioral mode will aid in avoiding these pitfalls. We acknowledge that missing variables could even be confounding the correlations presented in this work.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors’ Contributions
Joseph Andrews Alves and Barbara Ciralli Boerner contributed equally to this work.
Acknowledgments
Dataset 1 was collected at the Levy Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior of The Rockefeller University, New York, NY, USA, with funding provided by the Leon Levy Foundation. Dataset 2 was collected at the Brain Institute, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, RN, Brazil, with funding provided by CNPQ, Brazil (Universal 461735/2014-8). Joseph Andrews Alves received funding from CAPES, Brazil. Barbara Ciralli Boerner received funding by CNPQ, Brazil. The authors would like to thank the Brazilian people for supporting their work, Yevgeniy Sirotin for kindly providing the wireless headstage, and Adriano Tort for discussion.
[1] W. I. Welker, "Analysis of sniffing of the albino rat," Behaviour, vol. 22 no. 3, pp. 223-244, DOI: 10.1163/156853964x00030, 1964.
[2] M. Wachowiak, "All in a sniff: olfaction as a model for active sensing," Neuron, vol. 71 no. 6, pp. 962-973, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.030, 2011.
[3] C. Hegoburu, K. Shionoya, S. Garcia, B. Messaoudi, M. Thévenet, A.-M. Mouly, "The RUB cage: respiration-ultrasonic vocalizations-behavior acquisition setup for assessing emotional memory in rats," Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 5, article 25,DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00025, 2011.
[4] Y. B. Sirotin, M. E. Costa, D. A. Laplagne, "Rodent ultrasonic vocalizations are bound to active sniffing behavior," Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 8, article 399,DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00399, 2014.
[5] A. M. MacLarnon, G. P. Hewitt, "The evolution of human speech: the role of enhanced breathing control," American Journal of Physical Anthropology, vol. 109 no. 3, pp. 341-363, DOI: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8644(199907)109:3<341::aid-ajpa5>3.0.co;2-2, 1999.
[6] R. Assini, Y. B. Sirotin, D. A. Laplagne, "Rapid triggering of vocalizations following social interactions," Current Biology, vol. 23 no. 22, pp. R996-R997, DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.007, 2013.
[7] J. E. James, M. De Burgh Daly, "Reflex respiratory and cardiovascular effects of stimulation of receptors in the nose of the dog," The Journal of Physiology, vol. 220 no. 3, pp. 673-696, DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1972.sp009729, 1972.
[8] J. D. Moore, M. Deschênes, T. Furuta, D. Huber, M. C. Smear, M. Demers, D. Kleinfeld, "Hierarchy of orofacial rhythms revealed through whisking and breathing," Nature, vol. 497 no. 7448, pp. 205-210, DOI: 10.1038/nature12076, 2013.
[9] S. Grillner, "Locomotion in vertebrates: central mechanisms and reflex interaction," Physiological Reviews, vol. 55 no. 2, pp. 247-304, 1975.
[10] M. G. Locomotion, The Behavior of the Laboratory Rat, 2004.
[11] G. B. Gillis, A. A. Biewener, "Hindlimb muscle function in relation to speed and gait: in vivo patterns of strain and activation in a hip and knee extensor of the rat ( Rattus norvegicus )," Journal of Experimental Biology, vol. 204 no. 15, pp. 2717-2731, 2001.
[12] C. S. Mendes, I. Bartos, Z. Márka, T. Akay, S. Márka, R. S. Mann, "Quantification of gait parameters in freely walking rodents," BMC Biology, vol. 13 no. 1, article 50,DOI: 10.1186/s12915-015-0154-0, 2015.
[13] M. F. Bobbert, C. B. Gómez Álvarez, P. R. van Weeren, L. Roepstorff, M. A. Weishaupt, "Validation of vertical ground reaction forces on individual limbs calculated from kinematics of horse locomotion," Journal of Experimental Biology, vol. 210 no. 11, pp. 1885-1896, DOI: 10.1242/jeb.02774, 2007.
[14] G. D. Muir, I. Q. Whishaw, "Red nucleus lesions impair overground locomotion in rats: a kinetic analysis," European Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 12 no. 3, pp. 1113-1122, DOI: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00987.x, 2000.
[15] I. S. Young, R. Alexander, A. J. Woakes, P. J. Butler, L. Anderson, "The synchronization of ventilation and locomotion in horses ( Equus caballus )," Journal of Experimental Biology, vol. 166, pp. 19-31, 1992.
[16] D. M. Bramble, D. R. Carrier, "Running and breathing in mammals," Science, vol. 219 no. 4582, pp. 251-256, DOI: 10.1126/science.6849136, 1983.
[17] D. M. Bramble, F. A. Jenkins, "Mammalian locomotor-respiratory integration: implications for diaphragmatic and pulmonary design," Science, vol. 262 no. 5131, pp. 235-240, DOI: 10.1126/science.8211141, 1993.
[18] R. V. Baudinette, B. J. Gannon, W. B. Runciman, S. Wells, J. B. Love, "Do cardiorespiratory frequencies show entrainment with hopping in the tammar wallaby?," Journal of Experimental Biology, vol. 129, pp. 251-263, 1987.
[19] D. R. Carrier, "Function of the intercostal muscles in trotting dogs: ventilation or locomotion?," Journal of Experimental Biology, vol. 199 no. 7, pp. 1455-1465, 1996.
[20] D. Morin, D. Viala, "Coordinations of locomotor and respiratory rhythms in vitro are critically dependent on hindlimb sensory inputs," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 22 no. 11, pp. 4756-4765, 2002.
[21] J. D. Moore, D. Kleinfeld, F. Wang, "How the brainstem controls orofacial behaviors comprised of rhythmic actions," Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 37 no. 7, pp. 370-380, DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.05.001, 2014.
[22] S. Ranade, B. Hangy, A. Kepecs, "Multiple modes of phase locking between sniffing and whisking during active exploration," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 33 no. 19, pp. 8250-8256, DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.3874-12.2013, 2013.
[23] R. P. Rao, F. Mielke, E. Bobrov, M. Brecht, "Vocalization-whisking coordination and multisensory integration of social signals in rat auditory cortex," eLife, vol. 3,DOI: 10.7554/elife.03185, 2014.
[24] L. H. Roberts, "Evidence for the laryngeal source of ultrasonic and audible cries of rodents," Journal of Zoology, vol. 175 no. 2, pp. 243-257, DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1975.tb01399.x, 1975.
[25] D. Drai, Y. Benjamini, I. Golani, "Statistical discrimination of natural modes of motion in rat exploratory behavior," Journal of Neuroscience Methods, vol. 96 no. 2, pp. 119-131, DOI: 10.1016/s0165-0270(99)00194-6, 2000.
[26] I. Hen, A. Sakov, N. Kafkafi, I. Golani, Y. Benjamini, "The dynamics of spatial behavior: how can robust smoothing techniques help?," Journal of Neuroscience Methods, vol. 133 no. 1-2, pp. 161-172, DOI: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.013, 2004.
[27] A. Delorme, S. Makeig, "EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis," Journal of Neuroscience Methods, vol. 134 no. 1,DOI: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009, 2004.
[28] R. Scheffer-Teixeira, A. B. L. Tort, "Lack of evidence for cross-frequency phase-phase coupling between theta and gamma oscillations in the hippocampus," bioRxiv,DOI: 10.1101/045963, 2016.
[29] P. Berens, "CircStat: a MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics," Journal of Statistical Software, vol. 31 no. 10,DOI: 10.18637/jss.v031.i10, 2009.
[30] L. Barnett, A. K. Seth, "The MVGC multivariate Granger causality toolbox: a new approach to Granger-causal inference," Journal of Neuroscience Methods, vol. 223, pp. 50-68, DOI: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.018, 2014.
[31] I. Golani, Y. Benjamini, D. Eilam, "Stopping behavior: constraints on exploration in rats ( Rattus norvegicus )," Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 53 no. 1-2, pp. 21-33, DOI: 10.1016/s0166-4328(05)80263-3, 1993.
[32] R. S. Simons, "Running, breathing and visceral motion in the domestic rabbit ( Oryctolagus cuniculus ): testing visceral displacement hypotheses," Journal of Experimental Biology, vol. 202 no. 5, pp. 563-577, 1999.
[33] S. M. Reilly, T. D. White, "Breathing with your belly: abdominal exhalation, loco-ventilatory integration and size constraints on locomotion in small mammals," Zoology, vol. 112 no. 3, pp. 161-168, DOI: 10.1016/j.zool.2008.08.002, 2009.
[34] M. S. Blumberg, "Rodent ultrasonic short calls: locomotion, biomechanics, and communication," Journal of Comparative Psychology, vol. 106 no. 4, pp. 360-365, DOI: 10.1037//0735-7036.106.4.360, 1992.
[35] D. F. Boggs, "Interactions between locomotion and ventilation in tetrapods," Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology, vol. 133 no. 2, pp. 269-288, DOI: 10.1016/s1095-6433(02)00160-5, 2002.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright © 2016 Joseph Andrews Alves et al. This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Quadrupedal mammals typically synchronize their respiration with body movements during rhythmic locomotion. In the rat, fast respiration is coupled to head movements during sniffing behavior, but whether respiration is entrained by stride dynamics is not known. We recorded intranasal pressure, head acceleration, instantaneous speed, and ultrasonic vocalizations from male and female adult rats while freely behaving in a social environment. We used high-speed video recordings of stride to understand how head acceleration signals relate to locomotion and developed techniques to identify episodes of sniffing, walking, trotting, and galloping from the recorded variables. Quantitative analysis of synchrony between respiration and head acceleration rhythms revealed that respiration and locomotion movements were coordinated but with a weaker coupling than expected from previous work in other mammals. We have recently shown that rats behaving in social settings produce high rates of ultrasonic vocalizations during locomotion bouts. Accordingly, rats emitted vocalizations in over half of the respiratory cycles during fast displacements. We present evidence suggesting that emission of these calls disrupts the entrainment of respiration by stride. The coupling between these two variables is thus flexible, such that it can be overridden by other behavioral demands.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer