This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
1. Introduction
Visual motor integration (VMI) skills are critical for participation in meaningful activities [1]. VMI skills are necessary to complete early education activities that later influence success in kindergarten and primary school [2, 3]. For example, fundamental childhood activities such as handwriting, keyboarding, and throwing and catching a ball all utilize VMI skills. These required VMI skills surpass an isolated motor response and are complex due to the visual motor and fine motor coordination components [4]. Deficiencies in VMI skills are multifaceted, negatively impacting participation and self-concept [5, 6].
VMI is the ability to perceive visual input, process the information, and coordinate a motor response [1]. VMI skills encompass eye-hand coordination, praxis, visual perceptual skills, gross motor coordination, and fine motor coordination [7]. The ability to control hand movements through vision is necessary for a multitude of academic and nonacademic endeavors.
Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), brachial plexus injury (BPI), and cerebral palsy (CP) who have motor coordination and praxis difficulties are at an increased risk for VMI deficits [8]. The research has noted praxis deficits in children with ASD, BPI, and CP [9]. Six recent studies including a meta-analysis found motor coordination to be negatively impacted by ASD [10–15]. Four recent studies found motor coordination to be negatively impacted by BPI ([16–20]). Five recent studies found motor coordination to be negatively impacted by CP [21–26]. Overall, children with ASD, BPI, and CP are at an increased risk for having VMI deficits due to dyspraxia and motor coordination limitations.
Previous studies suggest that occupational therapy intervention improves VMI skills in children with disabilities as captured by the Beery VMI [4, 27–32]. Six studies found occupational therapy interventions positively impact visual motor performance in children with developmental delays [4, 27–31]. Additionally, a recent systematic review concluded that occupational therapy intervention promotes motor development, a component of VMI skills, in children from zero to five years of age [32]. These seven landmark articles confirmed consensus that occupational therapy intervention improves VMI skills in children [4, 27–32].
Occupational therapy practitioners utilize the Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration (Beery VMI) to assess VMI in children. VMI skills develop sequentially, and the Beery VMI adheres to this developmental sequence [33, 34]. The Beery VMI is a pen and paper assessment that requires the patient to imitate and later copy forms of increasing difficulty [35]. The first several forms are imitated, where the occupational therapist physically demonstrates the form then the child attempts to imitate it. The remaining forms are copied, where the child looks at the form and attempts to copy it [34]. The child’s ability to imitate and copy the forms in a sequential order indicates the child’s VMI age equivalence. The Beery VMI has been found to be psychometrically sound and is considered the standard assessment for VMI skills [34–36].
The profession of occupational therapy strives to provide evidence-based practice. The utilization of standardized measures such as the Beery VMI supports best practice as it allows the effectiveness of occupational therapy intervention to be assessed over time [37]. The effectiveness of occupational therapy practice through standardized measures is essential for occupational therapy professionals to continue to make informed, evidence-based decisions about best practice and the effectiveness of intervention techniques.
The contemporary research regarding VMI in children with developmental disabilities and the impact of occupational therapy is limited in both quantity and quality (i.e., generalizability). Additionally, there is no reference available denoting the expected changes in Beery VMI scores following occupational therapy intervention. Due to the lack of current evidence on the topic, this review of literature broadly evaluated the current understanding of VMI scores of children with developmental disabilities. This literature review addressed the gap by evaluating the contemporary research and answers the question, “What factors influence VMI scores of children with developmental disabilities?”.
2. Review of the Literature
This literature review explored the factors that influence VMI of children with developmental disabilities. Relevant articles were identified by searching EBSCO, Google Scholar, and Sage Journals through Concordia University’s Research Databases based on the availability of search engines. Search terms included various combinations of the following: “visual motor integration,” “children,” and “developmental disabilities”.
Criteria for inclusion in this review were the following: (a) articles were original scientific reports of studies, (b) articles were focused on visual motor integration in children including those two to five years of age, (c) articles were published in English-language peer-reviewed journals, (d) articles were available in full-text, and (e) articles were published during or after 2010. Criteria for exclusion in this review were the following: (a) articles that were dissertations or theses, (b) articles that did not include visual motor integration or children, (c) articles that were published in non-English language, (d) published abstracts, (e) articles published before 2010, and (f) articles that focused on children with intellectual disabilities (ID). Much of the research on developmental disabilities and VMI has focused on children with ID. The authors determined that the research and understanding for this specific diagnosis was adequate due to published meta-analyses on the subject [38]. Therefore, ID were not included in this literature review.
The literature search produced 986 titles which were first scanned for relevance. Two hundred thirty-five relevant abstracts were evaluated with 54 being selected and reviewed. After applying inclusion and exclusion criteria, 24 articles were chosen for this review.
Themes were noted during the critique of articles. The themes were not derived through a specific qualitative methodological approach; rather they were generated based on recurring similarities. As such, articles were categorized into three demographic themes: “age,” “gender,” and “diagnosis”.
2.1. Age
Throughout the first five years of life, children achieve gross and fine motor milestones that enable them to interact with the world around them. Each milestone provides the foundation for the achievement of successive milestones as the child progresses towards autonomy [39]. When milestones are delayed or not achieved, the child is considered to have an early marker for further impaired development [40]. VMI developmental milestones begin at three months of age, when an infant can track an object in supine [41]. Milestones progress as the child develops with the Beery VMI accurately assessing VMI skills starting at two years of age [42].
Six recent studies explored the implications of age and VMI performance in children. Two studies included children five years and older [43, 44]. Three studies investigated performance of children starting at four years of age [45–47]. One study included children as young as three years six months [48]. All studies were at a consensus that age impacted VMI scores [43–48]. Table 1 provides a summary of the findings.
Table 1
Summary of age and visual motor integration performance studies.
Authors | Descriptions | Subjects | Findings |
Coutinho et al. [45] | Explored the effectiveness of iPad applications in children four years to seven years old with poor VMI skills | 20 children between the ages of four years and seven years 11 months with poor VMI skills | No significant difference was found |
Ercan et al. [43] | Utilized the Beery VMI to investigate the VMI skills of children of varying socioeconomic statuses | Children between the ages of 60 months to 72 months from low and high economic status | Children 67 to 72 months and those of high socioeconomic status had better VMI scores |
Fang et al. [46] | Utilized the Beery VMI to examine the relationship between motor coordination, visual perception, and executive function to VMI skills | 151 children between the ages of four and six years of age | VMI skills of children increased at four years of age and peaked at five years of age |
Heiz and Barisnikov [47] | Utilized the Beery VMI to assess the VMI differences between children with Williams syndrome and those of typical development | 26 participants with Williams syndrome aged six to 41 years old and 154 children of typical development between the ages of four and 12 years old | Age did not influence ability in the children with Williams syndrome, whereas age was a significant factor for those of typical development |
Ng et al. [48] | Utilized the Beery VMI to examine performance differences in preschool children | 288 children between three years six months to five years 11 months from 54 preschools | Hong Kong cohort scored significantly better when compared to the normative sample of preschoolers in the United States |
Visser et al. [44] | Utilized the Beery VMI to investigate skills of five-year-old English-speaking children who reside in South Africa | 68 children between the ages of five years six months to five years 11 months from seven schools | VMI skills of children scored below average in all the subtests, but compared well to the normative sample established in the United States |
Note. The abbreviations “Beery VMI” and “VMI” represent the “Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration” and “visual motor integration”, respectively.
Recently, one study utilized the Beery VMI to examine the relationship between motor coordination, visual perception, and executive function to VMI skills of four to six-year-old preschoolers [46]. VMI skills of children increased at four years of age and peaked at five years of age [46]. These findings, however, may not be applicable for all populations. Research investigating children between the ages of four and 12 years old with Williams Syndrome and those of typical development concluded that age impacted VMI scores of children who were typically developing, but not children with Williams syndrome [47]. Overall, these studies illustrate that age may have varying influences on different populations of children.
Two studies investigating preschool children aged three years six months to five years, respectively, concluded that age and geographic location affected VMI scores [44, 48]. Similar findings were noted previously when 60 to 72-month-old children were studied [43]. Additionally, this study concluded that age and socioeconomic status, commonly associated with location, were found to impact VMI scores. Overall, these three studies illustrate how age as well as location/socioeconomic status impact the VMI scores of children.
Lastly, one recent study explored VMI scores of children four years to seven years of age in relation to intervention [45]. Age was found to impact VMI score, while occupational therapy intervention was concluded to still be the superior VMI intervention when compared to the latest iPad intervention [45]. Overall, the six studies were at a consensus that age impacts VMI scores and needs to be accounted for during research and clinical practice [43–48].
2.2. Gender
It is theorized that gender-related differences in motor learning and performance impact VMI skills [49, 50]. Older studies have shown fine motor and visual motor skills to be similar between sexes or better in females [51]. The inconsistencies in the aforementioned body of literature do not allow for conclusive statements regarding VMI skills in preschool children.
The current literature clarifies previous findings. Four of the five studies indicate significant differences between gender while one study found no significant difference [52–56]. Table 2 provides a summary of the findings.
Table 2
Summary of gender and visual motor integration performance studies.
Authors | Descriptions | Subjects | Findings |
Coallier et al. [52] | Utilized the Beery VMI to assess children in Canada and compare to the norms established United States sample | 151 children ages of five to six years old from kindergarten classes in Quebec, Canada | Significant gender difference was found where females obtained higher mean scores than males |
Cui et al. [53] | Utilized the Beery VMI to determine VMI performance in Chinese children compared the normative sample established in the United States | 365 children ages of three to 12 years old from six public schools in Shanghai, China and Ningbo, China | Gender and residence differences were significantly greater for the Chinese children that were female and from Shanghai |
Memisevic and Hadzic [54] | Utilized the Beery VMI to assess the relationship between VMI and articulation disorders in children | 286 children ages of three to six years old from public schools in Canton Sarajevo | Significant gender difference was found for both VMI and articulation disorders where males are at a higher risk compared to females |
Nicola and Watter [55] | Utilized the Beery VMI to investigate the VMI skills of children four to 14 years old with severe language impairment | 100 children ages of four to 14 years old with language scores at least two standard deviations below the mean in Australia | No significant difference in mean performance between males and females; however, children with poor VMI skills also had poor receptive language skills |
Singh et al. [56] | Utilized the Beery VMI to investigate the impact of stimulating activities on Haryana children between the ages of two and three years old | 100 children between the ages of two and three years old were randomly selected from two Indian villages of Hisar District of Haryana, Gangva as the control and Muklan the experimental group | Initially, there were gender differences favoring females before the stimulating activities intervention; however, males superseded female skills after the intervention |
Note. The abbreviations “Beery VMI” and “VMI” represent the “Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration” and “visual motor integration”, respectively.
Four of the five studies noted differences between genders [52–54, 56]. Specifically, one study reported that females had greater VMI abilities than males [52]. While another study concluded that males were at a higher risk for VMI deficits [54]. Another study noted gender differences favoring females before a stimulating activity intervention; however, males superseded female skills after the intervention [56]. Therefore, although gender differences have been reported, there are inconsistencies regarding which gender has superior VMI skills [52, 54, 56].
Conversely, one study found no significant differences between genders for children four to 14 years old with severe language impairment [55]. There was no significant difference in mean performance between males and females; however, children with poor VMI skills also had poor receptive language skills [55]. VMI deficits in children with a language impairment component are common due to the interconnection between motor, visual, and integration difficulties. Overall, gender may impact VMI skills in children, but no definitive conclusions can be drawn at this time [53, 55, 56].
2.3. Diagnosis
In the United States, it is reported by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [57] that one in six children has one or more developmental disabilities or other developmental delays. Examples of developmental disabilities include, but are not limited to, ASD, BPI, and CP. Additional diagnoses that are less common can also impact VMI skills in children.
Findings in the literature and the author’s clinical experiences highlight the common association of ASD, BPI, and CP with VMI deficits. Children with ASD, BPI, and CP who have low motor abilities are at an increased risk for VMI deficits [8]. The neurological and physical aspects of the diagnoses contribute to the impaired fine motor and visual motor components necessary for VMI skills. The current literature illustrates the VMI deficits experienced by children with ASD, BPI, and CP [16, 58–64].
2.4. ASD
Three recent studies that focused on children with ASD reiterated the implications of the diagnosis on VMI skills [61–63]. Two studies specifically addressed VMI while one study explored the kinematic aspect of VMI skills of children with ASD [61–63]. Overall, the studies were at a consensus that ASD negatively impacts VMI skills [61–63]. Table 3 provides a summary of the findings.
Table 3
Summary of autism spectrum disorder and visual motor integration performance studies.
Authors | Descriptions | Subjects | Findings |
Dowd et al. [61] | Explored kinematics of children three to seven years old with ASD | 13 children with ASD between the ages of three and seven years old were matched to a group of 13 children of typical development in Australia | Children with ASD demonstrated increased variability in the preparation phase of movement due to VMI deficits |
Englund et al. [62] | Investigated the implications of ASD and ADHD on working memory and VMI when compared to typically developing children | 49 children with ADHD, 33 children with ASD, and 79 children of typical development aged four to 18 years old were compared in the United States | Both children with ASD and ADHD demonstrated working memory and VMI deficits |
Green et al. [63] | Examined Beery VMI performance in males with ASD compared to males with typical development between the ages of three to 23 years old | 56 participants with ASD and 36 participants of typical development were compared with multiple versions of the Beery VMI being administered throughout the longitudinal study conducted in the United States | No significant group differences were observed between the three versions for either group; however, a considerable number of males with ASD experienced difficulties compared to typically developing males in performing VMI-related tasks |
Note. The abbreviations “ASD,” “ADHD,” “Beery VMI,” and “VMI” represent “autism spectrum disorder,” “attention deficit hyperactivity disorder,” “Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration,” and “visual motor integration,” respectively.
Most recently, a study examined Beery VMI performance in males with ASD compared to males with typical development between the ages of three and 23 years old [63]. The study found a considerable number of the males with ASD experienced difficulties in performing VMI-related tasks compared to the males who were typically developing [63]. Similarly, previous research investigated the implications of ASD and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on working memory and VMI when compared to children who are typically developing aged four to 18 years old [62]. It was concluded that both children with ASD and ADHD demonstrated working memory and VMI deficits [62]. Overall, both studies illustrate deficient VMI skills in children with ASD.
One study explored kinematics of children three to seven years old with ASD [61]. Children with ASD demonstrated increased variability in the preparation phase of movement due to VMI deficits that allow the integration of visual information to inform movement planning and execution [61]. Overall, the studies were at a consensus that ASD negatively impacts VMI scores [61–63].
2.5. BPI
Only two recent studies explored the VMI skills of children with BPI. The two studies that focused on children with BPI alluded to the possible implications of the diagnosis on VMI skills [16, 64]. Overall, the studies were at a consensus that BPI may have a negative impact on VMI skills [16, 64]. Table 4 provides a summary of the findings.
Table 4
Summary of brachial plexus injury and visual motor integration performance studies.
Authors | Descriptions | Subjects | Findings |
Bellows et al. [16] | Explored coordination and balance deficits in children with BPI between the ages of five and 15 years old | 39 children with birth-related BPI between the ages of five and 15 years old in British Columbia | More than half the participants indicated disability for bilateral coordination or body coordination on a self-report measure of physical disability |
van der Holst et al. [64] | Investigated quality of life and motor function in children with BPI under three years of age based on parent perception | 59 children with a median age of 18 months participated from the Netherlands | Perception was impacted by side, severity of BPI, and motor function of the upper extremity |
Note. The abbreviation “BPI” represents “brachial plexus injury,” respectively.
Most recently, a study investigated quality of life in children with BPI under three years of age based on parent perception [64]. It was concluded that perception was impacted by the side affected (i.e., right or left) and severity of BPI [64]. Severity of BPI is linked to negative impacts on physical functioning including VMI tasks [65]. Similarly, a study explored coordination and balance deficits in children with BPI between the ages of five to 15 years old [16]. More than half the participants indicated disability for bilateral coordination or body coordination on a self-report measure of physical disability [16]. Overall, these studies demonstrated the functional limitations experienced by children with BPI, which can impact VMI skills [16, 64].
2.6. CP
Three recent studies included in this review that focused on the VMI of children with CP reiterated the implications of the diagnosis on VMI skills and the improvements in VMI skills with intervention. Two of the three studies investigated augmented biofeedback and traditional therapy as a specific intervention to address VMI skills in children with CP [58, 59]. The third study focused on the functional implications of the condition [60]. Overall, the studies included in this review were at a consensus that CP negatively impacts VMI skills [58–60]. Table 5 provides a summary of the findings.
Table 5
Summary of cerebral palsy and visual motor integration performance studies.
Authors | Descriptions | Subjects | Findings |
Alwhaibi et al. [58] | Investigated eye-hand coordination in children with CP between the ages of five and eight years old before and after receiving varied interventions | 45 children between the ages of five to eight years old with spastic hemiplegia were randomized into three groups | Augmented biofeedback with traditional physical therapy intervention significantly improved VMI when compared to children that only received one intervention |
Alwhaibi et al. [59] | Examined VMI in children with CP between the ages of five and eight years old before and after receiving varied interventions | 45 children between the ages of five and eight years old with spastic hemiplegia were randomized into three groups | Augmented biofeedback with traditional physical therapy intervention significantly improved eye hand coordination when compared to children that only received one intervention |
Cho et al. [60] | Explored visual perceptual intervention on VMI and activities of daily living performance of children with CP | 56 children with CP between four and seven years old | Visual perceptual intervention had a positive influence on the VMI and activities of daily living performance of children with cerebral palsy |
Note. The abbreviations “CP” and “VMI” represent “cerebral palsy” and “visual motor integration,” respectively.
Most recently, two studies investigated eye-hand coordination in children with CP between the ages of five to eight years old before and after receiving varied interventions [58, 59]. Augmented biofeedback with traditional therapy intervention significantly improved VMI when compared to children that only received one intervention [58, 59]. Overall, the studies demonstrated the impact of CP and intervention on VMI scores.
The third research study explored visual perceptual intervention on VMI skills and activities of daily living performance of children with CP [60]. Visual perceptual intervention had a positive influence on the VMI skills and activities of daily living performance of children with CP [60]. Overall, the studies illustrate that deficient VMI skills in children with CP can be improved through intervention [58–60].
2.7. Other Diagnoses
Five recent studies explored the implications of other diagnoses (e.g., coordination disorders, prematurity, and HIV) and VMI performance in children [66–70]. Three studies examined VMI deficits in children with coordination disorders [66, 68, 70]. One study reported VMI deficits in children of very preterm and very low birth weight [67]. Another study reported a negative impact on VMI skills in children with primary stereotypic movement disorder [70]. Overall, the literature was at a consensus that diagnosis impacts VMI skills [66–70]. Table 6 provides a summary of the findings.
Table 6
Summary of other diagnoses and visual motor integration performance studies.
Authors | Descriptions | Subjects | Findings |
Duijff et al. [66] | Investigated intelligence and VMI in children with 2q11-deletion syndrome the ages of five years zero months to five years 11 months old through administration of the Beery VMI | 65 children between the ages of the ages of five years zero months and five years 11 months old with 22q11-deletion syndrome | A significant correlation was found between IQ scores and Beery VMI scores; however, no correlation between motor coordination and IQ was found |
Geldof et al. [67] | Investigated VMI in children of very preterm and very low birth weight through administration of the Beery VMI | 106 children at five years six months old who were born before 32 weeks gestational age and weighed less than 1500 grams | Approximately 1/3 of the children demonstrated impaired motor functioning and a range of deficits in attention and VMI skills |
Jarus et al. [68] | Investigated participation patterns of children with and without DCD | 25 children with DCD between the ages of the ages of five to seven years old and 25 children without DCD between the ages of the ages of five years and seven years old | Preference to participate in activities was positively correlated with motor ability as captured by the VMI score |
Odejayi et al. [69] | Investigated VMI delays in children infected with HIV | 71 children between the ages of five years zero months and five years 11 months old who were HIV positive through vertical transmission | A moderate relationship was found between VMI scores, school attendance, and mother’s level of education |
Valente et al. [70] | Investigated the motor profiles of children with primary stereotypic movement disorder and compare to children of typical development | 26 children between 35 months and 76 months old with primary stereotypic movement disorder were compared to 27 children of typical development between 36 and 59 months old | No difference was found between children with primary stereotypic movement disorder and children of typical development in VMI total score |
Note. The abbreviations “VMI,” “Beery VMI,” and “DCD” represent “visual motor integration,” “Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration,” and “developmental coordination disorder,” respectively.
Three studies examined VMI deficits in children with coordination disorders [66, 68, 70]. Participation in activities was positively correlated with motor ability as captured by the VMI score in children with DCD [68]. However, no difference was found between children with primary stereotypic movement disorder and children of typical development in VMI total score [70]. Similarly, no correlation between motor coordination and IQ was found in children with 22q11-deletion syndrome, but a significant correlation was found between IQ scores and Beery VMI scores [66]. Overall, VMI skills varied depending on the diagnosis studied.
Additionally, two studies investigated various subpopulations of children and found VMI deficits in each population [67, 70]. Children of very preterm and very low birth weight demonstrated impaired motor functioning and a range of deficits in attention and VMI skills [67]. For children infected with HIV, a moderate relationship was found between VMI scores, school attendance, and mother’s level of education [69]. Overall, these studies illustrate how children with developmental disabilities often have VMI deficits specific to their diagnosis.
3. Discussion
The systematic review investigated the VMI of children with developmental disabilities. The literature was at a consensus that age effects VMI [43–48]. The literature reported conflicting information regarding the impact of gender on VMI [52–56]. The literature was at a consensus that diagnosis impacts VMI [66–70]. Specifically, ASD, BPI, and CP are associated with VMI deficits [16, 58–64].
These findings are clinically relevant and follow the three major themes of this literature review: age, gender, and diagnosis. First, the findings reiterate the importance of age-based norms when assessing VMI skills. Standardized assessments, such as the Beery VMI, that utilize age-based norms and describe performance with age equivalence are recommended. Occupational therapy practitioners can then use this quantitative data to create measurable goals. Therapeutic activities can also be selected that best promote the acquisition of VMI milestones through the just-right challenge. Second, any VMI evaluation tool needs to be assessed to assure that there is no gender bias (i.e., the Beery VMI). Third, diagnosis can impact VMI and should therefore be assessed by pediatric rehabilitation professionals. VMI can be easily overlooked in pediatric practice when children have multiple deficits; however, VMI can impact multiple areas of occupation. The improvement of VMI can lead to positive changes in occupational performance and should be included during evaluation. Overall, these findings impact current clinical practice.
These findings are also relevant to future research. First, future research will need to control for age and effects of maturation. This can be accomplished by establishing age groups and limiting the length of intervention (e.g., six months). Diagnosis can be addressed by researching specific populations or controlling for diagnosis as a covariate. At this time, it is not necessary to control for gender as long as an appropriate assessment is utilized (e.g., the Beery VMI). With these recommendations in mind, future research should investigate the effects of intervention on the VMI of children with developmental disabilities.
4. Conclusion
This literature review broadly explored the VMI of preschool-aged children with developmental disabilities from which three main themes emerged: age, gender, and diagnosis. The literature was at a consensus that age effects VMI [43–48]. The literature reported conflicting information regarding the impact of gender on VMI [52–56]. The literature was at a consensus that diagnosis impacts VMI [66–70]. Specifically, ASD, BPI, and CP are associated with VMI deficits [16, 58–64].
At this time, it is recommended that both clinicians and researchers take note of the impact of age and diagnosis on VMI. Future studies may clarify the impact of gender on VMI, but the use of an unbiased assessment (e.g., the Beery VMI) helps to control for this potentially confounding variable. Future research should focus on interventions for VMI to improve the efficacy of services provided to children with developmental disabilities. The literature included in this review was mostly descriptive in nature and did not investigate the efficacy of occupational therapy interventions to promote best practice.
[1] C. M. Schneck, "Visual perception," Occupational therapy for children, pp. 357-386, 1996.
[2] K. Beery, The developmental test of visual-motor integration: administration and scoring manual, 1967.
[3] M. J. Weil, S. J. C. Amundson, "Relationship between visuomotor and handwriting skills of children in kindergarten," American Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 48 no. 11, pp. 982-988, DOI: 10.5014/ajot.48.11.982, 1994.
[4] H. L. Dankert, P. L. Davies, W. J. Gavin, "Occupational therapy effects on visual-motor skills in preschool children," American Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 57 no. 5, pp. 542-549, DOI: 10.5014/ajot.57.5.542, 2003.
[5] O. Lahav, A. Apter, N. Z. Ratzon, "Psychological adjustment and levels of self esteem in children with visual- motor integration difficulties influences the results of a randomized intervention trial," Research in Developmental Disabilities, vol. 34 no. 1, pp. 56-64, DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.07.024, 2013.
[6] R. Sanghavi, R. Kelkar, "Visual-motor integration and learning disabled children," Indian Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 37 no. 2, pp. 33-38, 2005.
[7] H. Cornhill, J. Case-Smith, "Factors that relate to good and poor handwriting," American Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 50 no. 9, pp. 732-739, DOI: 10.5014/ajot.50.9.732, 1996.
[8] P. Bonifacci, "Children with low motor ability have lower visual-motor integration ability but unaffected perceptual skills," Human Movement Science, vol. 23 no. 2, pp. 157-168, DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2004.08.002, 2004.
[9] K. J. Steinman, S. H. Mostofsky, M. B. Denckla, "Toward a narrower, more pragmatic view of developmental dyspraxia," Journal of Child Neurology, vol. 25 no. 1, pp. 71-81, DOI: 10.1177/0883073809342591, 2010.
[10] K. A. Fournier, C. J. Hass, S. K. Naik, N. Lodha, J. H. Cauraugh, "Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-analysis," Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 40 no. 10, pp. 1227-1240, DOI: 10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3, 2010.
[11] R. W. Isenhower, K. L. Marsh, M. J. Richardson, M. Helt, R. C. Schmidt, D. Fein, "Rhythmic bimanual coordination is impaired in young children with autism spectrum disorder," Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, vol. 6 no. 1, pp. 25-31, DOI: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.08.005, 2012.
[12] M. Kaur, S. M. Srinivasan, A. N. Bhat, "Comparing motor performance, praxis, coordination, and interpersonal synchrony between children with and without autism spectrum disorder (ASD)," Research in Developmental Disabilities, vol. 72, pp. 79-95, DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.10.025, 2018.
[13] L. K. MacNeil, S. H. Mostofsky, "Specificity of dyspraxia in children with autism," Neuropsychology, vol. 26 no. 2, pp. 165-171, DOI: 10.1037/a0026955, 2012.
[14] D. McAuliffe, A. S. Pillai, A. Tiedemann, S. H. Mostofsky, J. B. Ewen, "Dyspraxia in ASD: impaired coordination of movement elements," Autism Research, vol. 10 no. 4, pp. 648-652, DOI: 10.1002/aur.1693, 2017.
[15] M. Miller, L. Chukoskie, M. Zinni, J. Townsend, D. Trauner, "Dyspraxia, motor function and visual-motor integration in autism," Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 269 no. 1, pp. 95-102, DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.04.011, 2014.
[16] D. Bellows, M. Bucevska, C. Verchere, "Coordination and balance in children with birth-related brachial plexus injury: a preliminary study," Physiotherapy Canada, vol. 67 no. 2, pp. 105-112, DOI: 10.3138/ptc.2013-77, 2015.
[17] L. S. Boylan, Fouladvand, "Developmental apraxia arising from neonatal brachial plexus palsy," Neurology, vol. 56 no. 4, pp. 576-577, DOI: 10.1212/wnl.55.1.24, 2000.
[18] C. H. Mayfield, S. N. Kukke, S. Brochard, C. J. Stanley, K. E. Alter, D. L. Damiano, "Inter-joint coordination analysis of reach-to-grasp kinematics in children and adolescents with obstetrical brachial plexus palsy," Clinical Biomechanics, vol. 46, pp. 15-22, DOI: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2017.04.010, 2017.
[19] O. Nukanna, Visual perceptual abilities in obstetric brachial plexus palsy: an investigation of the incidence and a comparative analysis (Publication No. 8625232), [M.S. thesis], 1998.
[20] O. A. Rapalino, D. N. Levine, "Developmental apraxia arising from neonatal brachial plexus palsy," Neurology, vol. 55 no. 11,DOI: 10.1212/wnl.55.11.1761, 2000.
[21] M. Islam, A. Gordon, A. Sköld, H. Forssberg, A. Eliasson, "Grip force coordination during bimanual tasks in unilateral cerebral palsy," Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, vol. 53 no. 10, pp. 920-926, DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04040.x, 2011.
[22] J. G. Pearsall-Jones, J. P. Piek, F. Levy, "Developmental coordination disorder and cerebral palsy: categories or a continuum?," Human Movement Science, vol. 29 no. 5, pp. 787-798, DOI: 10.1007/s40474-014-0009-3, 2014.
[23] C. Peters, A. Chang, A. Morales, K. Barnes, A. Allegretti, "An integrative review of assessments used in occupational therapy interventions for children with cerebral palsy," Cadernos Brasileiros de Terapia Ocupacional, vol. 27 no. 1, pp. 168-185, DOI: 10.4322/2526-8910.ctoAR1856, 2019.
[24] B. C. M. Smits-Engelsman, K. Kingels, H. Feys, "Bimanual force coordination in children with spastic unilateral cerebral palsy," Research in Developmental Disabilities, vol. 32 no. 5, pp. 2011-2019, DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.04.007, 2011.
[25] A. J. Spittle, J. Orton, "Cerebral palsy and developmental coordination disorder in children born preterm," Seminars in Fetal and Neonatal Medicine, vol. 19 no. 2, pp. 84-89, DOI: 10.1016/j.siny.2013.11.005, 2014.
[26] J. Williams, C. Hyde, A. Spittle, "Developmental coordination disorder and cerebral palsy: is there a continuum?," Current Developmental Disorders Reports, vol. 1 no. 2, pp. 118-124, DOI: 10.1007/s40474-014-0009-3, 2014.
[27] J. Case-Smith, "Effects of occupational therapy services on fine motor and functional performance in preschool children," American Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 54 no. 4, pp. 372-380, DOI: 10.5014/ajot.54.4.372, 2000.
[28] A. S. Desai, P. V. Rege, "Correlation between developmental test of visual motor integration (VMI) and handwriting in cerebral palsy children," Indian Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 37 no. 2, pp. 27-32, 2005.
[29] R. Kannegieter, "The results of a perceptual-motor-cognitive learning program designed for normal preschool children," American Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 24, pp. 208-214, 1970.
[30] C. E. Oliver, "A sensorimotor program for improving writing readiness Skills in elementary-age children," American Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 44 no. 2, pp. 111-116, DOI: 10.5014/ajot.44.2.111, 1990.
[31] S. Parush, J. Markowitz, "A comparison of two group settings for group treatment in promoting perceptual-motor function of learning disabled children," Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, vol. 17, pp. 47-57, 1997.
[32] K. Tanner, E. Schmidt, K. Martin, M. Bassi, "Interventions within the scope of occupational therapy practice to improve motor performance for children ages 0-5 years: a systematic review," American Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 74 no. 2, article 7402180060p1,DOI: 10.5014/ajot.2020.039644, 2020.
[33] K. E. Beery, N. A. Buktenica, N. A. Beery, The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration: administration, scoring, and teaching manual, 2004.
[34] K. E. Beery, N. A. Buktenica, N. A. Beery, The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration: administration, scoring, and teaching manual, 2010.
[35] Pearson, "Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration, Sixth Edition, The (BEERY™ VMI)," 2018. https://www.pearsonclinical.com/therapy/products/100000663/the-beery-buktenica-developmental-test-of-visual-motor-integration-6th-edition-beery-vmi.html
[36] K. Crotty, I. S. Baron, "Beery developmental test of visual-motor integration (VMI)," Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2011.
[37] M. Law, C. Baum, "Evidence-based occupational therapy," Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 65 no. 3, pp. 131-135, DOI: 10.1177/000841749806500301, 1998.
[38] H. Memisevic, M. Djordjevic, "Visual-motor integration in children with mild intellectual disability: a meta-analysis," Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol. 125 no. 4, pp. 696-717, DOI: 10.1177/0031512518774137, 2018.
[39] J. M. Iverson, "Early motor and communicative development in infants with an older sibling with autism spectrum disorder," Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, vol. 61 no. 11, pp. 2673-2684, DOI: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-RSAUT-18-0035, 2018.
[40] R. Bedford, A. Pickles, C. Lord, "Early gross motor skills predict the subsequent development of language in children with autism spectrum disorder," Autism Research, vol. 9 no. 9, pp. 993-1001, DOI: 10.1002/aur.1587, 2016.
[41] R. J. Scharf, G. J. Scharf, A. Stroustrup, "Developmental milestones," Pediatrics in Review, vol. 37 no. 1, pp. 25-38, DOI: 10.1542/pir.2014-0103, 2016.
[42] C. Y. Lim, P. C. Tan, C. Koh, E. Koh, H. Guo, N. D. Yusoff, C. Q. See, T. Tan, "Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration (Beery-VMI): lessons from exploration of cultural variations in visual-motor integration performance of preschoolers," Child: Care, Health and Development, vol. 41 no. 2, pp. 213-221, DOI: 10.1111/cch.12190, 2015.
[43] Z. Ercan, E. Ahmetoğlu, N. Aral, "Investigating the visual-motor integration skills of 60-72 month children from high and low Socio- Economic status as regard age factor," International Education Studies, vol. 4 no. 3, pp. 100-104, DOI: 10.5539/ies.v4n3p100, 2011.
[44] M. Visser, R. Nel, T. Jansen, L. Kinmont, S. Terblanché, J. van Wyk, "Visual perception of five-year-old English-speaking children in Bloemfontein using the Beery VMI-6, DTVP-3 and TVPS-3," South African Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 47 no. 2, pp. 17-26, DOI: 10.17159/231-3833/1017/v47n2a4, 2017.
[45] F. Coutinho, M. Bosisio, E. Brown, S. Rishikof, E. Skaf, X. Zhang, C. Perlman, S. Kelly, E. Freedin, N. Dahan-Oliel, "Effectiveness of iPad apps on visual-motor skills among children with special needs between 4y0m-7y11m," Disability & Rehabilitation: Assistive Technology, vol. 12 no. 4, pp. 402-410, DOI: 10.1080/17483107.2016.1185648, 2017.
[46] Y. Fang, J. Wang, Y. Zhang, J. Qin, "The Relationship of Motor Coordination, Visual Perception, and Executive Function to the Development of 4–6-Year-Old Chinese Preschoolers’ Visual Motor Integration Skills," BioMed Research International, vol. 2017,DOI: 10.1155/2017/6264254, 2017.
[47] J. Heiz, K. Barisnikov, "Visual-motor integration, visual perception and motor coordination in a population with Williams syndrome and in typically developing children," Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, vol. 60 no. 10, pp. 945-955, DOI: 10.1111/jir.12328, 2016.
[48] M. Ng, M. Chui, L. Lin, A. Fong, D. Chan, "Performance of the visual-motor integration of preschool children in Hong Kong," Hong Kong Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 25 no. 1,DOI: 10.1016/j.hkjot.2015.06.002, 2015.
[49] P. Moreno-Briseño, R. Díaz, A. Campos-Romo, J. Fernandez-Ruiz, "Sex-related differences in motor learning and performance," Behavioral and brain functions, vol. 6 no. 1,DOI: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-74, 2010.
[50] J. R. Thomas, K. E. French, "Gender differences across age in motor performance a meta-analysis," Psychological Bulletin, vol. 98 no. 2, pp. 260-282, DOI: 10.2466/pms.1987.64.2.503, 1987.
[51] J. Kokštejn, M. Musálek, J. J. Tufano, "Are sex differences in fundamental motor skills uniform throughout the entire preschool period?," PloS One, vol. 12 no. 4, article e0176556,DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0176556, 2017.
[52] M. Coallier, N. Rouleau, F. Bara, M. Morin, "Visual-motor skills performance on the Beery-VMI: a study of Canadian kindergarten children," The Open Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 2 no. 2,DOI: 10.15453/2168-6408.1074, 2014.
[53] Y. Cui, Y. Zhu, H. Laukkanen, J. Rabin, "Evaluation of visual-motor integration skills in preschool and elementary school-aged Chinese children," Journal of Behavioral Optometry, vol. 23 no. 5-6, pp. 123-128, 2012.
[54] H. Memisevic, S. Hadzic, "The relationship between visual-motor integration and articulation disorders in preschool children," Journal of Occupational Therapy, Schools, & Early Intervention, vol. 6 no. 1, pp. 23-30, DOI: 10.1080/19411243.2013.771103, 2013.
[55] K. Nicola, P. Watter, "Visual-motor integration performance in children with severe specific language impairment," Child: Care, Health and Development, vol. 42 no. 5, pp. 742-749, DOI: 10.1111/cch.12365, 2016.
[56] C. K. Singh, B. Dhanda, P. Shanwal, "Gender difference in motor and mental development in children: an impact of stimulating activities," The Anthropologist, vol. 12 no. 2, pp. 153-154, DOI: 10.1080/09720073.2010.11891146, 2010.
[57] Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, "Facts about developmental disabilities," 2019. https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/developmentaldisabilities/facts.html
[58] R. M. Alwhaibi, R. S. Alsakhawi, S. M. ElKholi, "Augmented biofeedback training with physical therapy improves visual-motor integration, visual perception, and motor coordination in children with spastic hemiplegic cerebral palsy: a randomised control trial," Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, vol. 40 no. 2, pp. 134-151, DOI: 10.1080/01942638.2019.1646375, 2020.
[59] R. M. Alwhaibi, R. S. Alsakhawi, S. M. ElKholi, "Effects of auditovisual feedback on eye-hand coordination in children with cerebral palsy," Research in Developmental Disabilities, vol. 101,DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103635, 2020.
[60] M. Cho, D. Kim, Y. Yang, "Effects of visual perceptual intervention on visual-motor integration and activities of daily living performance of children with cerebral palsy," Journal of Physical Therapy Science, vol. 27 no. 2, pp. 411-413, DOI: 10.1080/09638280701673609, 2008.
[61] A. Dowd, J. McGinley, J. Taffe, N. Rinehart, "Do planning and visual integration difficulties underpin motor dysfunction in autism? A kinematic study of young children with autism," Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, vol. 42 no. 8, pp. 1539-1548, DOI: 10.1007/s10803-011-1385-8, 2012.
[62] J. A. Englund, S. L. Decker, R. A. Allen, A. M. Roberts, "Common cognitive deficits in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and Autism," Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, vol. 32 no. 2, pp. 95-106, DOI: 10.1177/0734282913505074, 2014.
[63] R. R. Green, E. D. Bigler, A. Froehlich, M. B. D. Prigge, B. G. Travers, A. N. Cariello, J. S. Anderson, B. A. Zielinski, A. Alexander, N. Lange, J. E. Lainhart, "Beery VMI performance in autism spectrum disorder," Child Neuropsychology, vol. 22 no. 7, pp. 795-817, DOI: 10.1080/09297049.2015.1056131, 2016.
[64] M. van der Holst, D. Steenbeek, W. Pondaag, R. G. H. H. Nelissen, T. P. M. Vliet Vlieland, "Neonatal Brachial Plexus Palsy in Children Aged 0 to 2.5 Years; Parent- Perceived Family Impact, Quality of Life, and Upper Extremity Functioning," Pediatric Neurology, vol. 62, pp. 34-42, DOI: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2016.06.015, 2016.
[65] D. Lazzeri de Medeiros, N. B. Agostinho, L. Mochizuki, A. Siriani de Oliveira, "Quality of life and upper limb function of children with neonatal brachial plexus palsy," Revista Paulista de Pediatria, vol. 38, article e2018304,DOI: 10.1590/1984-0462/2020/38/2018304, 2020.
[66] S. Duijff, P. Klaassen, F. Beemer, H. Swanenburg de Veye, J. Vorstman, G. Sinnema, "Intelligence and visual motor integration in 5-year-old children with 22q11-deletion syndrome," Research in Developmental Disabilities, vol. 33 no. 2, pp. 334-340, DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.10.004, 2012.
[67] C. J. A. Geldof, J. W. P. van Hus, M. Jeukens-Visser, F. Nollet, J. H. Kok, J. Oosterlaan, A. G. van Wassenaer-Leemhuis, "Deficits in vision and visual attention associated with motor performance of very preterm/very low birth weight children," Research in Developmental Disabilities, vol. 53-54, pp. 258-266, DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.02.008, 2016.
[68] T. Jarus, Y. Lourie-Gelberg, B. Engel-Yeger, O. Bart, "Participation patterns of school-aged children with and without DCD," Research in Developmental Disabilities, vol. 32 no. 4, pp. 1323-1331, DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.01.033, 2011.
[69] R. Odejayi, D. Franzsen, P. De Witt, "Visual motor integration delay in preschool children infected with HIV," South African Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 49 no. 3, pp. 24-30, DOI: 10.17159/2310-3833/2019/vol49n3a5, 2019.
[70] F. Valente, C. Pesola, V. Baglioni, M. T. Giannini, F. Chiarotti, B. Caravale, F. Cardona, "Developmental motor profile in preschool children with primary stereotypic movement disorder," BioMed Research International, vol. 2019,DOI: 10.1155/2019/1427294, 2019.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright © 2021 Blair Carsone et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Abstract
Original research articles regarding visual motor integration skills in children with developmental disabilities and the impact of occupational therapy were identified, appraised, and synthesized. Twenty-four articles were chosen for this review. Themes were noted during the critique of articles. Three themes emerged: “age,” “gender,” and “diagnosis.” Regarding the impact on visual motor integration, there was strong evidence for age, moderate evidence for gender, and strong evidence for diagnosis. Future research investigating visual motor integration in children should control for age and diagnosis.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer