Introduction
Infertility is a global health issue [1] that often poses a physical, psychological, sociocultural, emotional, and financial burden on affected individuals [2]. Studies have shown that infertile couples experience significant anxiety and emotional distress, often describing it as “the most upsetting experience in their lives,” classifying it as either stressful or extremely stressful [3], with anxiety and depression being the most commonly reported mental health concerns [4]. Further, infertility is often seen as a silent struggle that is not openly discussed [5,6] due to associated feelings of shame, low self-esteem, fear that others won’t understand how they feel, and stigma due to social and cultural norms and values [7,8]. This secrecy in effect exacerbates/ evokes feelings of loneliness and isolation as individuals withdraw from sources of support, at a time when they paradoxically may need it the most [7]. In effect, this constellation of factors constitutes/creates significant challenges/obstacles for infertile individuals to maintain high levels of perceived social support.
The role of social support as a buffer in the context of numerous life stressors is well known [9]. It is thus of no surprise that social support has a positive effect on individuals experiencing issues with fertility [10], especially as a vulnerable population that is already at a higher risk of developing depression [5]. Specifically, studies suggest that higher perceptions of social support availability are associated with lower levels of general and infertility distress in both men and women [10,11].
Perceived social support is best conceptualized as a person’s subjective appraisal of his or her situation, rather than a true reflection of how much support is received [12].
In fact, perceived social support has been shown to be more predictive of health and more reliable in buffering against the adverse effects of stressors on psychological and physical well-being [13]. In a study of individuals with depression, greater perceived social support in contrast to greater received social support had a significantly larger relationship with lower depressive symptoms [12,14]. Moreover, perceived social support had a weak association with received support.
What is less understood is whether the perceptions of social support between the two sexes in the context of infertility differ. The literature on sex differences in the context of social support is mixed and inconsistent [13]. Given biomedical differences and differences in socialization processes and gender-role expectations [15], it is reasonable to anticipate differences in how infertility may be perceived and experienced between men and women. For example, male-factor infertility may be perceived as ‘inferior’ sperm quality and to some extent affect infertile males' perceptions of their masculinity [16]. For females, on the other hand, an inability to bear children and fulfill that ‘biological capability’ may challenge their core female identity and evoke intense fears of being blamed for the inability to give birth to a child [6,17]. Perhaps borne from this perception that motherhood is an intrinsic component of female nature and function, studies suggest that women were more likely to be labeled both by themselves and by others as responsible, regardless of which partner was actually infertile [17].
The purpose of this study is to examine whether there are differences in the level of perceived social support between sexes in the context of infertility. While some studies have investigated gender differences within couples with male-factor infertility and/or female-factor infertility [18], to our knowledge, there are no studies that focus on the personalization of the individual sexes’ perception of their social support. This study specifically aims to explore those gender differences in solely infertile individuals without regard to their partner (males with male-factor infertility vs. females with female-factor infertility. This understanding will provide insight and information for developing interventions to improve the experiences of infertile individuals.
Materials and methods
Participants and study design
Participants were recruited through an anonymous Qualtrics survey (Qualtrics, North Sydney, Australia) posted to various online infertility support groups. All individuals above 18 years of age who reported experiencing issues with infertility (currently or prior) were eligible to participate in this observational, cross-sectional study. Participation in the study was voluntary and no individuals were excluded as long as they were at least 18 years of age. There was no cost nor compensation to the participants participating in this study. The study was approved by the Rowan University Institutional Review Board #Pro2020001151 (16 September 2021).
Survey tool
Participants who consented to this study completed a 24-question Qualtrics survey. Eight questions assessed demographics including: age, sex assigned at birth, race/ ethnicity, marital status, sexual orientation, gender identity, education level and household income. Fifteen questions assessed additional background information consisting of questions regarding relationships, pregnancies, infertility and social support network. The 12-item Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) [19] was used to assess participants’ level of perceived social support. The scale is comprised of three subscales which correspond with distinct sources of social support: significant other, family and friends (see Table 1).
Table 1
MSPSS Factors and Item Content
MSPSS: Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support
Family |
My family really tries to help me |
I get the emotional help and support I need from my family |
I can talk about my problems with my family |
My family is willing to help me make decisions |
Friends |
My friends really try to help me |
I can count on my friends when things go wrong |
I have friends with whom I can share my joys and sorrows |
I can talk about my problems with my friends |
Significant Other |
There is a special person who is around when I am in need |
There is a special person with whom I can share my joys and sorrows |
I have a special person who is a real source of comfort to me |
There is a special person in my life who cares about my feelings |
Each subscale consists of four items with response options ranging from 1 (very strongly disagree) to 7 (very strongly agree). Higher scores indicate greater perceived social support. The MSPSS has been shown to be a valid measure of perceived social support and has demonstrated high internal consistency as well as reliability and validity with alpha values for the subscales and total scale between 85 and 91 [19]. Statistical analyses of survey results were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 28 (IBM Corp, Armonk, USA).
Results
Demographic data were collected from participants and summarized in Table 2.
Table 2
Demographic Characteristics of Respondents (n= 938)
Demographic Variables | n =938 (%) | Male sex at birth (n=60) (%) | Female sex at birth (n=878) (%) | |
Sex at birth | Male | 60 (7.55) | ||
Female | 878 (92.45) | |||
Age range | 18-23 | 15 (1.60) | 2 (3.33) | 13 (1.48) |
24-29 | 178 (18.98) | 8 (13.33) | 170 (19.36) | |
30-35 | 508 (54.16) | 26 (43.33) | 482 (54.90) | |
36-41 | 209 (22.28) | 16 (26.67) | 193 (21.98) | |
42-47 | 28 (2.99) | 8 (13.33) | 20 (2.28) | |
Race/ ethnicity | White | 801 (85.39) | 42 (70.00) | 759 (86.45) |
Hispanic or Latino | 44 (4.69) | 8 (13.33) | 36 (4.10) | |
Black or African American | 16 (1.71) | 3 (5.00) | 13 (1.48) | |
Native American or American Indian | 2 (0.21) | 0 (0) | 2 (0.23) | |
Asian or Pacific Islander | 45 (4.80) | 4 (6.67) | 41 (4.67) | |
Other | 30 (3.20) | 3 (5.00) | 27 (3.08) | |
Marital status | Single | 18 (1.92) | 6 (10.00) | 12 (1.37) |
Married | 826 (88.25) | 48 (80.00) | 778 (88.61) | |
Divorced | 3 (0.32) | 1 (1.67) | 2 (0.23) | |
In a relationship (non-married) | 85 (9.08) | 4 (6.67) | 81 (9.23) | |
Widowed | 1 (0.11) | 1 (1.67) | 0 (0) | |
Other | 3 (0.32) | 0 (0) | 3 (0.34) | |
Sexual orientation | Heterosexual or straight | 793 (84.54) | 56 (93.33) | 737 (83.94) |
Bisexual | 101 (10.77) | 3 (5.00) | 98 (11.16) | |
Homosexual | 18 (1.92) | 0 | 18 (2.05) | |
Pansexual | 19 (2.03) | 0 | 19 (2.16) | |
Asexual | 7 (0.75) | 1 (1.67) | 6 (0.68) | |
Level of education | High school degree or equivalent | 73 (7.78) | 8 (13.33) | 65 (7.40) |
Bachelor’s degree | 390 (41.58) | 29 (48.33) | 361 (41.12) | |
Master’s degree | 295 (31.45) | 10 (16.67) | 285 (32.46) | |
Doctorate degree | 114 (12.15) | 6 (10.00) | 8 (0.91) | |
Other | 66 (7.04) | 7 (11.67) | 59 (6.72) | |
Household income | < $24,999 | 11 (1.18) | 3 (5.00) | 8 (0.91) |
$25,000-$49,999 | 57 (6.09) | 3 (5.00) | 54 (6.16) | |
$50,000-$74,999 | 108 (11.54) | 9 (15.00) | 99 (11.30) | |
$75,000-$99,999 | 145 (15.49) | 6 (10.00) | 139 (15.87) | |
$100,000-$149,999 | 258 (27.56) | 22 (36.67) | 236 (26.94) | |
> $150,000 | 357 (38.14) | 17 (28.33) | 340 (38.81) |
1,018 individuals participated in this study with 938 adequately completing the survey. A total of 878 females and 60 males participated in our study. The majority of respondents were White, between ages 30-35, married, heterosexual, with a bachelor’s degree as the highest level of education and household income above $100,000 amongst both females and females.
Data are mean ± standard deviation, unless otherwise stated. Mean total scores were higher in women (5.13 ± 1.10) compared with men (4.43 ± 1.58). Mean scores on all subscales (SO, family, friend) were higher in women (5.90 ± 1.20, 4.53 ± 1.53, 4.97 ± 1.40) compared with men (5.04 ± 1.90, 4.06 ± 1.76, 4.20 ± 1.81), respectively (Table 3, Figure 1).
Table 3
Group Descriptive Statistics
Factor | Mean | Standard Deviation (SD) | Minimum | Maximum | |
Male | Total | 4.43 | 1.58 | 1 | 6.666667 |
Significant Other | 5.04 | 1.90 | 1 | 7 | |
Family | 4.06 | 1.76 | 1 | 6.75 | |
Friends | 4.20 | 1.81 | 1 | 7 | |
Female | Total | 5.13 | 1.11 | 1 | 7 |
Significant Other | 5.90 | 1.20 | 1 | 7 | |
Family | 4.53 | 1.53 | 1 | 7 | |
Friends | 4.97 | 1.40 | 1 | 7 |
Figure 1
Clustered Bar Chart of Mean Scores by Subscale
A Welch t-test was run to determine if there were differences in scores on the MSPSS subscales (total, SO, family, friend) between males and females, due to the assumption of homogeneity of variances being violated, as assessed by Levene’s test for equality of variances (p < 0.001, p < 0.001, p= 0.013, p < 0.001, respectively). There were outliers in the data, as assessed by inspection of a boxplot, however, it was determined that the outliers would have no significant effect as results were similar when testing both with and without the inclusion of outliers. Scores for all subscales were approximately normally distributed, as assessed by visual inspection of a Normal Q-Q Plot, which is the most preferable method in the setting of large sample sizes. There was a statistically significant difference in mean total score between females and males, with females scoring higher than males, 0.70 (95% CI, -1.11 to -0.28), t(63.018) = -3.360, p = .001. There was a statistically significant difference in mean SO score between females and males, with females scoring higher than males, 0.86 (95% CI, -1.35 to -0.36), t(62.277) = -3.452, p = .001. There was a statistically significant difference in mean family score between females and males, with females scoring higher than males, 0.47 (95% CI, -0.94 to -0.01), t(65.219) = -2.039, p = 0.046. There was a statistically significant difference in mean friend score between females and males, with females scoring higher than males, 0.76 (95% CI, -1.24 to -0.29), t(63.018) = -3.360, p = 0.002 (Table 4). Figure 2 provides a visual representation of the breakdown of individual item scores within the MSPSS for males and females.
Table 4
Independent Samples Test
SO: significant other
Levene's Test for Equality of Variances | |||||||||||
F | Sig. | t | df | Significance | Mean Difference | Std. Error Difference | 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference | ||||
One-Sided p | Two-Sided p | Lower | Upper | ||||||||
Total score | Equal variances assumed | 29.622 | < .001 | -4.573 | 935 | < .001> | < .001> | -.69812 | .15266 | -.99772 | -.39851 |
Equal variances not assumed | -3.360 | 63.018 | < .001> | .001 | -.69812 | .20777 | -1.11332 | -.28292 | |||
SO score | Equal variances assumed | 38.259 | < .001 | -5.111 | 935 | < .001> | < .001> | -.85628 | .16753 | -1.18505 | -.52751 |
Equal variances not assumed | -3.452 | 62.277 | < .001> | .001 | -.85628 | .24806 | -1.35211 | -.36045 | |||
Family score | Equal variances assumed | 6.242 | .013 | -2.308 | 935 | .011 | .021 | -.47473 | .20571 | -.87844 | -.07103 |
Equal variances not assumed | -2.039 | 65.219 | .023 | .046 | -.47473 | .23285 | -.93975 | -.00972 | |||
Friend score | Equal variances assumed | 11.565 | < .001 | -3.991 | 935 | < .001> | < .001> | -.76334 | .19125 | -1.13867 | -.38800 |
Equal variances not assumed | -3.205 | 63.966 | .001 | .002 | -.76334 | .23815 | -1.23910 | -.28757 |
Figure 2
Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support Responses by Gender
As our sample sizes were largely unequal, we additionally repeated our analysis with a random sampling of the larger group (females) to create a more similarly sized comparison group for the smaller group (males), which resulted in similar results. Specifically, we compared 60 male and 64 female participants. Mean total scores were higher in women (5.21 ± 1.09) compared with men (4.43 ± 1.58). Mean scores on all subscales (SO, family, friend) were higher in women (5.88 ± 1.19, 4.66 ± 1.47, 5.10 ± 1.34) compared with men (5.04 ± 1.90, 4.06 ± 1.76, 4.20 ± 1.81), respectively (Table 5). Our findings regarding outliers and normality were the same as described above. As the assumption of homogeneity of variances was again violated, as assessed by Levene’s test for equality of variances (p <0.001, p =0.020, p = 0.005, p < 0.001, respectively), a Welch t-test was run. There was a statistically significant difference in mean total score between females and males, with females scoring higher than males, 0.78 (95% CI, -1.26 to -0.30), t(103.932) = -3.170, p = .002. There was a statistically significant difference in mean SO score between females and males, with females scoring higher than males, 0.84 (95% CI, -1.40 to -0.27), t(97.859) = -2.93, p = .004. There was a statistically significant difference in mean family score between females and males, with females scoring higher than males, 0.60 (95% CI, -1.18 to -0.02), t(115.350) = -2.059, p = 0.042. There was a statistically significant difference in mean friend score between females and males, with females scoring higher than males, 0.90 (95% CI, -1.47 to -0.33), t(108.506) = -3.123, p = 0.002 (Table 6).
Table 5
Group Descriptive Statistics
SO: significant other
sex | N | Mean | Std. Deviation | |
Total score | male | 60 | 4.4347 | 1.58308 |
female | 64 | 5.2135 | 1.09087 | |
Family score | male | 60 | 4.0583 | 1.75898 |
female | 64 | 4.6602 | 1.47221 | |
Friend score | male | 60 | 4.2042 | 1.80776 |
female | 64 | 5.1016 | 1.34055 | |
SO score | male | 60 | 5.0417 | 1.89567 |
female | 64 | 5.8789 | 1.18522 |
Table 6
Independent Samples Test
SO: Significant Other
Levene's Test for Equality of Variances | |||||||||
F | Sig. | t | df | 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference | |||||
One-Sided p | Lower | Upper | |||||||
Total score | Equal variances assumed | 14.456 | < .001> | -3.207 | 122 | < .001> | -1.25958 | -.29805 | |
Equal variances not assumed | -3.170 | 103.932 | .001 | -1.26603 | -.29161 | ||||
Family score | Equal variances assumed | 5.590 | .020 | -2.071 | 122 | .020 | -1.17713 | -.02651 | |
Equal variances not assumed | -2.059 | 115.350 | .021 | -1.18077 | -.02288 | ||||
Friend score | Equal variances assumed | 8.274 | .005 | -3.153 | 122 | .001 | -1.46080 | -.33399 | |
Equal variances not assumed | -3.123 | 108.506 | .001 | -1.46686 | -.32793 | ||||
SO score | Equal variances assumed | 15.817 | < .001> | -2.969 | 122 | .002 | -1.39555 | -.27892 | |
Equal variances not assumed | -2.927 | 97.859 | .002 | -1.40497 | -.26951 |
A two-way ANOVA was conducted on a random smaller sub-sample of individuals who provided their infertility diagnoses to examine the effects of gender and infertility diagnosis on total score. Data are mean ± standard deviation, unless otherwise stated. Residual analyses for each subscale were performed to test for the assumptions of the two-way ANOVA. Outliers were assessed by inspection of a boxplot, normality was assessed using Shapiro-Wilk's normality test for each cell of the design and homogeneity of variances was assessed by Levene's test. There was one outlier in the data, however, it was determined that the outlier would have no significant effect as results were similar when testing both with and without the inclusion of the outlier. Residuals were normally distributed (p > .05) and there was homogeneity of variances (p = 0.085).
There was a statistically significant interaction between sex and infertility diagnosis for "Total score,” F(1, 22) = 6.404, p = .019, partial η2 = 0.225. Therefore, an analysis of simple main effects for gender and for infertility diagnosis was performed with statistical significance receiving a Bonferroni adjustment and being accepted at the p < .025 level.
There was a statistically significant difference in mean “total score” for males with either male-factor or female-factor infertility, F(1,22) = 9.638, p < .005, partial η2 = .305, but not for females, F(1, 22) = 0.225, p = 0.640, partial η2 = 0.010 (Table 7). All pairwise comparisons were run for each simple main effect with reported 95% confidence intervals and p-values Bonferroni-adjusted within each simple main effect. Mean “total scores” for males with male-factor and female-factor infertility were 2.93 ± 1.62 and 5.15 ± 1.28, respectively. Males with male-factor infertility had a statistically significantly lower mean total score than males with female-factor infertility, -2.22 (95% CI, -3.71 to -0.74), p= 0.005. Mean total scores for females with male-factor and female-factor infertility were 5.66 ± 0.80 and 5.32 ± 1.15, respectively. There was no statistically significant difference in mean total scores among females with male-factor and female-factor infertility, 0.34 (95% CI, -1.15 to 1.82), p= 0.640 (Table 8).
Table 7
Univariate Tests for Total Score
Sex | Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | Sig. | Partial Eta Squared | |
male | Contrast | 15.204 | 1 | 15.204 | 9.638 | .005 | .305 |
Error | 34.704 | 22 | 1.577 | ||||
female | Contrast | .355 | 1 | .355 | .225 | .640 | .010 |
Error | 34.704 | 22 | 1.577 |
Table 8
Pairwise Comparisons for Total Score
MFI= male-factor infertility, FFI= female-factor infertility
*The mean difference is significant at the .05 level; bAdjustment for multiple comparisons: Bonferroni
sex | (I) infertility | (J) infertility | Mean Difference (I-J) | Std. Error | Sig.b | 95% Confidence Interval for Differenceb | |
Lower Bound | Upper Bound | ||||||
male | MFI | FFI | -2.223* | .716 | .005 | -3.708 | -.738 |
FFI | MFI | 2.223* | .716 | .005 | .738 | 3.708 | |
female | MFI | FFI | .340 | .716 | .640 | -1.145 | 1.824 |
FFI | MFI | -.340 | .716 | .640 | -1.824 | 1.145 |
For males and females with male-factor infertility, mean “total” score was 2.73 (95% CI, 1.43 to 4.03) points lower for males than females, F(1,22) = 18.89, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.462. In contrast, for males and females with female-factor infertility, mean “total” score was 0.167 (95% CI, -1.81 to 1.48) points lower in males than females, F (1, 22) = 0.044, p < 0.836, partial η2 = 0.002, which was not significantly different (Table 9, Table 10).
Table 9
Univariate Tests for Total Score
MFI= male-factor infertility, FFI= female-factor infertility
infertility | Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | Sig. | Partial Eta Squared | |
MFI | Contrast | 29.793 | 1 | 29.793 | 18.887 | .001 | .462 |
Error | 34.704 | 22 | 1.577 | ||||
FFI | Contrast | .069 | 1 | .069 | .044 | .836 | .002 |
Error | 34.704 | 22 | 1.577 |
Table 10
Pairwise Comparisons for Total Score
MFI= male-factor infertility, FFI= female-factor infertility
*The mean difference is significant at the .05 level; bAdjustment for multiple comparisons: Bonferroni
infertility | (I) sex | (J) sex | Mean Difference (I-J) | Std. Error | Sig.b | 95% Confidence Interval for Differenceb | |
Lower Bound | Upper Bound | ||||||
MFI | male | female | -2.729* | .628 | < .001> | -4.032 | -1.427 |
female | male | 2.729* | .628 | < .001> | 1.427 | 4.032 | |
FFI | male | female | -.167 | .794 | .836 | -1.814 | 1.481 |
female | male | .167 | .794 | .836 | -1.481 | 1.814 |
Discussion
Our findings contribute to a growing body of literature that examines gender differences in perceived social support in couples experiencing issues with fertility. In a comparison of MSPSS scores between males and females, we found higher mean scores across all subscales in females. Numerical scores on each subscale correspond with the following categories: low social support is defined as a score of 1-2.9, moderate as 3-5 and high as 5.1-7. Females were found to have high levels of total and SO perceived support, and moderate levels of family and friend support. In comparison, males had high levels of SO support and moderate levels of total, family, and friend support. The biggest difference between sexes was seen with SO and friend support, with the smallest difference being seen with family support.
While these findings are inconsistent with a majority of existing literature that found no statistically significant gender differences in perceived social support [18,20], several studies have found higher levels of social support in women than in men [18,21-23].
Hosseini et al argue that these differences in social support indicate that women may be considerably more affected by the issue of infertility than men [23], however, we suggest an alternative explanation. Previous studies have found that lower overall scores in males may be associated with lower levels of social support seeking and disclosure to others [17,24-25]. These findings may be related to the profoundly adverse impact that infertility has on masculinity and the more markedly stigmatizing nature of male infertility as men may often conflate infertility, virility and sexual potency which can therefore lead to perceived personal inadequacy [17,26].
Research on North American couples has shown that men struggling with infertility tend to use fewer coping strategies overall and are less apt to use social support [6] or explicitly seek it out and spontaneously disclose their emotional needs [27]. Findings from a Swedish study found that a large proportion of males had not confided in anyone other than their spouse, which is consistent with our findings suggestive of SO as the highest source of social support in males [25,27]. Males diagnosed as infertile were found to avoid disclosure with anyone other than their wives largely due to shame about the diagnosis [27,28]. While it was found that men experience intense ‘emotional anguish’ as a result of infertility, they reported a need for this to be suppressed so as to protect their partners. In similar regard, online support groups, which do not involve face-to-face encounters, were suggested as a possibly useful tool for men to confide in others [27,29]. These findings merely demonstrate a different means of coping in men, but do not necessarily mean that men don’t need the support [26] as men appear to be at least equally affected, if not more affected, by infertility, and perhaps even neglected. It may thus be advantageous for clinicians to explicitly inquire and assess male mental health to detect psychological symptoms. A statement and question such as, “I have met many men experiencing infertility, and they often feel sad, worried, embarrassed, lonely, etc. ... have you had any feelings of this kind [27]?” may be a useful approach to do so.
When comparing scores with respect to infertility diagnoses, males with male-factor infertility showed significantly lower total scores compared to males with female-factor infertility. This finding is consistent with those of Cousinaeu and Domar [6] and Nachtigall et al [30] and is likely related to the aforementioned negative emotional responses and increased stigma associated with male-factor infertility, as is related to perceived masculinity. In contrast, total scores with respect to infertility diagnoses did not differ in females. This may suggest generalized distress over infertility in women, regardless of cause, in contrast to men, where concerns may be amplified or most visible when directly related to the male factor. Alternatively, these findings may simply highlight heightened distress when it is due to the male factor, relative to the already high levels of distress brought on by infertility.
Additionally, amongst males and females with a male-factor infertility diagnosis, total scores were significantly lower in males compared with females, whereas amongst males and females with a female-factor infertility diagnosis, total scores were not statistically significantly different.
Research concerning the psychosocial aspects of infertility and treatment more often focuses on women than men [27]. Our findings suggest that men experience significant psychological repercussions of infertility, highlighting the importance of focusing on the male experience that seems to be under-represented in the literature [6].
A significant strength of our study is seen in our sampling methods as we sampled an online support group rather than a clinic-based population. A majority of previous research relies too heavily on convenience samples drawn from a patient population which is certainly not representative, as this leaves out a large portion of the infertile population - those that don’t seek treatment. That same over-reliance on clinic samples forces a focus on infertility patients rather than on infertility people which makes it difficult to separate the psychological consequences of infertility from the psychological consequences of treatment [22].
Additionally, our male and female samples were not drawn from the infertile couple unit but rather independently. It has been shown that couples' scores on distress scales are correlated and therefore studies which use couple samples for gender comparison may be under-reporting the real extent of the differences between genders [22]. Examining these differences among couples, or the “shared experience” of infertility, may lead to a narrowing of the gender gap. That is, men and women that are together may be more similar than different in their perceptions of social support [17].
Our study had several limitations. Firstly, individuals may have had varying interpretations of what was meant by infertility or by their specific diagnoses. As infertility is an issue that affects both individuals, individuals may have chosen “both male and female-factor” even if the true diagnosis was exclusively “male” or “female-factor.” Further, as a distressing and emotional issue, individuals may consider themselves infertile based on their own definition, even when formally, they don’t meet the criteria required for that diagnosis. While these issues could be adjusted for those individuals that provided their experiences or commentaries at the end of the survey, not all individuals decided to do so, thereby leaving many data points ambiguous. Additionally, we had a limited male sample size. Although this is not surprising given males’ general hesitancy for disclosure, those that did respond may have had different characteristics and/ or sentiments compared with non-responders. While this may make it more difficult to generalize/limits generalizability, our preliminary findings introduce important and overlooked findings regarding the male experience with infertility. Future studies would thus benefit from a larger sample size of males to both further investigate our findings and also to gain more insights and perspectives on the infertile male experience.
Conclusions
In conclusion, we found significantly lower levels of perceived social support in males experiencing issues with infertility as compared with females. Moreover, males with male-factor infertility had lower scores than females with the same diagnosis, or females with female-factor infertility. Given the implications of high levels of perceived social support on improved overall well-being, our findings highlight the importance of implementing interventions that are focused on improving perceptions of social support in males experiencing issues with infertility, with specific consideration given to the unique experiences/challenges and factors that may impact their experience.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright © 2022, Borowczak et al. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Objective
The purpose of this study is to examine whether there are gender differences in the level of perceived social support in couples experiencing issues with fertility.
Methods
A total of 938 participants aged 18-47 years, with self-reported issues of infertility, were evaluated using the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) which comprises three subscales which correspond with distinct sources of social support: significant other (SO), family, and friends. Differences between sexes for total score and for all subscale scores were subsequently analyzed using SPSS Statistics (IBM Corp, Armonk, USA).
Results
Mean total scores and scores on all subscales (SO, family, friend) were higher in women (5.13 ± 1.10, 5.90 ± 1.20, 4.53 ± 1.53, 4.97 ± 1.40, respectively) compared with men (4.43 ± 1.58, 5.04 ± 1.90, 4.06 ± 1.76, 4.20 ± 1.81, respectively), a statistically significant difference of 0.70 (95% CI, -1.11 to -0.28), t(63.018) = -3.360, p = .001), 0.86 (95% CI, -1.35 to -0.36), t(62.277) = -3.452, p = .001, 0.47 (95% CI, -0.94 to -0.01), t(65.219) = -2.039, p = 0.046, 0.76 (95% CI, -1.24 to -0.29), t(63.018) = -3.360, p = 0.002), respectively. Males with male-factor infertility had a statistically significantly lower mean total score than males with female-factor infertility, -2.22 (95% CI, -3.71 to -0.74), p= 0.005. For males and females with male-factor infertility, mean “total” score was 2.73 (95% CI, 1.43 to 4.03) points lower for males than females, F(1,22) = 18.89, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.462.
Conclusion
Perceived social support among individuals experiencing issues with fertility was higher in females than in males across all subscales (SO, family, friends) with the biggest difference seen in SO score. Total scores differed with respect to infertility diagnosis in males but not in females, and amongst males and females with a male-factor infertility diagnosis, total scores were statistically significantly lower in males compared with females. Given the implications of high levels of perceived social support on improved overall well-being, our findings underscore the importance of implementing interventions that are focused on improving perceptions of social support in males experiencing issues with infertility, with specific consideration given to the unique experiences/ challenges and factors that may impact their experience.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer