1. Introduction
In early 2020 the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2), known as COVID-19, spread across the world. By March and April 2020 many countries, including the United Kingdom (UK), were entering strict lockdowns, restricting movement, enforcing work from home, and prohibiting household mixing. The depth and breadth of the restrictions led both researchers and policy makers to suggest that there would be a tsunami of mental health problems. Studies at the beginning of the lockdown did indeed demonstrate an increase in psychological distress [1, 2]. However, as time progressed the impact of the pandemic became more nuanced, with people showing varying trajectories in symptoms and responses [3–5].
In March 2020, the COVID-19 Psychological Research Consortium (C19PRC) was launched in the UK with the aim to investigate the UK population’s psychological, social and political responses to the pandemic. The C19PRC is an international project [6] with parallel surveys in several countries including Italy [7, 8], Spain [9] and Ireland [10]. Surveys were administered online with quota samples stratified to represent the countries for age, sex and household income. One important research question that the C19PRC has addressed is the extent of the mental health impact. Shevlin et al. [5], using the first three Waves of the survey (March–July 2020), identified five classes of anxiety-depression trajectory: resilient, chronic, adaptive, deteriorating, and vulnerable. The patterns of anxiety-depression that emerged in the UK during the first several months of the pandemic had stabilised by the fifth Wave (W5) one-year follow-up [5]. Remarkably, just under 70% of the sample demonstrated resilience in terms of low anxiety, depression and stress symptoms at this time point.
Resilience has been conceptualised in a number of ways, including as a trait [11], as stability in wellbeing despite challenge [12], or as bouncing back from adverse life events [13]. We utilise Windle’s [14] large scale concept analysis, and employ her definition of resilience: “Resilience is the process of negotiating, managing and adapting to significant sources of stress or trauma. Assets and resources within the individual, their life and environment facilitate this capacity for adaptation and bouncing back in the face of adversity. Across the life course, the experience of resilience will vary” (p. 163). Resilience differs from notions of good mental health in its central requirement for challenge or trauma, in the case of this study, living through the COVID-19 pandemic [15]. Other contexts relevant to resilience include natural disasters [16], bereavement [17], dementia [18], and war [19]. Cosco et al. [20] reviews different measurement approaches, and in this study we operationalise resilience as the absence of both depressive and anxiety symptoms (below the criteria for clinical caseness) in the face of significant challenge, namely the COVID-19 pandemic.
A full understanding of the factors that led to this resilience will lead to recommendations for policy and practice to promote a more resilient response to future pandemics and similar events. One model which may help us to understand resilient responses is the Ecological Model of Resilience (EMR) [21]. This model posits that the presence or absence of individual, community and societal resources leads to a resilient outcome or alternatively to a negative effect on wellbeing (see Fig 1). Individual factors include demographic, psychological and health-related resources. Community variables include family, social support, and social participation. Societal variables include health and welfare support, social policies, and the economy. This model has been found to be a useful analytical tool for explaining resilience in the context of caring for people with dementia [22], widowhood [23], chronic health problems [24] and poverty amongst older adults living in Colombia [25].
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
In relation to the pandemic, the EMR has recently been applied amongst people living in four regions of Italy, two in the north of the country (Lombardia and Veneto) and two in the south (Campania and Lazio) [26]. We defined resilience as conjoint low levels of depressive symptoms as measured by the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 item version (PHQ-9) with a score below the cut-off of 10 (PHQ-9 <10) [27] and anxiety symptoms as measured by the Generalized Anxiety Disorder scale with 7 items (GAD-7) with a totale score below 10 (GAD-7 <10) [28]. The remaining participants were defined as Non-Resilient (see [20] for a review of methods to measure resilience). In addition to individual, community, and societal resources we added a COVID-19 specific component which included social distancing, COVID-related anxiety and trauma and exposure to COVID-19. Consistently with findings in the UK by Shevlin and colleagues [5] again about 70% of the sample were resilient, and 64% of the variance was explained by the components of the EMR. Psychological variables such as trait resilience and conscientiousness were associated with Resilient outcomes, whilst loneliness and intolerance of uncertainty were associated with being Non-Resilient. No community variables were found to be significant predictors of Resilient outcomes and region was the only societal variable which was significant, with living in the south associated with Non-resilient outcomes. Among the COVID-specific factors, social distancing was associated with resilience, and COVID-19 anxiety and COVID-19 related trauma were associated with Non-Resilient outcomes. With respect to demographic variables, having children in the household, and higher education level were associated with being Non-Resilient. The study had some limitations. As the Italian survey was not designed to measure resilience in the first instance, some variables, especially with respect to community and society, were not ideal, and others, such as health and welfare service use were not available. Moreover, as this study was cross-sectional, we were unable to determine whether the model was predictive over time [27].
In the current study, we utilise data from the larger C19PRC survey conducted in the UK, which had the opportunity to include additional variables, to further test the EMR’s usefulness for explaining resilient outcomes during the pandemic and to compare the findings with the Italian ones. First, we run a replication of the Italian analysis [26], using the same variables, and broadly the same time points in the pandemic (July/August 2020), which was survey Wave 3 in the UK study and equivalent to Wave 1 in the Italian study (July 2020). Both countries had emerged from national lockdowns. Second, we investigate the predictors of resilience one year into the pandemic in the UK (Wave 5; March/April 2021) using baseline data (Wave 1; March/April 2020), and where necessary variables from Wave 3 (July/August 2020) in which we included EMR-specific variables.
Our research questions (RQ) are as follows:
1. RQ#1: What proportion of the UK sample are resilient, at Wave 3 and Wave 5?
2. RQ#2: In which ways, if any, does the UK data replicate the Italian Study?
3. RQ#3: Does the ERM explain resilient outcomes longitudinally?
4. RQ#4: Which resources predict resilient outcomes and to what extent?
2. Materials and methods
This longitudinal study utilises longitudinal panel survey data collected by the C19PRC in the UK, launched in March 2020. In the first year of the pandemic, data was collected through 5 Waves (W1: N = 2025; W2: N = 1406; W3: N = 2019; W4: N = 1796; W5: N = 2520) examining the impact of the pandemic over time. Wave 1 was conducted between March and April 2020, Wave 3 between July and August 2020 and Wave 5 between March and April 2021. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Sheffield (reference number: 033759).
2.1. Participants
Participants were recruited online, via Qualtrics, with stratified quota sampling to achieve representative sample in terms of age and sex—using 2016 population estimates form Eurostat [29] and household income matched to Office of National Statistics data—for more details see [30]. Qualtrics alerted participants to the study, via Qualtrics. Details of the C19PRC methodology are available elsewhere [6, 31–33]. Participants were eligible to take part if they were at least 18 years old. The online survey took a median completion time of 20 minutes. All respondents were informed about the aims of the study and provided informed written consent.
A cross-sectional and a longitudinal study will be presented. The cross-sectional sample consisted of all respondents to Wave 3 (N = 2019, 51% females, mean age = 45.39, SD = 15.95). The longitudinal study included only those participants who fully completed 1st, 3rd, and 5th Waves (N = 847, 45.04% females, mean age = 51.46, SD = 14.47), thus a subset of the participants of W3. Due to the attrition in longitudinal studies, the longitudinal analysis has a less nationally representative sample, as it underrepresented younger people.
2.2. Measures
Below we describe the measures tested in the model; the full list of variables and more details about the survey are available from [6]. For Wave 5 please see McBride et al. [33].
2.2.1 Defining resilience, the dependent variable.
Resilience outcome was defined as a dichotomous variable (1 = Resilient, 0 = Non-Resilient) resulting from the conjoint levels of depressive and anxious symptoms. Resilient individuals had low levels of depressive symptoms (PHQ-9 <10) [27] and low levels of anxiety symptoms (GAD-7 <10) [28]. All other participants were defined as Non-Resilient (PHQ ≥10 or GAD ≥10) [26]. This procedure was followed both for the replication of the Italian study and the longitudinal model.
Depression symptoms were measured with the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) [27], its 9 items reflect the diagnostic criteria for the major depressive disorder of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.; DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association) [34]. Participants indicated how often they had been bothered by each symptom over the past 2 weeks using a four-point Likert type scale from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day). Total scores range from 0 to 27, with scores of 5–9 = mild, 10–14 = moderate–used for clinical caseness, and ≥ 15 = severe depression. Clinical caseness is defined as ≥ 10. The PHQ-9 psychometric properties are strong [35, 36] and in this study the internal reliability of the scale was excellent (α = .93).
Anxiety symptoms were assessed with the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale (GAD-7) [28], a 7-item questionnaire measure of the frequency of anxiety symptoms (e.g., trouble relaxing, becoming easily annoyed or irritable). Participants indicated how often they had been affected by each symptom over the past 2 weeks using a four-point Likert scale from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day). Scores range from 0 to 21 with a cut-off of 10 identifying the generalised anxiety disorder with good sensitivity (.89) and specificity (.82). Clinical caseness is defined as ≥ 10. The GAD-7 showed good psychometric properties [28, 35, 36], and the internal reliability of the scale was excellent (α = .95) in this study. Note that whilst we use the clinical cut-offs for both PHQ- 9 and GAD-7, we are not using them for the purpose of clinical diagnosis.
2.2.2 Independent variables.
We next describe the independent variables as they appear in the levels of the model in the replication of the Italian study: individual, community, societal and COVID-specific. For the replication, these variables were taken from the C19PRC Wave 3 data, to correspond with the point in time that the Italian data was collected (July 2020). In the longitudinal analyses, they were taken from Wave 1, unless stated otherwise. Variables that appear only in the longitudinal analysis are then described, and the Wave in which they were collected. Binary variables were coded as 1 = present/upper level and 0 = absent/lower level–unless specified (i.e., gender). Scale reliability was evaluated through Cronbach’s alpha (α).
2.2.2.1 Individual resources. The demographic variables included in the model were: gender (women = 1, men = 0; age (continuous variable); education (split as 0 = until high school and 1 = over high school); living alone or not (yes = 1, no = 0); caring for children in the home or not (yes = 1, no = 0); home ownership (yes = 1, no = 0); household income above £25,341/year or not (yes = 1, no = 0); employed or not (yes = 1, no = 0).
In addition, we included precarious health. The respondents were asked: “Were you diagnosed with a health condition (e.g., heart or lung disease; diabetes; cancer) before December 31st, 2019 (i.e. before the start of the coronavirus COVID-19 outbreak)?”. Poor health was coded as 1 (present) or 0 (absent).
The following psychological variables were also included:
Death anxiety (DA) was measured using the Death Anxiety Inventory (DAI) [37], which has 17 items across 4 dimensions. DAI Acceptance (6 items, α = .87) concerns the acceptance of the individual emotional dimension about death and its meaning—an item example is “I find it really difficult to accept that I have to die”. DAI Externally Generated (4 items, α = .78) refers to situations or elements with an external reference to death in our cultural context (e.g., coffins, cemeteries) that can trigger unpleasant feelings—an item example is “I get upset when I am in a cemetery”. DAI Finality (4 items, α = .90) concerns a spiritual dimension of concerns about mortality and the limits of human existence—an item example is “The idea that there is nothing after death frightens me”. DAI Thoughts (3 items, α = .82) is related to the frequent thoughts and concerns about own death—an item example is “I frequently think of my own death”. The response format is a 5-point Likert type scale from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree).
Personality traits were assessed with the Big Five Inventory (BFI) [38], a 10-item measure with 5 scales, each for a personality trait: Extraversion (e.g., “I see myself as someone who is outgoing, sociable”), Agreeableness (e.g., “I see myself as someone who is generally trusting”), Consciousness (e.g., “I see myself as someone who does a thorough job”), Neuroticism (e.g., “I see myself as someone who gets nervous easily”), Openness to Experience (e.g., “I see myself as someone who has an active imagination”). Items are rated on a 5-step scale from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly), each scale ranges from 2 to 10. The BFI-10 has good reliability and validity [38].
Loneliness was measured with the Loneliness Scale (LS) [39], a 3-item tool assessing the frequency of social (dis)connectedness (e.g., lacking companionship, isolation from others) in large-scale population surveys. An item example is “How often do you feel isolated from others?”. The response format is a 3-point scale from 1 (hardly ever) to 3 (often) with a minimum of 3 and a maximum of 9. In this study, α was .89.
Self-esteem was assessed with the Single-Item Self-esteem Scale (SISES) [40], respondents rated their agreement with one statement (“I have high self-esteem”) using a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (not very true of me) to 7 (very true of me).
Trait resilience was measured with the Brief Resilience Scale (BRS) [41] with 6 items (e.g., “I tend to bounce back quickly after hard times”) scored on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 “strongly disagree” to 5 “strongly agree”, total scores range from 5 to 30. The BRS has good psychometric properties [41]. In this study, α was .88.
2.2.3 Community variables.
Social Support was measured in its Emotional and Instrumental components with the Modified Medical Outcome Social Support Survey (MOS-SS) [42], asking participants: “People sometimes look to others for companionship, assistance, or other types of support. How often are each of the following kinds of support available to YOU if you need it?”. Each component was assessed with 4 items scored on a 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 (None of the time) to 5 (All of the time). Higher scores are associated with higher perceived support. The reliability was excellent for the Emotional (α = .93). and Instrumental components (α = .95)
2.2.4 Societal resources.
Religious. Participants were asked whether they identified with any religion (Catholic, Jewish, Muslim, other = 1), or not (atheist, agnostic, none = 0). Whilst this variable could also be seen as an individual resource, it was included as a societal resource because of its broader cultural significance [25, 43].
2.2.5 COVID-19 specific variables.
We included several variables specifically related to COVID-19.
COVID-19 anxiety was measured with a single item (“How anxious are you about the coronavirus COVID-19 pandemic?”) on an electronic visual analogue scale to indicate the degree of anxiety from 0 “not at all anxious” on the left to 100 “extremely anxious” with 10-point increments. Higher scores reflected higher levels of COVID-19 related anxiety.
Post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms in relation to the COVID-19 experience (PTSD Symptoms) was measured with the International Trauma Questionnaire (ITQ) [44] referring to the last month with the following instructions: “You will be asked questions about different ways that people sometimes react following a traumatic or stressful life event. Please answer the following questions in relation to your experience of the COVID-19 pandemic. Please read each item carefully, then select one of the answers to indicate how much you have been bothered by that problem in the past month”. The ITQ has 6 items encompassing three clusters of symptoms of Re-experiencing, Avoidance, and Sense of Threat. A 5-point Likert scale from 0 (Not at all) to 4 (Extremely) generates scores ranging from 0 to 24. A was .93 in this study.
Exposure to COVID-19 was defined as the experience of self, family or acquaintance being infected or tested for COVID-19 (whether the outcome was positive or negative: as there may be anticipatory anxiety), or a family member or acquaintance having died because of COVID-19, as well having been in self-isolation because of the (suspected) infection. Those exposed to COVID-19 were scored 1 and those not exposed were scored 0.
The following variables were included in the longitudinal analyses only.
Intolerance of Uncertainty (Wave 1) was assessed with the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale Short Form (IUS-12) [45], with 12 items scored on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 “not at all characteristics of me” to 5 “entirely characteristic of me”. It includes both prospective cognitions “Unexpected events are negative and should be avoided”) and inhibitory cognitions “Uncertainty leads to the inability to act”) about uncertainty that are well described in a single dimension [45]. The total score ranges from 12 to 60. The IUS-12 has good psychometric properties [45], and α was .90 in this study.
Trust in People (Wave 1) was assessed with the following ad-hoc question “Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted or that you need to be very careful in dealing with people?” with a response format scored on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (= “Mostly people can be trusted”) to 5 (= “Need to be very careful”). Scores were dichotomized according to the median that was 3, resulting with 1110 persons saying that they did trust people and 778 who did not.
Neighbourhood Connectedness (Wave 1) was assessed with three measures. Belongingness to the neighbourhood was measured with a single question “How strongly do you feel you belong to your immediate neighbourhood?” scored on a 4-point response format from 1 “not at all” to 4 “very strongly”. There were two specific questions on trust in neighbours asking about willingness to leave house keys to them, and whether they could be asked to buy groceries in case of need, scored on a 4-point scale from 1 (= “very uncomfortable”) to 4 (= “very comfortable”). Total scores range from 2 to 8, with higher scores associated with higher trust and connectedness.
Belongingness to Wider Neighbourhood (Wave 1) was assessed with 2 questions “How much do you identify with (feel a part of, feel love toward, have concern for) your community?” and “How much would you say you feel involved when bad things happen to your community?”–scored on a 5-point format from 1 (= “not at all”) to 5 (= “a lot”). Scores were dichotomized according to the median (= 4).
Hygiene Behaviours (Wave 1) were measured with 17 items based on the Capability, Opportunity, Motivation-Behaviour, version 1 (COM-B) [46, 47]. Participants were asked “Please answer the following questions to indicate the extent to which the following statements are true for you with respect to maintaining hygienic practices (e.g., hand washing frequently, cleansing surfaces) during the COVID-19 pandemic”. Answers were scored on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “strongly agree” to 5 “strongly disagree”. In this study, α was .84.
Social Distance Behaviours (Wave 1) were assessed with 17 statements according to the COM-B [46, 47]. Participants were asked “Please answer the following questions to indicate the extent to which the following statements are true for you with respect to social distancing (e.g., avoiding crowds, maintaining personal distance, avoiding non-essential meetings, less socialising in public) during the COVID-19 pandemic”. Answers were scored on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 “strongly agree” to 5 “strongly disagree”. A was .86 in this study.
Service Use was assessed at Wave 5 by asking participants to “…answer the following questions in relation to your attendance at health appointments and use of healthcare services since the beginning of the pandemic. Since the pandemic, have you attended…” any of the 13 following health services: dentist; sight test; hearing test; other health check-ups; physiotherapist; occupational therapist; chiropodist; social worker; speech therapist; day centre; outpatient services; inpatient services; and General Practitioner appointments. Each service was assessed by one dedicated item. Items were scored as ‘Yes, I had a consultation in person’ (= 1), ‘Yes, I had a consultation online or by telephone’ (= 2), ‘No, the pandemic prevented me from doing so’ (= 3), ‘No, I didn’t need this service during the pandemic’ (= 4). Since we were interested in access to services during the pandemic, the response options were recoded as follows. Those who were prevented from using at least one service because of the pandemic were scored 0, and those who used the services or did not need them were scored as 1.
Social Engagement (Wave 5) was measured by asking participants “The following items are about your social contact with family and friends. Please read each item and indicate which option best applies to you”. The 5 dichotomous items (yes = 1, no = 0) concerned the social interactions of the participants through social media, personal calls, letters/emails, meetings and friends living in the surroundings. Summing the answers, the score ranged from 0 to 5 with higher scores indicating higher social engagement. The scale was based on the Brief Assessment of Social Engagement Scale (BASE) [48] which measured both actual and symbolic social engagement, updated to the current time. α was .47.
Self-Rated Health (Wave 5) was measured through one question–- “Compared to someone your own age, would you say your health has on the whole been…” and asking participants to move a slider on a scale from poor (= 1) to excellent (= 5), with higher scores indicating higher levels of perceived self-rated health.
2.3. Statistical analysis
Descriptive analyses were used to describe the sample and the psychosocial characteristics of participants. The analytical plan was pre-registered on Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/8dn27). Within the family of generalized linear models, a binary logistic univariate multiple regression was fitted to predict the Resilient (= 1) or Non-Resilient (= 0) outcome of participants relying on multiple predictor variables that were progressively entered in blocks according to the EMR model. Regressions allowed to estimate the effect of each predictor variable on the dependent variable once the effect of the other variables has been considered; in this model the interactions among variables were not considered.
Model fit [49] was evaluated through the Akaike Information Criteria (AIC), Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC), Pseudo R² of Cragg-Uhler [50], Odds ratio (OR) and the 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated and reported.
The first set of regression models is based on cross-sectional data of Wave 3 from the UK study (N = 2019) (6) and focuses on predicting state resilience at W3 (resilient = 1 or not resilient = 0) with multiple independent variables progressively entered in blocks according to the EMR–thus replicating and going beyond the previous Italian study [26].
The second set of regression models is based on longitudinal data from the UK study. It aims to predict resilience at Wave 5 (resilient = 1 or not resilient = 0) [33] still relying on multiple predictors variables that were measured over time in the same individuals at Wave 1, Wave 3, and Wave 5 (N = 847). As in the previous model, the predictors were entered in blocks in line with the EMR [21].
3. Results
3.1. Cross-sectional replication of the Italian study
3.1.1 Characteristics of sample.
Table 1 shows the characteristics of the sample at Wave 3 [31] including a total of 2019 respondents with a mean age of 45.39 years (SD = 15.95) and a representative gender-balance, 51% were women. Most of participants had qualifications equal to or above high school (e.g., degree) (61%), whilst the remaining 49% attained a lower level of education (O-Level/GCSE or similar). Only 21% of the sample lived alone.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
3.1.2 Replication of the Italian study: Regression.
The cross-sectional model of binary logistic regression had as dependent variable state Resilience at Wave 3 (1 = Resilient, 0 = Non-Resilient) and all the predictor variables were measured at Wave 3. Predictors were consecutively entered in blocks according to the EMR [21]. Table 2 shows the results of the cross-sectional regression analysis that was performed on 848 complete cases who completed 1st, 3rd, 5th Waves.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Block 1 included the individual demographic variables, and the model showed a good fit to the data (χ²(9) = 105.625, p < .001; AIC = 1032.219, BIC = 1079.648), explaining 16% of variance of the dependent variable (Pseudo-R² = 0.160). The predictors significantly and positively associated with resilience were higher age (OR = 1.05, CI = 1.03–1.06, p < .001) and income above £25,341/y (OR = 1.55, CI = 1.11–2.16, p < .010) whilst precarious health had a significant negative association with Resilient outcomes (OR = 0.38, CI = 0.25–0.58, p < .001).
Block 2 added the individual psychological variables. The model had good fit (χ²(21) = 434.996, p < .001; AIC = 726.848, BIC = 831.191) and explained 55% of variance (Pseudo-R² = 0.548), adding an incremental .532 R². Among the demographic variables, age (OR = 1.03, CI = 1.01–1.05, p < .001) and precarious health (OR = 0.46, CI = 0.27–0.79, p = .005) were still significantly associated with Resilient outcomes, respectively with a positive and a negative direction. Among the psychological variables, both DAI-Finality (OR = 1.11, CI = 1.02–1.21, p = .021) and Self-Esteem (OR = 1.19, CI = 1.03–1.38, p = .018) positively associated with Resilient outcomes, whilst the variables with a significant negative association with Resilient outcomes were DAI Acceptance (OR = 0.92, CI = 0.86–0.99, p = .030), DAI Thoughts (OR = 0.79, CI = 0.72–0.86, p < .001), Extraversion (OR = 0.76, CI = 0.67–0.86, p < .001), Neuroticism (OR = 0.69, CI = 0.59–0.80, p < .001), and loneliness (OR = 0.63, CI = 0.56–0.71, p < .001).
Block 3 added to the model the community variables that are represented by both Instrumental Social Support and Emotional Social Support. With a good fit (χ²(23) = 431.873, p < .001; AIC = 676.588, BIC = 789.049) the model explained 57% of variance (Pseudo-R² = 0.568), but added only .02 of R². The Social Support variables were not significantly associated with Resilient outcomes and the other significant predictors were the same as in the previous block.
In Block 4 we added the only societal variable, namely being religious or not. The model provided good fit (χ²(24) = 434.144, p < .001; AIC = 676.317, BIC = 793.463) and still explained the 57% of variance (Pseudo-R² = 0.570) but with an incremental R² of .002. Indeed, being religious or not was not associated with Resilience.
Lastly, in Block 5 the COVID19-related variables were added, the model showed a good fit (χ²(27) = 540.469, p < .001; AIC = 575.992, BIC = 707.196) and explained the 70% of variance (Pseudo-R² = 0.669) with an incremental R² of .099 compared to the previous block. Age was the only significant predictor among the demographics (OR = 1.02, CI = 1.00–1.04, p = .038). Among the psychological factors, only Self-Esteem (OR = 1.34, CI = 1.13–1.60, p = .001) was significantly positively associated with resilience whilst the significant predictors negatively associated with resilience were Death Anxiety Thoughts (0.84, CI = 0.75–0.94, p = .002), Extraversion (OR = 0.82, CI = 0.71–0.94, p = .005), Neuroticism (OR = 0.67, CI = 0.56–0.79, p < .001), and Loneliness (OR = 0.69, CI = 0.60–0.79, p < .001). Concerning the COVID-19-related variables, the severity of PTSD Symptoms related to COVID-19 was the only significant predictor negatively associated with Resilience (OR = 0.79, CI = 0.75–0.83, p < .001).
Compared to the Italian cross-sectional model [26], the UK model shows better fit in explaining the data according to the ERM (UK model: χ²(27) = 540.469, p = < .001; AIC = 575.992, BIC = 707.196, Pseudo-R² = 0.669; IT model: Nagelkerke R² = 0.642; AIC = 707.3; BIC = 885.2; Residual deviance: 635.28, df = 998).
Fig 2 shows the odds ratio plot of all the factors in the cross-sectional UK model at Wave 3.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 3 shows the comparison of the odds ratio plot of all factors between Italy and UK.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 4 shows the odds ratio plot only of the factors significantly associated with resilience in the cross-sectional UK model (W3).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
3.2. Longitudinal model
3.2.1 Characteristics of sample.
The longitudinal model was fitted on a sample of 847 complete cases who completed the surveys at W1, W3, and W5. This sample had a mean age of 51.46 (SD = 14.47) and was balanced in terms of gender (women = 381, 45.04%).
Considering resilience at Wave 5, 80% of the sample showed Resilient outcomes defined as the absence of severe anxiety and depression. The proportions of UK participants who were resilient showed a statistically significant increase from Wave 3 (1446 of 2019, 72%) to Wave 5 (677 of 847, 80%) as found by the one-sided (less) two-sample test for equality of proportions with continuity correction (X2 = 21.024, df = 1, p < .001, 95% CI = -1–0.054). Table 3 reports the complete descriptive statistics of the variables included in the longitudinal UK model.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
3.2.2 Longitudinal model: Regression.
The longitudinal model included 847 participants. The variables were added in subsequent blocks according to the EMR [21]. Table 4 shows the results of the longitudinal regression model.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Block 1 included the individual demographic variables. The model showed good fit indexes (χ²(9) = 44.057, p < .001; AIC = 824.838, BIC = 872.243) and explained the 8% of variance (Pseudo-R² = 0.080). The variables significantly associated with Resilient outcomes were male gender (OR = 1.30, p = .046), higher age (OR = 1.03, CI = 1.02–1.04, p < .001), income above £25,341/Y (OR = 1.54, CI = 1.04, 2.29, p = .032), and being employed (OR = 2.75, CI = 1.12–6.69, p = .025). Conversely, precarious health was significantly associated with a Non-Resilient outcome (OR = 0.62, CI = 0.42, 0.94, p = .023).
Block 2 added the individual psychological variables, showing good fit (χ²(23) = 246.007, p < .001; AIC = 639.001, BIC = 752.545) and explained the 40% of variance (Pseudo-R² = 0.403), with an R² increment of 32% compared to the previous block. Regarding the significant predictors, none of the first block remained significant whilst among the psychological variables self-rated health (W5) (OR = 1.87, CI = 1.47, 2.39, p < .001) was significantly associated with Resilient outcomes (OR = 1.87, p < .001) and the predictors associated with Non-Resilient outcomes were DAI Thoughts (W1) (OR = 0.91, CI = 0.83–1.00, p = .043), Neuroticism (W1) (OR = 0.85, CI = 0.73–0.99, p = .037), and loneliness (W1) (OR = 0.76, CI = 0.67–0.86, p < .001).
In Block 3 the community variables were added, the model had good fit (χ²(30) = 242.950, p < .001; AIC = 614.557, BIC = 759.819) and explained the 42% of variance (Pseudo-R² = 0.416), adding less than 2% compared to the previous block. There were no significant demographic predictors of resilient outcomes at W5. Among the psychological variables, self-rated health (W5) (OR = 1.75, CI = 1.36–2.25, p < .001) was still significantly associated with Resilient outcomes at W5 as well as Neuroticism (OR = 0.82, CI = 0.70–0.96, p = .016) and loneliness (OR = 0.75, CI = 0.66–0.85, p < .001) that were still negatively associated with Resilient outcomes. The DAI Thoughts was no longer significant (OR = 0.92, CI = 0.84, 1.01, p = .097). Among the newly added community variables, both belongingness to neighbourhood (W1) (OR = 1.67, CI = 1.03–2.72, p = .038) and Instrumental Social Support (W3) (OR = 1.08, CI = 1.01–1.16, p = .036) were significantly associated with Resilient outcomes.
Block 4 added the predictors about society, namely being religious or not (W1) with good fit (χ²(31) = 245.552, p < .001; AIC = 613.955, BIC = 763.902), but the explained variance was almost the same as in the previous model and the delta increase in the Pseudo-R² was negligible (Pseudo-R² = 0.419; Δ Pseudo-R² = +0.003). Indeed, religion was not significantly associated with Resilient outcomes (OR = 0.68, CI = 0.42–1.09, p = .110). The other significant predictors were the same as in the previous model: self-rated health (OR = 1.77, CI = 1.38–2.28, p < .001); Neuroticism (OR = 0.82, CI = 0.70–0.97, p = .018); loneliness (OR = 0.75, CI = 0.66, 0.86, p < .001); neighbourhood belongingness (OR = 1.69, CI = 1.04–2.76, p = .035); Instrumental Social Support (W3) (OR = 1.08, CI = 1.00–1.16, p = .044).
Lastly, Block 5 added the COVID-19-related variables and showed a good fit (χ²(37) = 283.802, p < .001; AIC = 587.706, BIC = 765.768) with an explained variance equal to 47% (Pseudo-R² = 0.474) and an increment of .055 in R² compared to the previous model. There were no significant demographic predictors. Among the psychological variables, the predictors significantly associated with Resilient outcomes were self-rated health (OR = 1.89, CI = 1.44–2.50, p < .001), DAI Externally Generated (OR = 1.09, CI = 1.00–1.19, p = .047), Openness (OR = 1.17, CI = 1.02–1.34, p = .028), Loneliness (OR = 0.79, CI = 0.68–0.90, p < .001), and self-esteem (OR = 1.30, CI = 1.08–1.58, p = .007). Among the community block, neighbourhood belongingness (OR = 1.96, CI = 1.18–3.30; p = .010) was significantly and positively associated with Resilient outcomes. In the newly added COVID-block, PTSD symptoms (OR = 0.89, CI = 0.84–0.94, p < .001) were significantly and negatively associated with resilient outcomes whilst service use was positively associated with state Resilient outcomes at Wave 5 (OR = 2.34, CI = 1.48–3.74, p < .001). Fig 5 shows the plot of the odds ratio of the longitudinal model in the UK.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
3.2.2.1 Resources predicting resilience or non-resilience. Regarding the fourth research question, the resources that significantly predicted Resilient outcomes at W5 in the longitudinal model were, in order of strength: service use (access to health services was associated with Resilient outcomes) (OR = 2.34, CI = 1.47–3.37, p < .001), belongingness to wider neighbourhood (greater sense of belonging was associated with Resilient outcomes) (OR = 1.96, CI = 1.18–3.30; p = .010), self-rated health (more positive self-rated health was associated with Resilient outcomes) (OR = 1.89, CI = 1.44–2.50, p < .001), self-esteem (higher self-esteem was associated with Resilient outcomes) (OR = 1.30, CI = 1.08–1.58, p = .007), Openness (greater openness was associated with Resilient outcomes) (OR = 1.17, CI = 1.02–1.34, p = .028), DAI Externally Generated (higher levels of Externally Generated DAI was associated with Resilient outcomes)(OR = 1.09, CI = 1.00–1.19, p = .047).
Fig 6 shows a plot of the odds ratio of the resources (significant predictors) associated with Resilient outcomes in the longitudinal model. The predictors are ordered according to their strength in positively predicting resilience at W5, those in red are negatively associated with resilience–PTSD symptoms (OR = 0.89, CI = 0.84–0.94, p < .001) and loneliness (OR = 0.79, CI = 0.68–0.90, p < .001).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
4. Discussion
This study aimed to further examine the effectiveness of the Ecological Model of Resilience (EMR) in explaining resilient outcomes during the COVID-19 pandemic in the UK. First, a cross-sectional model replicated the Italian study at Wave 3 in the UK. Second, going beyond the Italian cross-sectional study, a longitudinal regression model including additional EMR specific variables explored the predictors of resilience at Wave 5 using Wave 1, and with variables from Waves 3 and 5.
With respect to the first research question, at Wave 3 in the UK, 72% of participants were resilient, a finding in line with the Italian data, whilst at Wave 5, 80% were classified resilient. The Wave 3 findings were in line with the proportion of Italian participants who were resilient. Interestingly, the proportions of UK participants who were resilient increased between Wave 3 and Wave 5. In both cases most participants were just emerging from lockdowns. The increase in resilience suggests that there was some habituation to the effects of the pandemic. It is also possible that participants had developed successful coping strategies not measured in this study. It is also important to note that although the Wave 3 sample (N = 2019) was nationally representative this was not the case for the longitudinal study sample (N = 848) which included only those who had previously completed W1 and W3. As attrition naturally occurs in longitudinal studies, the longitudinal sample was not nationally representative [33, 51] and underrepresented younger people who have been more impacted by COVID-19 than older people [52, 53].
The cross-sectional model in the UK
The results of the cross-sectional model in the UK (W3) successfully explained 67% of variance at W3. Self-esteem and age were the only significant predictors positively associated with Resilient outcomes (W3), whilst the other significant predictors were negatively associated with resilience and included DAI Thoughts, Extraversion, PTSD symptoms, loneliness and Neuroticism. The UK cross sectional study partially replicated the Italian results. Predictors in both studies were broadly in the same direction, that is either positively or negatively associated with resilience. Non-resilient participants in both countries showed higher levels of loneliness, higher levels of COVID-related PTSD symptoms [54], higher DAI Thoughts [55] and higher Neuroticism [56] than Resilient participants–these represent cross-cultural risk factors, in line with existing literature.
However, there were variables which were significant in one study and not in the other. Notably, self-esteem, age (associated with Resilience) and Extraversion (associated with Non-Resilience) in the UK, and in Italy caring for children, precarious health and Intolerance of Uncertainty were associated with Non-Resilient outcomes, also Conscientiousness and social distancing were associated with Resilient outcomes. However, the age effect needs to be treated cautiously as the confidence intervals are near the non-effect boundary and is similar to the Italian estimate. Intolerance of Uncertainty was present only in the Italian study and not in the cross-sectional UK data at W3. There is some evidence that the UK has an uncertainty approaching culture whilst Italy has uncertainty avoidant one—for some considerations see [57, 58]. It is unclear why there are other different patterns regarding psychological factors for the two countries, although there may be cultural and contextual factors which may be influential but which we have not measured. This may also be the case for caring for children where there are likely to be both cultural and both general and COVID-19-specific contextual factors. In recent decades in Italy, family units have become progressively smaller, without relatives able to care for children [59, 60]. In addition, children in recent years are less likely to play unsupervised in the neighbourhood. These factors were compounded during the pandemic. In both countries, children could no longer play out, grandparents were isolating. In addition, children needed to be home schooled, and parents often juggled working from home and home schooling. In the UK children of key workers were able to attend school. In Italy parents often had to take unpaid leave to care for the children.
Comparing the UK and Italy, most predictors of resilience were consistent in the direction of their effect. However, none were statistically significant in both countries. This may suggest a concordant role for those predictors despite not reaching statistical significance. In the UK higher age was significant in positively predicting Resilient outcomes but on in Italy. In Italy, but not in the UK, DAI Finality and Conscientiousness were significantly positively associated with Resilient outcomes. On the other hand, caring for children was significantly negatively associated with Resilient outcomes in Italy but not in the UK. There were only two significant predictors that were not directionally consistent between the two countries. Extraversion was significantly negatively associated with Resilient outcomes in the UK and self-esteem was positively associated with Resilient outcomes also in the UK but in Italy the results were non-significant. It is possible that the social restrictions imposed during the lockdown might have a greater impact on those with higher extraversion, than those with lower. However, it is not clear why that should be the case in the UK but not in Italy. It is noteworthy, that when comparing the UK and Italian cross-sectional EMRs, there were no predictors that were both statistically significant and in divergent directions, indicating a generally consistent pattern.
The longitudinal model in the UK
Regarding the longitudinal model in the UK (RQ3), the results explained 47% of the variance of Resilient outcomes at W5 (compared to 67% of UK-W3). The predictors significantly associated with resilience in the UK longitudinal model were in the majority of resources with a positive association with Resilient outcomes. These protective factors included service use, belongingness to neighbourhood, self-rated health, self-esteem, Openness, and Death Anxiety Externally Generated. The two statistically significant predictors negatively associated with Resilient outcomes were PTSD symptoms and loneliness.
The positive association between service use and Resilient outcomes emphasises the importance of a well-functioning health system in times of health emergencies. Those that had access to, or did not need health services, were twice as likely to be Resilient than those who needed a service but could not access it. Thus, needing a service but not being able to access presents serious difficulties for people with mental/physical health issues. Our finding underlies the importance–at a societal level–of providing and guaranteeing essential health services during health-related emergencies, regardless of the pressure to restrict routine medical checks.
Belongingness to neighbourhood also promotes Resilience, providing people with a sense of connection to their communities which can be drawn on in times of need. Those with higher belongingness were also almost twice as likely to be Resilient. This is consistent with a wide range of evidence that identification with groups is facilitates mental health and protects against threats to wellbeing to the extent to which this process has been described as a ‘social cure’ [61]. Individuals reporting higher belongingness to neighbourhood may also perceive higher social support (both instrumental and emotional), an important resource for psychological health [62].
Among the psychological variables, the strongest predictor of Resilience was self-rated health. Self-rated health which, in previous research, has been found to be an important predictor of mortality [63]. Indeed, individuals with overall better subjective health draw on stronger perceived foundations (e.g., strength, wellness, physical functioning) in facing difficulties (both practical and psychological). Individuals with higher self-rated health are also less worried about physical issues that are known to negatively affect psychological health and quality of life, especially during health-related emergencies [64, 65]. In addition, this result is in line with research which finds that self-rated health reflects individual psychological health [26].
Self-esteem also promoted Resilient outcomes. Having a positive idea, representation, and evaluation of oneself may be an advantage for several reasons [66], since it allows the experience of more confidence, acceptance, and unconditional appreciation toward self. Positive self-evaluation is often associated with a positive view of the world, life and the future, allowing being more optimistic and feeling in control of one’s own life. As a result, people with higher self-esteem are less prone to a negative cognitive style–(i.e., negative events interpretations)–and less prone to experience negative affectivity [67]. Research on Terror Management Theory (TMT) [68], which focuses on how individuals cope with knowledge of mortality, supports this finding. According to TMT, in threatening contexts such as the COVID-19 pandemic [69], self-esteem acts as a buffer, shielding individuals from anxiety and depression [70].
Among the personality traits, Openness to Experience was a protective factor promoting Resilience, Literature shows the positive associations among Openness to Experience, positive emotions, quality of life, and well-being [71]. Conversely, Openness to Experience is negatively related to negative affect [72]. This variable may be associated with psychological flexibility which is associated with good mental health [64, 73, 74]. Further, a lack of psychological flexibility is a well-known risk-factor for psychological difficulties, intolerance of uncertainty and the need for closure [75–79].
Lastly, the Externally Generated Death Anxiety–consisting in being upset when facing external stimuli related to death (e.g., coffins, cemeteries)–showed a significant positive association with Resilient outcomes. Fearing death-related stimuli can be considered as a cognitive avoidance process [75]. According to the cognitive avoidance theory [80], the visual exposure (real or imaginary) to triggering stimuli generates a strong physiological activation, favouring emotional processing, thus ultimately promoting lower levels of depression and anxiety over time (i.e., through the pandemic). Conversely, cognitive avoidance/suppression uses verbalization as a strategy for abstraction, disengagement, and emotion control, to lower the physiological activation to aversive material. In a nutshell, abstract verbal thoughts are less emotionally activating than visual images (e.g., corpses, blood) so that, in the short-term, abstract thought prevents emotional elaboration but in the mid-long term can lead to emotional disturbance [81]. Similarly, in a first phase of the pandemic, high levels of externally generated anxiety when visualizing of death-related stimuli reflected the heightened psycho-physiological activation that promoted emotional processing [82] and favoured resilience in the long term.
Regarding the risk factors hindering Resilient outcomes, loneliness was the strongest one followed by PTSD symptoms. The COVID-19 pandemic exacerbated feelings of loneliness feelings through the forced isolation and physical distancing that weakened social connections [83, 84]. Loneliness is a well-known risk factor for mental ill-health, since it can trigger distressing thoughts focusing on comparisons between the actual and the desired socio-relational situation, leading to strong symptoms of anxiety that–in turn–can trigger depressive symptoms [85–87]. Moreover, this finding is also congruent and complementary to the above-discussed association of belongingness to neighbourhood with Resilience.
Lastly, PTSD symptoms related to COVID-19 were a risk-factor for Non-Resilient outcomes, suggesting how the COVID-19 has been perceived as a traumatic event concretely threatening the brief and long-term health of self and loved ones. PTSD symptoms are characterised by three areas consisting in frequent negative emotions, heightened arousal and re-experiencing of the traumatic events—all together these characteristics lead to higher anxious and depressive symptoms resulting in Non-Resilient outcomes.
It was notable that Openness to Experience, loneliness, self-esteem and PTSD symptoms–all measured at W1 predicted Resilient or Non-Resilient outcomes at W5, suggesting that interventions for prevention and treatment of psychological issues should consider these indicators. Interestingly, Openness to Experience was not a significant predictor at W3 but it was from W1 to W5, possibly suggesting that the abilities to cope with the situation and elaborate it may require a certain time to be effective [88, 89].
Among the significant predictors from W1 to W5, the majority were dispositional-like traits—as self-esteem and Openness to Experience. Openness to Experience is clear is a personality trait and therefore likely stable across time but this is less certain for self-esteem. However, the available research literature suggests that self-evaluation tends to be stable over time to the extent to which it is grounded in early personal relationships and then reflected stable behavioural patterns [90]. It is not surprising that trait-like variables are associated with state resilience over time, since their stable nature allows scores to be more consistent over time when compared to state-like characteristics that are more prone to change. Although trait-like factors have traditionally been seen as more difficult to modify, recent third Wave psychotherapy approaches are having some success [91, 92].
It is interesting to consider how the positive associations with Resilient Outcomes might be utilised to mitigate against the Non-Resilient Outcomes. For example, interventions which promote self-esteem and increase Belongingness to Neighbourhood might reduce loneliness [93]. Similarly, access to services is an important resource for people with PTSD symptoms, and with COVID-related PTSD symptoms. Policy makers should consider ways of keeping services open during times of pandemic. Increasing Belongingness to Neighbourhood might also enable people with PTSD to engage with support from local communities.
Although the focus of this paper is on Resilient outcomes, it is important to note that 20% of the sample were not resilient. These participants had reached either, or indeed both, the threshold for clinical caseness for either anxiety or depression. Much of the focus of research during the pandemic has focused on these groups (e.g. 3). Shevlin et al.’s [94] one-year follow-up analysis using the C19PRC dataset found that approximately 5% of the sample had deteriorating mental health associated with the pandemic. Future research which focuses on facilitating resilience amongst this population in the face of significant societal challenge would be particularly useful.
This research is not without limitations. Even though the W1 and W3 samples were balanced and stratified to reflect the population characteristics, the internet-administered survey may have reached a population that is friendly with technology use. Further, the sample is less representative in the longitudinal analyses due to attrition over time. For a discussion of the limitations of the C19PRC study see for Wave 1 and 2 [6], Wave 3 [31], and Wave 5 [33]. When interpreting the results, we should be aware that there may be other variables which we have not measured which may impact on resilience outcomes. There may also be high-order interactions which should be explored in future research.
Despite these limitations, this research has considerable strengths. As part of an international C19PRC project, the structure of the panel study allows several variables to be measured across different areas (psychological, community, societal, COVID-related), at different points of time, and across different countries. The replication of the Italian resilience study [26] allowed cross-cultural comparison of the ERM. In addition, the flexible nature of the data collection in the UK enabled us to refine the data collection to include variables relevant to the model such as service use, social support and social engagement [21, 95]. This research also provides validation of results in two distinct cohorts.
Future research will test the validity of the EMR in other countries to extend the impact of this research. Further studies may also enable us to understand further cross-cultural differences in resilience. Lastly, studying the trajectories of resilience over time is a complex topic requiring further attention and comprehension which will lead to more effective interventions.
In conclusion, the EMR provided a useful framework for explaining and understanding resilience and the role of vulnerability factors and resources in the COVID-19 pandemic in the UK. Further, the model identifies individual, community and societal resources which are open to modification to enhance resilience.
Citation: Bennett KM, Panzeri A, Derrer-Merk E, Butter S, Hartman TK, Mason L, et al. (2023) Predicting resilience during the COVID-19 Pandemic in the United Kingdom: Cross-sectional and longitudinal results. PLoS ONE 18(5): e0283254. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283254
About the Authors:
Kate M. Bennett
Roles: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3164-6894
Anna Panzeri
Roles: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5999-858X
Elfriede Derrer-Merk
Roles: Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom
Sarah Butter
Roles: Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: School of Psychology, Ulster University, Coleraine, United Kingdom
Todd K. Hartman
Roles: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Social Statistics, University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom
Liam Mason
Roles: Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London, United Kingdom
Orla McBride
Roles: Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: School of Psychology, Ulster University, Coleraine, United Kingdom
Jamie Murphy
Roles: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: School of Psychology, Ulster University, Coleraine, United Kingdom
Mark Shevlin
Roles: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: School of Psychology, Ulster University, Coleraine, United Kingdom
Jilly Gibson-Miller
Roles: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
Liat Levita
Roles: Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom
Anton P. Martinez
Roles: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
Ryan McKay
Roles: Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, London, United Kingdom
Alex Lloyd
Roles: Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London, United Kingdom
Thomas V. A. Stocks
Roles: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
Gioa Bottesi
Roles: Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8411-8887
Giulo Vidotto
Roles: Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
Richard P. Bentall
Roles: Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
Marco Bertamini
Roles: Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8617-6864
1. Shevlin M, McBride O, Murphy J, Miller JG, Hartman TK, Levita L, et al. Anxiety, depression, traumatic stress and COVID-19-related anxiety in the UK general population during the COVID-19 pandemic. BJPsych Open. 19 ottobre 2020;6(6):e125. pmid:33070797
2. Xiong J, Lipsitz O, Nasri F, Lui LMW, Gill H, Phan L, et al. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in the general population: A systematic review. J Affect Disord. 1 dicembre 2020;277:55–64. pmid:32799105
3. Fancourt D, Steptoe A, Bu F. Trajectories of anxiety and depressive symptoms during enforced isolation due to COVID-19 in England: a longitudinal observational study. Lancet Psychiatry. 1 febbraio 2021;8(2):141–9. pmid:33308420
4. Pierce M, McManus S, Hope H, Hotopf M, Ford T, Hatch SL, et al. Mental health responses to the COVID-19 pandemic: a latent class trajectory analysis using longitudinal UK data. Lancet Psychiatry. 1 luglio 2021;8(7):610–9. pmid:33965057
5. Shevlin M, Butter S, McBride O, Murphy J, Gibson-Miller J, Hartman TK, et al. Refuting the myth of a ‘tsunami’ of mental ill-health in populations affected by COVID-19: evidence that response to the pandemic is heterogeneous, not homogeneous. Psychol Med. 2021;1.
6. McBride O, Murphy J, Shevlin M, Gibson-Miller J, Hartman TK, Hyland P, et al. Monitoring the psychological, social, and economic impact of the COVID-19 pandemic in the population: Context, design and conduct of the longitudinal COVID-19 psychological research consortium (C19PRC) study. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2021;30(1):1–55. pmid:33166018
7. Bruno G, Panzeri A, Granziol U, Alivernini F, Chirico A, Galli F, et al. The Italian COVID-19 Psychological Research Consortium (IT C19PRC): General Overview and Replication of the UK Study. J Clin Med. 2020;10(1):52. pmid:33375763
8. Mignemi G, Panzeri A, Granziol U, Bruno G, Bertamini M, Giulio Vidotto, et al. The mediating role of scientifical-medical satisfaction between COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and vaccine confidence: a two-waves structural equation model. J Behav Med 2022. 22 giugno 2022;1:1–11. pmid:35732987
9. Valiente C, Contreras A, Peinado V, Trucharte A, Martínez AP, Vázquez C. Psychological Adjustment in Spain during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Positive and Negative Mental Health Outcomes in the General Population. Span J Psychol [Internet]. 2021 [citato 4 agosto 2021];24. Disponibile su: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33551011/ pmid:33551011
10. Spikol E, McBride O, Vallières F, Butter S, Hyland P. Tracking the Irish adult population during the first year of the COVID-19 pandemic: A methodological report of the COVID-19 psychological research consortium (C19PRC) study in Ireland. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1 ottobre 2021;220. pmid:34517260
11. Rossi NE, Bisconti TL, Bergeman CS. The Role of Dispositional Resilience in Regaining Life Satisfaction after the Loss of a Spouse. Death Stud. 16 ottobre 2007;31(10):863–83. pmid:17924502
12. Galatzer-Levy IR, Bonanno GA. Beyond normality in the study of bereavement: Heterogeneity in depression outcomes following loss in older adults. Soc Sci Med. giugno 2012;74(12):1987–94. pmid:22472274
13. Bennett KM. How to achieve resilience as an older widower: Turning points or gradual change? Ageing Soc. aprile 2010;30(3):369–82.
14. Windle G. What is resilience? A review and concept analysis. Rev Clin Gerontol. 2011;152–69.
15. Rossi AA, Panzeri A, Taccini F, Parola A, Mannarini S. The rising of the shield hero. Development of the Post-Traumatic Symptom Questionnaire (PTSQ) and assessment of the protective effect of self-esteem from trauma-related anxiety and depression. Journal of Child and Adolescent Trauma—in press. 2022; pmid:36532142
16. Heppenstall CP, Wilkinson TJ, Hanger HC, Dhanak MR, Keeling S. Impacts of the emergency mass evacuation of the elderly from residential care facilities after the 2011 Christchurch earthquake. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2013;7(4):419–23. pmid:24229527
17. Boerner K, Wortman CB, Bonanno GA. Resilient or at risk? A 4-year study of older adults who initially showed high or low distress following conjugal loss. J Gerontol—Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci [Internet]. 2005 [citato 18 novembre 2020];60(2). Disponibile su: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15746020/ pmid:15746020
18. Gaugler JE, Kane RL, Newcomer R. Resilience and transitions from dementia caregiving. J Gerontol—Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci [Internet]. 2007 [citato 18 novembre 2020];62(1). Disponibile su: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17284556/ pmid:17284556
19. Kimhi S, Hantman S, Goroshit M, Eshel Y, Zysberg L. Elderly people coping with the aftermath of war: Resilience versus vulnerability. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;20(5):391–401. pmid:21358387
20. Cosco TD, Kaushal A, Hardy R, Richards M, Kuh D, Stafford M. Operationalising resilience in longitudinal studies: A systematic review of methodological approaches. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2017;71(1):98–104. pmid:27502781
21. Windle G, Bennett KM. The Social Ecology of Resilience. In: The Social Ecology of Resilience. New York, NY: Springer; 2012. p. 219–31.
22. Donnellan WJ, Bennett KM, Soulsby LK. How does carer resilience change over time and care status? A qualitative longitudinal study. Aging Ment Health. 2 novembre 2019;23(11):1510–6. pmid:30449140
23. Bennett KM, Morselli D, Spahni S, Perrig-Chiello P. Trajectories of resilience among widows: a latent transition model. Aging Ment Health. 1 dicembre 2019;0(0):1–8. pmid:31397178
24. Windle G, Bennett KM, MacLeod C. The Influence of Life Experiences on the Development of Resilience in Older People With Co-morbid Health Problems. Front Med. 22 settembre 2020;7:502314. pmid:33072779
25. Bennett KM, Reyes-Rodriguez MF, Altamar P, Soulsby LK. Resilience amongst Older Colombians Living in Poverty: an Ecological Approach. J Cross-Cult Gerontol. 1 dicembre 2016;31(4):385–407. pmid:27585577
26. Panzeri A, Bertamini M, Butter S, Levita L, Gibson-Miller J, Vidotto G, et al. Factors impacting resilience as a result of exposure to COVID-19: The ecological resilience model. Ottoboni G, curatore. PLOS ONE. 18 agosto 2021;16(8):e0256041.
27. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(9):606–13. pmid:11556941
28. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW, Löwe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. 22 maggio 2006;166(10):1092–7. pmid:16717171
29. Eurostat. Population on 1 January by age and sex. [Internet]. Eurostat. 2020. Disponibile su: https://appsso.eurostat.ec.europa.eu/nui/show.do?dataset=demo_pjan&lang=en
30. McBride O, Murphy J, Shevlin M, Gibson Miller J, Hartman TK, Hyland P, et al. Monitoring the psychological impact of the COVID-19 pandemic in the general population: an overview of the context, design and conduct of the COVID-19 Psychological Research Consortium (C19PRC) Study [Internet]. PsyArXiv; 2020 apr [citato 23 novembre 2022]. Disponibile su: https://osf.io/wxe2n
31. McBride O, Butter S, Murphy J, Shevlin M, Hartman TK, Hyland P, et al. Context, design and conduct of the longitudinal COVID-19 psychological research consortium study–wave 3. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 1 settembre 2021;30(3):e1880. pmid:34021946
32. McBride O, Butter S, Hartman TK, Murphy J, Hyland P, Shevlin M, et al. Sharing data to better understand one of the world’s most significant shared experiences: Data resource profile of the longitudinal COVID-19 psychological research consortium (C19PRC) study. Int J Popul Data Sci [Internet]. 17 febbraio 2020 [citato 27 marzo 2022];5(4). Disponibile su: /pmc/articles/PMC8900652/ pmid:35310464
33. McBride O, Butter S, Murphy J, Hartman TK, McKay R, Hyland P, et al. Tracking the psychological and socio-economic impact of the COVID-19 pandemic in the UK: a methodological report from Wave 5 of the COVID-19 Psychological Research Consortium (C19PRC) Study. 2022 [citato 27 marzo 2022]; Disponibile su: https://psyarxiv.com/fyqjb/
34. APA E. DSM-IV-TR—Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: APA, E.; 2000.
35. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Löwe B. The Patient Health Questionnaire Somatic, Anxiety, and Depressive Symptom Scales: A systematic review. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 1 luglio 2010;32(4):345–59. pmid:20633738
36. Shevlin M, Butter S, McBride O, Murphy J, Gibson-Miller J, Hartman TK, et al. Measurement invariance of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) and Generalized Anxiety Disorder scale (GAD-7) across four European countries during the COVID-19 pandemic. BMC Psychiatry. 1 marzo 2022;22(1):154.
37. Tomás-Sábado J, Gómez-Benito J, Limonero JT. The death anxiety inventory: A revision. Psychol Rep. 2005;97(3):793–6. pmid:16512297
38. Rammstedt B, John OP. Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. J Res Personal. 2007;41:203–12.
39. Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A Short Scale for Measuring Loneliness in Large Surveys Results From Two Population-Based Studies. Res Aging. 2004;26(6):655–72. pmid:18504506
40. Robins RW, Hendin HM, Trzesniewski KH. Measuring Global Self-Esteem: Construct Validation of a Single-Item Measure and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Personal Soc Psychol Bullettin. 2001;27(2):151–61.
41. Smith BW, Dalen J, Wiggins K, Tooley E, Christopher P, Bernard J. The brief resilience scale: Assessing the ability to bounce back. Int J Behav Med. luglio 2008;15(3):194–200. pmid:18696313
42. Ganz PA, Guadagnoli E, Landrum MB, Lash TL, Rakowski W, Silliman RA. Breast cancer in older women: Quality of life and psychosocial adjustment in the 15 months after diagnosis. J Clin Oncol. 1 novembre 2003;21(21):4027–33. pmid:14581426
43. Religions Weller P. and Capital Social. Theses on Religion(s), State(s), and Society(ies): with Particular Reference to the United Kingdom and the European Union. J Int Migr Integr. 2005;6(2):271–89.
44. Cloitre M, Shevlin M, Brewin CR, Bisson JI, Roberts NP, Maercker A, et al. The International Trauma Questionnaire: development of a self-report measure of ICD-11 PTSD and complex PTSD. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1 dicembre 2018;138(6):536–46. pmid:30178492
45. Buhr K, Dugas MJ. The intolerance of uncertainty scale: Psychometric properties of the English version. Behav Res Ther. 1 agosto 2002;40(8):931–45. pmid:12186356
46. Gibson-Miller J, Hartman TK, Levita L, Martinez AP, Mason L, McBride O, et al. Capability, opportunity, and motivation to enact hygienic practices in the early stages of the COVID-19 outbreak in the United Kingdom. Br J Health Psychol. 16 novembre 2020;25(4):856–64. pmid:32415918
47. Michie S, van Stralen MM, West R. The behaviour change wheel: A new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implement Sci. 23 aprile 2011;6(1):1–12. pmid:21513547
48. Morgan K, Dallosso HM, Arie T, Byrne EJ, Jones R, Waite J. Mental health and psychological well-being among the old and the very old living at home. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150(JUNE):801–7. pmid:3651735
49. Simonetti B, Sarnacchiaro P, González Rodríguez MR. Goodness of fit measures for logistic regression model: an application for students’ evaluations of university teaching. Qual Quant. 1 novembre 2017;51(6):2545–54.
50. Cragg JG, Uhler RS. The demand for automobiles. Can J Econ Can Econ. 1970;3(3):386–406.
51. Lynn P. Methodology of Longitudinal Surveys [Internet]. Methodology of Longitudinal Surveys. wiley; 2009 [citato 18 giugno 2022]. 1–396 p. Disponibile su: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/book/10.1002/9780470743874
52. Gagné T, Schoon I, Mcmunn A, Sacker A. Mental distress among young adults in Great Britain: long-term trends and early changes during the COVID-19 pandemic. 2022;57:1261–72.
53. Parola A, Rossi A, Tessitore F, Troisi G, Mannarini S. Mental Health Through the COVID-19 Quarantine: A Growth Curve Analysis on Italian Young Adults. Front Psychol. 2 ottobre 2020;11:2466. pmid:33132973
54. Hong S, Kim H, Park MK. Impact of COVID-19 on post-traumatic stress symptoms in the general population: An integrative review. Int J Ment Health Nurs. 2021;30(4):834–46. pmid:33884723
55. Menzies RE, Menzies RG. Death anxiety in the time of COVID-19: Theoretical explanations and clinical implications. Cogn Behav Ther [Internet]. 2020 [citato 4 maggio 2022];13. Disponibile su: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1754470X20000215 pmid:34191938
56. Ikizer G, Kowal M, Aldemir İD, Jeftić A, Memisoglu-Sanli A, Najmussaqib A, et al. Big Five traits predict stress and loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic: Evidence for the role of neuroticism. Personal Individ Differ. 1 maggio 2022;190:111531. pmid:35095147
57. Wennekers S, Thurik R, Van Stel A, Noorderhaven N. Uncertainty avoidance and the rate of business ownership across 21 OECD countries, 1976–2004. J Evol Econ. 9 aprile 2007;17(2):133–60.
58. Bottesi G, Ghisi M, Carraro E, Barclay N, Payne R, Freeston MH. Revising the intolerance of uncertainty model of generalized anxiety disorder: Evidence from UK and Italian undergraduate samples. Front Psychol. 2016;7(November):1–13. pmid:27847496
59. Istituto Nazionale di Statistica. Conciliazione tra lavoro e famiglia. Istituto Nazionale di Statistica; 2019 nov p. 14. (Statistiche report.).
60. Volpi F. Il vero volto della famiglia italiana: un racconto attraverso i dati [Internet]. Roma, Italy: Istituto di ricerche formative e educative; (Fondazione IREF). Disponibile su: https://www.acli.it/wp-content/uploads/2019/12/REPORT_Famiglia-1.pdf
61. Haslam SA, Jetten J, Postmes T, Haslam C. Social Identity, Health and Well-Being: An Emerging Agenda for Applied Psychology. Appl Psychol- Int Rev-Psychol Appl-Rev Int. gennaio 2009;58(1):1–23.
62. Panzeri A, Bettinardi O, Bottesi G, Bertolotti G, Brambatti L, Monfredo M, et al. Assessment of perceived support in the context of emergency: Development and validation of the psycho-social support scale. Curr Psychol. giugno 2022;12. pmid:35789631
63. Benyamini Y, Idler LE. Community studies reporting association between self-rated health and mortality. Res Aging. maggio 1999;21(3):392–401.
64. Bu F, Hei , Mak W, Fancourt D. Rates and predictors of uptake of mental health support during the COVID-19 pandemic: an analysis of 26,720 adults in the UK in lockdown. 2021;56:2287–97.
65. Driggin E, Madhavan MV, Bikdeli B, Chuich T, Laracy J, Biondi-Zoccai G, et al. Cardiovascular Considerations for Patients, Health Care Workers, and Health Systems During the COVID-19 Pandemic. J Am Coll Cardiol. 12 maggio 2020;75(18):2352–71. pmid:32201335
66. Dobson KS, Dozois DJ. Handbook of cognitive-behavioral therapies [Internet]. New York: Guilford Publications; 2019 [citato 10 maggio 2019]. Disponibile su: https://books.google.it/books?hl=it&lr=&id=3T2EDwAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PP1&dq=cognitive+behavioral+theory+beck&ots=xrWrCqEoSS&sig=TqK6pD8JI0PFNW7oLf_iU3rtw6w&redir_esc=y#v=onepage&q=cognitive behavioral theory beck&f=false
67. Giuntoli L, Marchetti I, Panzeri A, Spoto A, Vidotto G, Caudek C. Measuring cognitive vulnerability to depression: Further evidence on the factorial and predictive validity of negative cognitive style. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. dicembre 2019;65(April):101479. pmid:31078919
68. Pyszczynski T, Solomon S, Greenberg J, Arndt J, Schimel J. Why do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical review. Psychol Bull. 2004;130(3):435–68. pmid:15122930
69. Rossi A, Panzeri A, Pietrabissa G, Manzoni GM, Castelnuovo G, Mannarini S, et al. The Anxiety-Buffer Hypothesis in the Time of COVID-19: When Self-Esteem Protects From the Impact of Loneliness and Fear on Anxiety and Depression. Front Psychol. 2020;11(August):2177. pmid:33240140
70. Burke BL, Martens A, Faucher EH. Two decades of terror management theory: A meta-analysis of mortality salience research. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. maggio 2010;14(2):155–95. pmid:20097885
71. Oshio A, Taku K, Hirano M, Saeed G. Resilience and Big Five personality traits: A meta-analysis. Personal Individ Differ. 1 giugno 2018;127:54–60.
72. Steel P, Schmidt J, Shultz J. Refining the Relationship Between Personality and Subjective Well-Being. Psychol Bull. gennaio 2008;134(1):138–61. pmid:18193998
73. Kashdan TB, Rottenberg J. Psychological Flexibility as a Fundamental Aspect of Health. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(7):865. pmid:21151705
74. Kroska EB, Roche AI, Adamowicz JL, Stegall MS. Psychological flexibility in the context of COVID-19 adversity: Associations with distress. J Context Behav Sci. 1 ottobre 2020;18:28–33. pmid:32837889
75. Borkovec TD, Ray WJ, Stöber J. Worry: A cognitive phenomenon intimately linked to affective, physiological, and interpersonal behavioral processes. Cogn Ther Res. 1998;22(6):561–76.
76. Bottesi G, Ghisi M, Sica C, Freeston MH. Intolerance of uncertainty, not just right experiences, and compulsive checking: Test of a moderated mediation model on a non-clinical sample. Compr Psychiatry. 1 febbraio 2017;73:111–9. pmid:27939647
77. Sorrentino RM, Roney CJR. The Uncertain Mind [Internet]. The Uncertain Mind. New York: Psychology Press; 2013. Disponibile su: https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203765487
78. Kruglanski AW, Webster DM. Motivated Closing of the Mind: «Seizing» and «Freezing». Psychol Rev. 1996;103(2):263–83.
79. Bottesi G, Marchetti I, Sica C, Ghisi M. What is the internal structure of intolerance of uncertainty? A network analysis approach. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;75(January):1–10. pmid:32827868
80. Borkovec DT, Alcaine MO, Behar E. Avoidance Theory of Worry and Generalized Anxiety Disorder. In: Heimberg RG, Turk CL, Mennin DS, curatori . Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Advances in Research and Practice. New York: Guilford Press; 2004. p. 77–108.
81. Skodzik T, Leopold A, Ehring T. Effects of a training in mental imagery on worry: A proof-of-principle study. J Anxiety Disord. 1 gennaio 2017;45:24–33. pmid:27915122
82. Rossi AA, Panzeri A, Mannarini S. The Italian Version of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale—Short Form (IT-DERS-SF): A Two-Step Validation Study. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, In press. 2022;
83. Russell DW. UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): Reliability, validity, and factor structure. J Pers Assess. 1996;66(1):20–40. pmid:8576833
84. Usher K, Bhullar N, Jackson D. Life in the pandemic: Social isolation and mental health. J Clin Nurs [Internet]. 2020; Disponibile su: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32250493 pmid:32250493
85. Cacioppo JT, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychol Aging. 2006;21(1):140–51. pmid:16594799
86. Cacioppo S, Capitanio JP, Cacioppo JT. Toward a Neurology of Loneliness HHS Public Access. Psychol Bull. 2014;140(6):1464–504.
87. Santini ZI, Jose PE, York Cornwell E, Koyanagi A, Nielsen L, Hinrichsen C, et al. Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and symptoms of depression and anxiety among older Americans (NSHAP): a longitudinal mediation analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2020;5(1):e62–70. pmid:31910981
88. Brindal E, Ryan JC, Kakoschke N, Golley S, Zajac IT, Wiggins B. Individual differences and changes in lifestyle behaviours predict decreased subjective well-being during COVID-19 restrictions in an Australian sample. J Public Health Oxf Engl. 4 marzo 2021;1–7.
89. Consoli S, Rossi A, Thompson LY, Volpi C, Mannarini S, Castelnuovo G, et al. Assessing Psychometric Properties of the Italian Version of the Heartland Forgiveness Scale. Front Psychol. 17 dicembre 2020;11:1–9.
90. Cvencek D, Greenwald AG, Meltzoff AN. Implicit measures for preschool children confirm self-esteem’s role in maintaining a balanced identity. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1 gennaio 2016;62:50–7.
91. Niveau N, New B, Beaudoin M. Self-esteem Interventions in Adults–A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. J Res Personal. 1 ottobre 2021;94:104131.
92. Williams CYK, Townson AT, Kapur M, Ferreira AF, Nunn R, Galante J, et al. Interventions to reduce social isolation and loneliness during COVID-19 physical distancing measures: A rapid systematic review. PLOS ONE. 1 febbraio 2021;16(2):e0247139. pmid:33596273
93. Panzeri A, Rossi Ferrario S. Supporting rehabilitation patients with COVID-19 during the pandemic: Experiences from a technology-based psychological approach. In: CEUR Workshop Proceedings: Second Symposium on Psychology-Based Technologies—Psychobit. Naples; 2020.
94. Shevlin M, Butter S, McBride O, Murphy J, Gibson-Miller J, Hartman TK, et al. Psychological responses to the COVID-19 pandemic are heterogeneous but have stabilised over time: One year longitudinal follow-up of the COVID-19 Psychological Research Consortium (C19PRC) study. Psychol Med. 2021;1–9.
95. Ratti MM, Rossi A, Delli Zotti GB, Sarno L, Spotti D. Social support, psychological distress and depression in hemodialysis patients. Psicol Della Salute. febbraio 2017;(1):112–22.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2023 Bennett et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Although the COVID-19 pandemic has impacted the psychological wellbeing of some people, there is evidence that many have been much less affected. The Ecological Model of Resilience (EMR) may explain why some individuals are not resilient whilst others are. In this study we test the EMR in a comparison of UK survey data collected from the COVID-19 Psychological Research Consortium (C19PRC) longitudinal study of a representative sample of the United Kingdom (UK) adult population and data from an Italian arm of the study. We first compare data from the third wave of the UK arm of the study, collected in July/August 2020, with data from an equivalent sample and stage of the pandemic in Italy in July 2020. Next, using UK longitudinal data collected from C19PRC Waves 1, 3 and 5, collected between March 2020 and April 2021 we identify the proportion of people who were resilient. Finally, we examine which factors, drawn from the EMR, predict resilient and non-resilient outcomes. We find that the 72% of the UK sample was resilient, in line with the Italian study. In the cross-sectional logistic regression model, age and self-esteem were significantly associated with resilience whilst death anxiety thoughts, neuroticism, loneliness, and Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) symptoms related to COVID-19 were significantly associated with Non-Resilient outcomes. In the longitudinal UK analysis, at Wave 5, 80% of the sample was Resilient. Service use, belonging to wider neighbourhood, self-rated health, self-esteem, openness, and externally generated death anxiety were associated with Resilient outcomes. In contrast, PTSD symptoms and loneliness were associated with Non-Resilient outcomes. The EMR effectively explained the results. There were some variables which are amenable to intervention which could increase resilience in the face of similar future challenges.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details



