1. Introduction
In fewer than 13 years the number of people worldwide aged 60 or over is projected to grow by 56%, reaching 1.4 billion (United Nations, 2015). At the same time, international migration will continue to impact on the demographics of numerous countries in Europe and elsewhere. Furthermore, in many European countries, age 65 marks the end of an individual’s professional activity, introducing them to retirement as an era of “personal achievement and fulfillment” (Laslett, 1987, p. 134). Such individuals may occupy their time in retirement in ways such as traveling, taking up new hobbies and learning new – and potentially complex – skills.
The growth of professional, educational and individual mobility internationally creates the desire for travelers and migrants to communicate comfortably in languages not well known to them – for which the present-day third-age generation is on the whole ill-equipped (Gabrys-Barker, 2018). Meanwhile, in cognitive psychology, second language (L2) learning is signaled as a promising way of contributing to healthy and active aging, promoting neural plasticity and fostering social interactions and individual mobility and autonomy (e.g. Antoniou et al., 2013; Green, 2018).
Although interest in the topic of language learning in the third age dates back to the 1970s (e.g. Kalfus, 1977), little attention has been paid to individual differences in older adults’ ability to acquire a new language, particularly in the L1 community. Additionally, research methodologies vary greatly, including, e.g.: case studies (e.g. Mackey & Sachs, 2012; Swain & Lapkin, 2011); qualitative interviews and surveys (e.g. Castañeda, 2016); and laboratory studies that either compare young adults with older adults or focus on older adult learning only (e.g. Lenet et al., 2011). These studies have mostly been conducted in North America, Asia and Latin America, although the topic is also gaining momentum in Europe (e.g. Kliesch et al., 2018; Pot et al., 2018; Ware et al., 2017).
Having in mind the goal of motivating further research into third age foreign language (FL) learning in Europe, we conducted a small-scale pilot study exploring whether age-advanced EFL learners would show FL gains, cognitive benefits and increased overall well-being following four weeks of EFL training in a school context in the L1 community, and whether this might be related to biological age or previous language-learning experience (bilingualism). We made use of an equal-status concurrent mixed methods design (see Singleton & Pfenninger, 2015), systematically integrating qualitative content analyses with quantitative approaches. Our goal was to identify the factors that determine the FL learning success of 12 older adults, on the one hand, and the question of how language learning benefits them on the other. Data from such a longitudinal investigation1 would make a substantial contribution to research on late FL learning as well as cognitive aspects of healthy aging; it would also help crucially in establishing learner profiles and in optimally designing individualized language training.
2. Literature review
2.1. Neurocognitive aspects of additional language acquisition in the third age
Age-related declines are widely believed to continue across the lifespan. This kind of account often refers to a slowing of processing speed (Salthouse, 2000), reduced working memory capacity (e.g. Engle & Kane, 2004), reduced learning and recall of new information (e.g. Lindenberger, 2014), a decrease in attention (e.g. Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009), an increase in reaction times (e.g. Goral et al., 2007), poorer encoding of contextual information in memory (e.g. Naveh-Benjamin & Old, 2008), and deficits in inhibitory control (e.g. Verhaeghen & Cerella, 2008), but also to changes in speech production and perception (e.g. Wingfield & Grossmann, 2006). Such age-related decline has been reported to be preceded by structural changes, such as a loss of functional brain connectivity, cortical thinning or the decline of white matter integrity (e.g. Damoiseaux et al., 2008).
There are also counter-arguments. On the one hand, the neurocognitive aging literature on the whole suggests that the brain preserves large parts of its plasticity even at an advanced age and remains receptive towards new languages (e.g. Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009). Furthermore, attention and cognitive control capacities have been shown to distinguish individuals on the basis, for example, of previous L2 experience and social class (Bialystok & Poarch, 2014), which raises the question: what do age-related declines in scores on neuropsychological tests really reveal? The above evidence may not necessarily index aging or decline but may simply reflect the predictable outcome of the cumulative experience of learning i.e. increased knowledge. An accumulating body of evidence in the domain of linguistic cognition (e.g. Ramscar and colleagues 2014) indicates no neurobiological evidence for any declines in the processing capacities of healthy older adults, except where there is evidence of pathology. Ramscar et al. showed in computational simulations that older adults’ changing performance on many neuropsychological and psychometric tests, often attributed to cognitive information-processing capacities declining through adulthood, may simply reflect experience-related performance differences such as memory-search demands, which escalate as experience grows.
2.2. The bilingual advantage in older adults learning an additional language
Bilingualism from early in life has been linked to an enhancement of domain-general executive control abilities in adults, such as monitoring (the ability to update information in working memory), shifting (the ability to switch between tasks), and/or inhibition (the ability to suppress dominant responses) (e.g. Antoniou et al., 2013). There thus seems to be a relationship between early bilingualism and more efficient later language learning, owing to a wider availability of language-learning strategies and the consequent capacity to attain both general language proficiency (e.g. Pfenninger & Singleton, in press) and literacy skills (e.g. Adesope et al., 2010). It is also suggested that early bilingualism is associated with a substantial retardation of Alzheimer’s disease and a decreased risk of senile dementia, possibly owing to a so-called ‘cognitive reserve’, which results from the use of multiple languages (see Bialystok et al., 2016).
However, substantial evidence questioning this bilingual advantage at the behavioral and neural level has emerged, relating to the multifaceted experience of the bilinguals in question, including lifestyle factors, but also to methodological inconsistencies (e.g. Paap et al., 2015). Thus, it has been suggested that the bilingual advantage reported is a finding circumscribed by specific and unclear situations, and that it does not extend in general to lifelong bilingual third agers or to neo-bilingual third agers (see e.g. Bak et al., 2014). In one of the sparse studies on this topic, Keijzer and Schmid (2016) investigated executive function advantages among 29 elderly, long-term, immersed Dutch bilinguals (aged 71+), who all started using their L2 English after puberty (mean AoA 27.23). This late bilingual experience seemed to result in an advantage for some and an overloading effect for others; i.e. no general bilingual advantage was attested.
Only one (training) study has investigated the effects of bilingualism on language learning in the third age (and in the participants’ L1 community), namely Cox’s (2017) analysis of differential effects of computer-mediated instruction in 45 older adults (aged 61–82), who were either monolingual or bilingual, learning novel Latin morphosyntax. While all participants showed significant gains in accuracy on tasks that required interpretation (written and aural), grammaticality judgment, and production (written), bilinguals showed an advantage over monolinguals, regardless of instructional condition. Even more important for our study is the (still open) question of whether the effects of long-term bilingual experience on executive functions are comparable to those of short-term FL training in a school context.
2.3. Socio-affective aspects of L2 acquisition in the third age
Besides neurocognitive factors, our understanding of the L2 learning process in older adults should also take into consideration the cognition-affect interface (see Schuller’s (2004) Three Capitals framework), i.e. the influence of changes in lifestyle, L2 learning motivation, overall well-being, self-esteem, different life and learning histories, present-day conditions, general communicative skills, construction of third age identity, and sense of purpose.
Addressing the effect of motivation in older adults in a school context, scholars have observed apprehension and bewilderment in relation to the learning environment (Brändle, 1986) and low coping potential plus a lack of self-confidence (Ramírez Gómez, 2016). Additionally, the impact of ageist views and social stereotypes vis-à-vis the elderly has been found to lead to self-defeating preconceptions among older learners regarding learning abilities (e.g. Andrew, 2012). If third agers accept ageist stereotypes from the culture (including community or family) as valid, self-stereotyping occurs, which may occasion physical, emotional, and cognitive declines (see Knowles et al., 2011; Oxford, 2018).
By contrast, some scholars have remarked that many older students – whatever their subject matter of choice – return to the classroom with excitement and very high motivation levels, which may lead to a substantial increase in well-being (Ware et al., 2017). Brändle (1986) and Johnstone (2002) comment particularly favorably on older learners’ general command of reading skills, their understanding of grammatical principles and the way in which they cope with lexical learning.
2.4. How older adults may benefit from second language learning
Cognitive capacities such as those outlined above not only vary as a function of age and genetics, but also in large part of lifestyle and the extent to which intellectually stimulating experiences are encountered (see Stern & Munn, 2010). However, there are only a few behavioral and neurocognitive studies investigating the impact of (intensive) L2 language training on cognitive or socio-affective functions in older adults and vice versa. First results of such intervention studies suggest that:
Within this context, we can now consider the research questions pertinent to this study.
3. Research questions
This study aims to begin to address the following research questions:
The main goal of the study is to provide preliminary data to promote further investigations in this area and to test the feasibility of EFL training and a variety of measures. Cognitive pre- and posttests will enable us to test Antoniou et al.’s (2013) hypothesis of the cognitive benefit of additional language learning in older adults, and provide an indication regarding the relationship between learner profiles and cognitive benefits.
4. Methodology
4.1. Research design
We seek to integrate qualitative and quantitative approaches by using an equal-status concurrent mixed methods design (see Singleton & Pfenninger, 2015) – both at the data collection and the analysis level, deriving corroboration (triangulation) as well as elaboration of the quantitative results with results from the qualitative dimension (complementarity).
4.2. Participants
We considered as “older” adult learners individuals between 63 and 90 years of age, roughly following Mackey & Sachs (2012). In order to address the various learner differences in the context of SLA and senescence, two groups of right-handed healthy participants were recruited who had no previous English-learning experience. Initially 19 participants signed up for the course, but only 12 of these (8 F, 4 M) completed the four-week intensive English course.
The experimental group consisted of six individuals (aged 74, 84, 89, 84, 87, and 88) who grew up as German/Slovenian sequential bilinguals. The second group was our active control group of six older adults (aged 63, 65, 72, 72, and 84), raised in a monolingual Austrian context with no more than school knowledge of any language other than German. Participants in both groups voluntarily signed up for the language course. They were retired by the time they took part in the study, did not report any musical training, neurological disease, language disorder or learning disability, and had no cerebral degradation (as measured by the concentration test for geriatric patients, see below).
Two points are important to emphasize here. On the one hand, studies involving cognitive training programs ideally include a passive control group, in order to be able to test the efficacy of the training and also to dissociate the effects of different cognitively demanding types of training and to explore repetition effects (Ramos et al., 2017). However, we believe that the lack of such an additional control group does not diminish the impact of the current findings, given the nature of our research questions (e.g. relating to the impact of previous bilingualism). Also, it is important to note that it was not the objective of this study to recruit as large a sample as possible. Psycholinguistic and SLA researchers, both quantitative and qualitative, have a tendency to restrict themselves to just one particular notion of generalizability – namely, a statistical, sampling-based notion – whilst ignoring theoretical generalizability, that is, the effort to shed empirical light on theoretical concepts or principles, thus yielding insights of potentially wider relevance and theoretical significance. Theoretical generalization seeks depth rather than breadth in its scope and analysis, an approach that is much needed in this area of SLA. In this sense, our focus is not so much on determining how L2 learning differs, such as in older adults versus younger adults, as on exploring the factors (e.g. previous bilingualism) that account for individual differences among older learners.
4.3. Teaching intervention
In one of our earlier pilot studies (Kliesch et al., 2018) we found that a training of four lessons a day over a period of three weeks was too demanding for older learners, both physically and mentally, causing self-reported fatigue and demotivation. On the basis of these results, we hypothesized that an intensive English training of four weeks, with three lessons of two hours a week (i.e. 24 hours in total), would be sufficient to achieve measurable differences in general FL proficiency.
All participants began the study by giving informed consent and completed a comprehensive language background questionnaire. The language course was taught by only one tutor (the second author of this paper); hence, it is unlikely that potential differences might relate to the personality and/or teaching style of individual tutors. The course book Headway A1 (Soars & Soars, 2007) served as a guideline for the course. However, the lesson plans needed continual adjustment. To this end, suitable teaching materials and methods were created, and the teaching pace was constantly adapted. The main goal was to teach the comprehension and use of familiar everyday expressions and very simple phrases to communicate basic needs. Best practice in older adult language education has developed little since the 1980s, when Joiner (1981) devised an optimal, goal-driven learning program (building communicative competence) with individualized, self-paced instruction at its core, and the inclusion of real-world materials to link learning to practice and boost motivation (an “immediate payoff” (1981: 33)).
Finally, it is important to mention that the participants in the experimental group wrote down and repeated the new vocabulary not only in English (FL) and German (L1), but also in Slovenian (their other L1).
4.4. Tasks
Data collection generally took place at three points of time: at the beginning of the course (T1), after two weeks (T2) and at the end (T3), although some tests were administered before and after each class. As pre- and post-tests, we used predominantly written FL proficiency tasks and cognitive tests that have either been successfully used before in studies on older populations and/or measure skills that are known to deteriorate with increasing age. There are two advantages in using the written modality to test general L2 skills in third age learners: first, the performance in listening comprehension tasks is likely to be affected by individual differences in hearing loss, which are known to affect even language comprehension in L1 (see e.g. Giroud et al., 2017), and would therefore be biased towards learners with better hearing. Second, both speech comprehension and production rely heavily on processing speed and working memory capacity, both of which have been shown to be reduced in older adults (see literature review). At each measurement, all tests were pseudo-randomized between individuals and performed in one session.
4.4.1. FL proficiency tasks
Different versions of language tests were used to assess skills that were trained in the course and could therefore be expected to improve with time. The tests were administered at T2 and T3 and addressed both language production and language reception, as well as both aspects that are susceptible to aging (e.g. morpho-syntax) and those that are less vulnerable to age-related changes (e.g. lexico-semantics) (see e.g. Johnstone, 2002).
We administered the C-Test to assess grammatical production skills (e.g. Aguado et al., 2007). It has been validated and found to correlate with self-evaluation procedures, school grades, and other language tests and batteries (Raatz & Klein-Braley, 2002) and has been used successfully in previous studies of third age language learners (e.g. Keijzer & Schmid, 2016); furthermore, it is able to track differences in performance even after a short FL training period (see Kliesch et al., 2018). At T1 the test included 70 gaps; the participants were given three points for correct answers (meaning a max. of 210 points was possible) and zero points for wrong answers. Two points were given for an answer with an orthographic (spelling) error (e.g. peopl instead of people), and one point for a response with a phonological error (e.g. vilis instead of village). At T3 the participants had to fill in 111 gaps (max. score: 333 points).
Receptive vocabulary was assessed using an odd-one-out task as well as an association task in both post-tests. In the odd-one-out task eight words and 10 words respectively were presented visually at T2 and T3, and participants were asked to identify the odd-one-out lexical item. In the association task participants had to underline all the words that did not fit the umbrella term.
4.4.2. Cognitive tasks
The Stroop Task (Stroop, 1935) was administered at T1 and T3 to measure verbal and non-verbal inhibition skills, respectively, while attention and concentration were assessed via the concentration test for geriatric patients (“Alters-Konzentrations-Test A-K-T”, Gatterer, 1989) at T1, T2 and T3. In the latter test, the participant has to focus on the position and pattern of symbols on a sheet of paper and has to find and mark every symbol that has the same position and pattern as the symbol given at the top of the sheet. The time needed as well as the correctly and incorrectly marked symbols are assessed. The test also provides information regarding the different stages of dementia; a criterion for a cerebral deterioration process would be a change in the ability to concentrate. In Kliesch et al. (2018) computerized tests proved too complicated and confusing for older participants, which is why in this study it was decided to forego computer tests to avoid confounds based on different PC-skills among older learners.
4.4.3. Socio-affective tasks
In order to account for the socio-affective dimension of FL learning in a classroom setting, we administered a questionnaire with 27 open-ended items, one closed-ended item and one question that required drawing a motivation curve so as to evaluate learning experiences, motivation, attitudes, overall well-being, personal goal-setting, age negotiations, the construction of aging identities, autonomy, L2 awareness, anxiety, expectations, self-confidence and learning strategies. The questionnaire was administered at T3 and consisted of the following main dimensions:
To add a further quantitative dimension, we administered a FL learning questionnaire and the Multidimensional Mood State Questionnnaire (Mehrdimensionaler Befindlichkeitsfragebogen MDBF) before and after each class. The latter consists of 24 items with a five-point Likert-scale, which assess a bipolar dimension of the mental state: good/bad mood (Steyer et al., 1997). A high score indicates a positive mood; the person feels comfortable, cheerful and satisfied. Low scores indicate a bad mood and the person feels uncomfortable; s/he is bad-tempered, gloomy and dissatisfied.
4.5. Data analysis
Because of inherent task differences, performance can differ across tasks (see Bak et al., 2016). For this reason, we did not calculate a composite score for the subtests, but rather analyzed each subtest separately.
The data were analyzed in accordance with their properties by Wilcoxon tests and Mann-Whitney U-tests. Bonferroni corrections were used where appropriate (p < .05). Furthermore, effect sizes were calculated for the difference between two median values (suitable for the Mann-Whitney U-test and the Wilcoxon test) from the standardized test statistics of tests (z) and the number of cases (n). Judging effect sizes was based on Cohen’s (1988) guidelines (r = 0.1 small, r = 0.3 medium, r = 0.5 large).
In the qualitative analysis we pursued an inductive, bottom-up approach, performing a content/theme analysis using the method proposed by Braun and Clarke (2006). At the first step, the responses were read separately multiple times. Second, the ideas expressed in the written responses were regrouped into themes. Afterwards, the responses were read again to check theme accuracy. Finally, the quotes selected from the participants’ interviews were translated from German to English by the second author.
5. Results
5.1. Impact of EFL training on EFL gains
The results of the odd-one-out test indicated a significant difference from the unilateral test level between both measurement times (see Table 1); after Bonferroni correction, however, this result was no longer significant (p = 0.017). The effect sizes, however, are large for both groups.
Wilcoxon-Test | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
------------ | |||||
Tasks | T2 (n = 12) | T3 (n = 12) | Z | punilateral | Effect size r |
------------ | |||||
M (SD) | M (SD) | ||||
------------ | |||||
Odd-one-out | 68.83 (12.48) | 80.91 (9.44) | –2.051 | 0.02 | –0.618 |
Association | 87.50 (12.51) | 92.73 (6.47) | –0.971 | 0.16 | –0.293 |
C-test | 75.19 (16.14) | 81.46 (7.86) | –0.978 | 0.16 | –0.295 |
In the association test and the C-test, there was no significant growth either, although all participants, i.e. bilinguals in the experimental group and monolinguals in the control group, improved their scores (see Figure 1 for the C-test). The effect sizes were relatively small for both tests.
That said, a finer-grained analysis revealed that while the participants did not make any significant gains with respect to overall correct scores in the C-test, they produced significantly fewer incorrect answers – unfilled gaps and incorrect answers (Z = –2.845, punilateral = 0.002, r = –0.859), fewer orthographic errors (Z = –1.779, punilateral = 0.037, r = –0.536) and fewer phonological errors (Z = –2.937, punilateral = 0.001, r = –0.886), with large effect sizes throughout. Also, with time, the groups became more homogeneous (less variance between the mean and the standard deviation).
As Table 1 above shows, there seemed to be a tendency for the control group (monolinguals) to learn faster at the beginning (after two weeks), while the experimental (bilingual) group made faster progress after two weeks, i.e. they were able to catch up with the monolingual group. Mann-Whitney U-tests, however, revealed no significant differences between the bilinguals and the monolinguals for any of the tasks: odd-one-out test (T2: Z = –0.512, punilateral = 0.304, r = –0.154; T3: Z = –0.873, punilateral = 0.191, r = –0.263); association test (T2: Z = –1.152, punilater = 0.125, r = –0.347; T3: Z = –0.512, punilater = 0.305, r = –0.154); C-test (T2: Z = –0.365, punilateral = 0.396, r = is –0.110; T3: Z = –0.365, punilateral = 0.356, r = –0.110).
5.2. Impact of FL training on cognitive abilities
The FL training had a significant impact on inhibition and interference, as measured by the Stroop test (see Figure 2 below), but not on cognitive control and attention (as measured by the A-K-T) (see Figure 3), although many participants (66%) reported noticing some changes in their ability to focus and concentrate (see qualitative results below).
Figure 3 Mean time required for each group in the geriatric concentration test (AKT) (in secs).
Tables 2 and 3 summarize these results.
Table 2
Summary of group performances in the Stroop test.
Wilcoxon-Test | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
------------ | |||||
Groups | T1 (n = 6) | T3 (n = 6) | Z | punilateral | Effect size r |
------------ | |||||
M (SD) | M (SD) | ||||
------------ | |||||
bilingual | 198.23 (58.86) | 172.93 (42.38) | –1.992 | 0.023 | –0.813 |
monolingual | 92.45 (19.34) | 82.77 (19.37) | –2.201 | 0.014 | –0.897 |
Table 3
Summary of group performances in the A-K-T.
Wilcoxon-Test | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
------------ | |||||
Groups | T1 (n = 6) | T3 (n = 6) | Z | punilateral | Effect size r |
------------ | |||||
M (SD) | M (SD) | ||||
------------ | |||||
bilingual | 118.80 (73.13) | 91.80 (34.03) | –1.219 | 0.112 | –0.545 |
monolingual | 34.33 (5.28) | 28.83 (4.58) | –1.782 | 0.038 | –0.728 |
While both groups made significant gains on the Stroop test, the inhibitory performance was better in monolinguals (control group) than bilinguals (experimental group) at both data collection times. What is more, the control group (monolinguals) self-corrected on average 60.58% of the errors at T1 (SD = 41.02), while at T3, 90.88% (SD = 10.50) were detected and corrected. This difference of proportionally corrected errors in relation to the total errors within both measurement times was statistically significant (Z = –1.826, punilateral = 0.034, r = –0.746). At T3, the experimental group (bilinguals) also identified and self-corrected a higher percentage of errors; however, this group showed no statistically significant improvement (Z = –0.674, punilateral = 0.250, r = –0.275). Likewise, while the experimental group showed no significant growth on the concentration test (A-K-T) (Z = –1.219, punilateral = 0.223/2 = 0.112, r = –0.545), the control group were able to improve their scores significantly, with large effect size (Z = –1.782, punilateral = 0.0375, r = –0.728).
5.3. Impact of L2 training on socio-affective factors
In the first dimension of the questionnaire, the participants were asked about their motivation to participate in an English language course (see Figure 4).
Figure 4 Participants’ motivation to participate in the FL classroom (count of participants per dimension). Note: CHA = new challenge; COM = communicate in English; FAM = motivated by friends and family; INT = general interest in the English language and the course; OTH = other; TRA = traveling; UND = understanding texts in English.
In contrast to the bilinguals, the main motivation for all participants in the monolingual group was a general interest in the English language and the English course, as well as a desire to understand texts in English; other motivations were: communicating with English-speaking people, travelling, and facing a new challenge. The main motivation for the bilingual participants was to communicate with English-speaking people. All 12 participants developed a personal goal over time, such as “keep learning/mutual learning”, “being able to communicate with others”, “knowing another language”, “ambition and thirst for knowledge”, “curiosity”, “fun”, “reading”, “thinking”, “writing” and “laughing”.
83% of the monolinguals claimed that the English course had a positive effect on their social life. They observed an increase in conversations with other people (family, friends, neighbors); two participants mentioned that they had told others about the course and that “it was well received”. Four participants reported that they “had fun” and “were praised by others” for attending the course; and also four of them remarked that their perspectives had changed, for example, in that they got new ideas such as travelling to an English-speaking country; communicating with other people was facilitated on vacation; 66% expressed a desire for another English-language program in the future. One participant reported trying to approach and greet people in English in his daily life. Four participants noted that their personal well-being improved in that their “time management in their everyday life was better”; they “felt much better”, were “very content” with themselves and “proud” that they “persevered”; they “felt more comfortable” also because of their personal impression of being able to “remember more things”. Furthermore, they considered it a “personal enrichment and valuable experience”. Only one participant “did not notice any change” in his well-being. The participants highlighted that their “self-confidence improved drastically” inasmuch as they felt “more attentive and stronger” and “content and proud of the progress” they had made. One participant reported a “higher stress level” due to her busy lifestyle.
The English course also had a positive influence on all participants in the bilingual group. In particular, they reported establishing strong social ties with one another; they had “fun within the group”, “fun with other elderly people” and the “conversations with other people in the house increased”. They found the language course cognitively stimulating in that they had gained insights into “new learning perspectives”. All participants reported an overall “boost in emotional well-being”; they “felt much better and optimistic”, with one participant highlighting that she “did not have any depressions any more”. The course had had a “positive influence” on five out of six participants’ “self-confidence”, as well as on their “self-esteem”. Two participants reported that the “family supported” them, and it seemed to be important that their relatives were “proud” of them.
All participants in the monolingual group preferred teaching methods such as group discussions, reading and playing games; five participants also enjoyed writing dialogues. In the bilingual group, four participants preferred activities such as group discussions, reading, watching YouTube videos and teacher-centered instruction. The participants “did not feel old”, they felt “taken seriously”, and the course was “encouraging” and “positively demanding”.
Asked about what personality traits a course instructor should have for a course with elderly people (60+) and what he/she should consider, the answers were the following: “speak slowly and loudly”, “repeat often”, “give simple instructions”, be “funny”, “cheerful”, “patient”, “friendly, “empathetic”, “encouraging”, “positively demanding”, “determined”, “respectful”, “humorous” and “not give the feeling that we are old”; and that he/she should have “stamina” as well.
The results from the closed-ended item in the motivation questionnaire, administered in each class, corroborated these positive findings: between T1 and T2 (first and last teaching unit) a statistically significant result could be observed, with a large effect size (Z = –2.677, punilateral = 0.007/2 = 0.0035, r = –0.847). As Figure 5 shows, learning motivation kept increasing with time.
Interestingly, these observations are not reflected in the quantitative findings yielded by the Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire (MDBF). Between T1 and T12 (first and last teaching unit) no significant difference could be detected (Z = –0.211, punilateral = 0.417; r = –0.063). Figure 6 illustrates the descriptive statistics for both groups before and after each English lesson.
Figure 6 Scores in the Mood State Questionnaire across all participants before and after each class.
6. Discussion
Our results demonstrate a significant improvement in a range of linguistic, cognitive and socio-affective tasks as a function of a four-week intensive English course. Although we could not observe any gains in the receptive language tasks, all participants were able to fill in significantly more gaps in the C-test after the language course, and significantly fewer errors were made in each error domain. Moreover, the improvement neither depended on biological age nor on bilingualism, which stands in contrast to Cox’s (2017) study, in which bilinguals consistently outperformed monolinguals.
The language training also led to measurable improvements on executive tasks (inhibition, interference) but not on focus and concentration – although there was a perceived increase in focus and attention on the part of the participants. These findings expand on the results of previous research (e.g. Bak et al., 2016) by demonstrating a language learning-related attentional and socio-affective improvement longitudinally within the same participants: the improvements were noted across all age-groups, from 63 to 89 years old, and among monolinguals and bilinguals. Hence, the current study is of help in ascertaining whether third-agers’ cognitive abilities can be improved by teaching them a new language, which is important considering the contradictory outcomes of studies on the bilingual advantage with respect to domain-general cognitive abilities (see literature review). However, since the knowledge and skills that are inevitably accumulated as experience grows are not controlled for in these tests, scientific prudence indicates that the way the results of these tests are interpreted should be tempered.
In line with predictions from social psychology and SLA (e.g. Ware et al., 2017), language training has been shown to lead to measurable improvements beyond the practiced tasks, with gains in the realm of (linguistic) self-confidence, communicative skills and subjective well-being, independent of the age of participants and bilingualism. Although the Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire did not reveal any significant improvement in terms of overall well-being from T1 to T12, the quantitative results displayed consistently high ratings on subjective well-being scales before and after each English class, with minimally higher ratings at the end of each course. On the basis of the second author’s classroom observations we speculate that this might have to do with the fact that participants were already very excited at the beginning of each class so that the training could not improve their mood greatly. The responses in the open-ended questionnaire, on the other hand, clearly suggest that language learning is perceived not only as a social activity but also as cognitive stimulation and connection with society. Participants in our study expressed their motivations for learning English in terms of the use of new behaviors and practices, the development of communicative strategies and desire to continue the program. These are discourses of positive aging (see Andrew 2012): their remarks advocate accepting aging as a fact of life and finding a sense of life satisfaction. Learning English is one way they have found to remain active and to continue to advance personally. More importantly, it is a major source of prestige for them in view of all that English signifies in terms of cultural capital. While they very much enjoy the academic challenge of learning, this undertaking has much more to do with their life in the world beyond the classroom. The participants often pointed to the importance of the approval and acknowledgment of their family members; they also had a tendency to compare themselves with other older adults (inside and outside their language classes). Similarly, Sigelman and Rider (2012) note that older adults can maintain self-esteem by comparing themselves socially or physically with other older adults, rather than with younger adults. Oxford (2018) reasons that when third agers apply ageist stereotypes (learned as early as childhood in some cultures, Levy, 2003) to themselves, their self-esteem suffers, but if they attach these stereotypes to other older adults, they feel better about themselves – a hypothesis supported by our findings.
7. Conclusion
Both the applied linguistics-related and the cognitive study of third-age additional language learning are at points where the topic is gaining traction, and we are at a juncture to take stock of what has been investigated, to what extent efforts have been fruitful, and how we should proceed. This pilot study focused on the effects of FL learning on older adults who begin the relevant language learning process in old age in a school context in the L1 community – a novel undertaking in SLA research.
Our findings not only present some initial support for the hypothesis that FL learning in older adults yield cognitive, linguistic and socio-affective benefits; they also highlight the importance of learning a completely new language in the third age not just as a goal in itself but as a means of promoting social interaction and integration – an important finding considering that it is partly through the stimulation of social well-being that its cognitive effects may be observed (Pot et al., 2018).
Our results are relevant not only for Austrian geragogy but for any country where third-age universities and courses are on the increase (for a further discussion of critical FL geragogy, see Ramírez Gómez (2016)). Understanding how cognition and motivation are involved in FL learning in older adults is essential for adult educators, policy-makers and designers of course materials and syllabi to develop teaching materials which cater for the needs of this learner group and to make decisions about type and impact of late additional language instruction. It is important to bear in mind that since there are multiple differences both within and between populations of older adults, there is no “one-size-fits-all” solution when teaching this age group; therefore, investigation into different approaches in diverse contexts is essential at this point.
The findings presented in this article are preliminary and the conclusions drawn from this study must be considered against the background of the methodological constraints inherent in the design (e.g. small sample sizes, high individual variation, etc.). In continuing this line of research, it will be critical for us to include a passive control group and, possibly, another active control group engaging in another type of training that does not involve the use of language, e.g. playing with a computer-based strategy game that has been shown to yield significant cognitive improvement in older adults (see e.g. Basak et al., 2008). Furthermore, research designs of existing explorations of these issues are typically case studies and “one-shot” group studies. Dense longitudinal studies will enable us to reach clear conclusions on the effects of third-age language learning. Finally, as Lenet et al. (2011) argue, older learners might profit more from implicit L2 training (but cf. Cox, 2017), while Brill-Schuetz and Morgan-Short (2014) demonstrate that only learners with high procedural memory show an advantage in implicit L2 learning conditions. Thus, future studies could investigate individual L2/FL progress under each learning condition, taking into consideration differences in cognitive capacities, such as procedural memory.
As we see it, one of the biggest challenges in this line of research is the prevalent ‘deficit view’, i.e. the fact that the ageism of vernacular representations of older people is inadvertently included in the representation of research findings:
Removing debilitating stereotypes in research design, task design and data analysis is a fundamental step towards understanding age-related changes and age parameters, and, by extension, encouraging and aiding older language learners.
Notes
1Since our study includes participants at successive levels of language ability within the same language users, we refer to it as ‘longitudinal research’. Note, however, that the short-span length of investigation chosen here suffers in comparison with those of at least several months, which is more typical of longitudinal design in SLA research.
Acknowledgements
Special thanks are due to Anton Laireiter for providing invaluable assistance as far as the cognitive tests were concerned, and to David Singleton for his insightful and generous feedback. We are also greatly indebted to the third age learners for their enthusiastic participation and support. Any errors, of course, remain our own.
Editor’s Note
Winner of the best JESLA paper for 2018.
Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 207–245. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654310368803
Aguado, K., Grotjahn, R., & Schlak, T. (2007). Erwerbsalter und Sprachlernerfolg: Zeitlimitierte C-Tests als Instrument zur Messung prozeduralen sprachlichen Wissens. In: Helmut, J. V. (Ed.), Synergieeffekte in der Fremdsprachenforschung: Empirische Zugänge, Probleme, Ergebnisse, 137–149. Frankfurt am Main: Lang.
Andrew, P. (2012). The social construction of age: Adult foreign language learners. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Antoniou, M., Gunasekera, G. M., & Wong, P. C. M. (2013). Foreign language training as cognitive therapy for age-related cognitive decline: A hypothesis for future research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10), 2689–2698. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.004
Bak, T. H., Long, M. R., Vega-Mendoza, M., & Sorace, A. (2016). Novelty, challenge, and practice: The impact of intensive language learning on attentional functions. PLoS One, 11, 1–11. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153485
Basak, C., Boot, W. R., Voss, M. W., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 765–777. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013494
Bialystok, E., Abutalebi, J., Bak, T. H., Burke, D. M., & Kroll, J. F. (2016). Aging in two languages: Implications for public health. Ageing Research Reviews, 27, 56–60. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2016.03.003
Bialystok, E., & Poarch, G. J. (2014). Language experience changes language and cognitive ability. Zeitschrift Für Erziehungswissenschaft, 17(3), 433–446. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11618-014-0491-8
Brändle, M. (1986). Language teaching for the “young-old”. Babel, 21(1), 17–21.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol., 3, 77–101. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
Brill-Schuetz, K. A., & Morgan-Short, K. (2014). The role of procedural memory in adult second language acquisition. In: Bello, P., Guarini, M., McShane, M., & Scassellati, B. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 260–265. Cognitive Science Society: Quebec City.
Castañeda, S. B. (2016). Methodologies for teaching English to adult students in Spanish vocational education programs. Journal of Professional, Continuing and Online Education, 2(1), 1–23. https://journals.library.ualberta.ca/jpcoe/index.php/jpcoe/article/view/20/10. DOI: https://doi.org/10.18741/P9KW2P
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
Cox, J. G. (2017). Explicit instruction, bilingualism, and the older adult learner. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39, 29–58. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263115000364
Damoiseaux, J. S., Beckmann, C. F., Arigita, E. J. S., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C. J., Rombouts, S. a. R. B., et al. (2008). Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(8), 1856–1864. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263115000364
Dowens, M. G., Vergara, M., Barber, H. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Morphosyntactic processing in late second-language learners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1870–1887. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21304
Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. (2004). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. In: Ross, B. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory. Elsevier Academic Press.
Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.) (2018). Third age learners of foreign languages. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Gatterer, G. (1989). The A-K-T (“Alters-Konzentrations-Test”): A new psychometric test for geriatric patients. Funct. Neurol., 4, 273–276.
Giroud, N., Lemke, U., Reich, P., Matthes, K. L., & Meyer, M. (2017). Longitudinal auditory learning facilitates auditory cognition as revealed by microstate analysis. Biological Psychology, 123, 25–36. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.11.007
Green, D. (2018). The interactional challenge: L2 learning and use in the third age. In: Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages, 31–47. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Isopahkala-Bouret, U. (2015). Graduation at age 50+: Contested efforts to construct “third age” identities and negotiate cultural age stereotypes. Journal of Aging Studies, 35, 1–9. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaging.2015.07.001
Johnstone, R. (2002). Addressing ‘the age factor’: Some implications for language policy. Strasbourg: Council of Europe.
Kalfus, R. (1977). A new audience for foreign language instruction: The older adult. Association of Departments of Foreign Languages Bulletin, 9, 49–50. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1632/adfl.9.2.49
Keijzer, M. C. J., & Schmid, M. (2016). Individual differences in cognitive control advantages of elderly late Dutch-English bilinguals. In: Bialystok, E., & Sullivan, M. D. (Eds.), Aging and Bilingualism, 64–85. Amsterdam: John Benjamin. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.14032.kei
Kliesch, M., Giroud, N., Pfenninger, S. E., & Meyer, M. (2018). Research on second language acquisition in old adulthood: What we have and what we need. In: Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages, 48–76. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Knowles, M., Holton, E., & Swanson, R. (2011). The adult learner: The definitive classic in adult education and human resource development (7th ed). Amsterdam; Boston: Elsevier.
Laslett, P. (1987). The emergence of the third age. Ageing & Society, 7(2), 133–160. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X00012538
Lenet, A., Sanz, C., Lado, B., Howard, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (2011). Aging, pedagogical conditions, and differential success in SLA: An empirical study. In: Sanz, C., & Leow, R. P. (Eds.), Implicit and explicit conditions, Processes and knowledge in SLA and bilingualism, 73–84. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
Levy, B. (2003). Mind matters: Cognitive and physical effects of aging self-stereotypes. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58, 203–211. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/58.4.P203
Liang, J., & Luo, B. (2012). Toward a discourse shift in social gerontology: From successful aging to harmonious aging. Journal of Aging Studies, 26, 327–334. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaging.2012.03.001
Lindenberger, U. (2014). Human cognitive aging. Science, 346(6209), 572–578. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1254403
Mackey, A., & Sachs, R. (2012). Older learners in SLA research: A first look at working memory, feedback, and L2 development. Language Learning, 62(3), 704–740. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2011.00649.x
Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Old, S. R. (2008). Aging and memory. In: Byrne, J. H., Eichenbaum, H., Menzel, R., Roediger, H. L., & Sweatt, D. (Eds.), Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference, 787–808. Oxford, UK: Elsevier. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012370509-9.00133-9
Oxford, R. (2018). A developmental perspective on third-age learning. In: Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages, 3–18. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014
Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173–196. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656
Pfenninger, S. E., & Singleton, D. (in press). Starting age overshadowed: The primacy of differential environmental and input effects on L2 attainment in an instructional context. Language Learning.
Pot, A., Keijzer, M., & de Bot, K. (2018). Enhancing language awareness in migrants’ third age to promote wellbeing. In: Gabrys-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages, 176–200. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Ramirez Gómez, D. (2016). Language teaching and the older adult: The significance of experience. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI: https://doi.org/10.21832/9781783096305
Ramos, S., Fernández García, Y., Antón, E., Casaponsa, A., & Andoni Duñabeitia, J. (2017). Does learning a language in the elderly enhance switching ability? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 43, 39–48. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.09.001
Ramscar, M., Hendrix, P., Shaoul, C., Milin, P., & Baayen, H. (2014). The myth of cognitive decline: Non-linear dynamics of lifelong learning. Topics in Cognitive Science 6, 5–42. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12078
Rughiniş, C., & Humă, B. (2015). Who theorizes age? The “socio-demographic variables” device and age–period–cohort analysis in the rhetoric of survey research. Journal of Aging Studies, 35, 144–159. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaging.2015.07.005
Salthouse, T. A. (2000). Aging and measures of processing speed. Biological Psychology, 54(1), 35–54. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00052-1
Schuller, T. (2004). Three capitals: A framework. In: Schuller, T., Preston, J., Hammond, C., Brassett-Grundy, A., & Bynner, J. (Eds.), The benefits of learning the impact of education on health, family life and social capital, 12–33. London: Routledge Falmer.
Sigelman, C. K., & Rider, E. A. (2012). Life-span Human Development. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Singleton, D., & Pfenninger, S. E. (2015). Insights from a mixed methods approach with respect to age and long-term instructed language learning. In: Navracsics, J., & Bátyi, Sz. (Eds.), First and second language: Interdisciplinary approaches, 11–21. Budapest: Tinta Könyvkiadó.
Stern, C., & Munn, Z. (2010). Cognitive leisure activities and their role in preventing dementia: a systematic review. International Journal of Evidence-Based Healthcare, 8(1), 2–17. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-1609.2010.00150.x
Steyer, R., Schwenkmezger, P., Notz, P., & Eid, M. (1997). Der Mehrdimensionale Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF). Göttingen: Verlag für Psychologie.
Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (2011). Languaging as agent and constituent of cognitive change in an older adult: An example. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics/Revue Canadienne de Linguistique Appliquée, 14(1), 104–117.
United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. (2015). World population ageing 2015. New York: United Nations.
Van Der Meij, M., Cuetos, F., Carreiras, M., & Barber, H. A. (2011). Electrophysiological correlates of language switching in second language learners. Psychophysiology, 48(1), 44–54. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01039.x
Verhaeghen, P., & Cerella, J. (2008). Everything We Know About Aging and Response Times: A Meta-Analytic Integration. In: Handbook of Cognitive Aging: Interdisciplinary Perspectives, 134–150. California: Thousand Oaks. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412976589.n8
Ware, C., Damnee, S., Djabelkhir, L., Cristancho, V., Wu, Y.-H., et al. (2017). Maintaining cognitive functioning in healthy seniors with a technology-based foreign language program: A pilot feasibility study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9(42), 1–10. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00042
Wingfield, A., & Grossman, M. (2006). Language and the aging brain: Patterns of neural compensation revealed by functional brain imaging. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(6), 2830–2839. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00628.2006
Simone E. Pfenninger, [email protected], University of Salzburg, AT
About Simone E.
Simone E. Pfenninger is Associate Professor at the English Department of the University of Salzburg. Her principal research areas are multilingualism, psycholinguistics and individual differences (e.g. the age factor) in second language acquisition, especially in regard to quantitative approaches and statistical methods and techniques for language application in education. Her work is published in several books, edited volumes and in journals such as Second Language Research and the International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. Recent books include Beyond Age Effects in Instructional L2 Learning: Revisiting the Age Factor (2017, co-authored, Multilingual Matters), The Changing English Language: Psycholinguistic Perspectives (2017, co-edited, CUP), and Future Research Directions for Applied Linguistics (2017, co-edited, Multilingual Matters). She is co-editor of the Second Language Acquisition book series for Multilingual Matters and recipient of the 2015 Mercator Award and the 2018 Conrad Ferdinand Meyer Prize for outstanding research.
Sabrina Polz, University of Salzburg, AT
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2018. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 (“the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
While there is a growing body of research on second language acquisition in children and prime-of-life learners, much remains to be explored about how older adults learn a new language and how good additional language learning is for them (see e.g. Mackey & Sachs, 2012). In this study we present the findings of a longitudinal pilot study in which 12 German-speaking subjects, half of them German-Slovenian bilinguals, between 63 and 90 years of age attended a four-week intensive English course for beginners, and were tested in a pre-/post-test design on a range of linguistic, cognitive, and socio-affective parameters. The results of non-parametric statistical tests and qualitative analyses suggest that: (1) the learning of an additional language in the third age can contribute to healthy and active aging, as it has a positive effect on executive function, (linguistic) self-confidence, autonomy, communicative skills and overall well-being, irrespective of age and prior language knowledge (bilingualism); and (2) age-related social, psychological and contextual factors seem to play as significant a role as strictly maturational factors. Such research promises in due course to inform adult educators about the establishment of relevant third-age learner profiles as well as the design of individualized third-age language training.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer